Skip to main content

A Study on the Morpho-syntactic Profiles of Syrian Children Learning Turkish as a Second Language

  • Conference paper
  • First Online:
Chaos, Complexity and Leadership 2016 (ICCLS 2016)

Part of the book series: Springer Proceedings in Complexity ((SPCOM))

Included in the following conference series:

  • 1371 Accesses

Abstract

Having a multifaceted complex structure, language is a combination of a pile of mental states or thoughts which are transferred by means of shared rules or principles created on the grounds of phonology, morphology, and semantics. Language, being such a humanly and complicated formation, is highly in touch with a group of pertinent scientific zones like psychology and sociology, and this interaction may mirror the morpho-syntactic features of people. That is to say, selecting and forming any word, structuring a full sentence, and seeing the meaning of the sentence necessitate intricate rules or phases. With respect to this phenomenon, intricate mental or cognitive processes might be challenged in second language learning which means picking up the syntactic rules of a language and converting these rules into language skills. In this study, ten Syrian children, being educated in a primary school and owning different psychological schemas, as well as being not at similar ages, were inspected. The children were required to talk about the picture book Smile Please by Sanjiv Jaiswal “Sanjay” in Turkish language, and the narrations were audiotaped by the researchers. Being formed on a descriptive research design, the data were gathered and analyzed qualitatively. As a consequence of the study that checked the general morpho-syntactic profiles of Syrian children, both different and shared morpho-syntactic characteristics were found out.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akbaşlı, S., & Üredi, L. (2015). An evaluation of the classroom teachers’ attitudes towards the constructivist approach according to complexity theory: A case of Mersin. In Ş. Ş. Erçetin (Ed.), Chaos, complexity and leadership 2013 (pp. 419–434). Cham: Springer International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Babayiğit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2010). Component processes of early reading, spelling, and narrative writing skills in Turkish: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing, 23(5), 539–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beedham, C. (2005). Language and meaning: The structural creation of reality (Vol. 55). John Benjamins Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berendson, E. (1986). The phonology of cliticization. Ph.D. dissertation, Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1991). Language processing in bilingual children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J.L., & Pagliuca, W. (1987). The evolution of future meaning. In Papers from the 7th international conference on historical linguistics (pp. 108–122). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F., Dell, G. S., Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). Structural priming as implicit learning: A comparison of models of sentence production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 217–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1986). Barriers (linguistic inquiry monograph 13). London: Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crystal, D. (1980). A first dictionary of linguistics and phonetics. Colorado: Westview Press Boulder.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giannakidou, A., & Mari, A. (2013). A two dimensional analysis of the future: modal adverbs and speaker’s bias. In Proceedings of the Amsterdam colloquium (Vol. 2013, pp. 115–122).

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldwater, S., & McClosky, D. (2005). Improving statistical MT through morphological analysis. In Proceedings of the conference on human language technology and empirical methods in natural language processing (pp. 676–683).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, P. (1982). From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. In The structure of phonological representations 1 (pp. 131–175).

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen-Freeman, D. (2009). Prediction or retrodiction?: The coming together of research and teaching. In Spotlight on re-search: A new beginning. The selected proceedings of the 2008 MITESOL Conference (pp. 5–16).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lázaro, A., & Garcia Mayo, M. D. P. (2012). L1 use and morphosyntactic development in the oral production of EFL learners in a CLIL context. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 50(2), 135–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, A. (2008). Statistical machine translation. ACM Computing Surveys (CSUR), 40(3), 8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lozano, C. (2006). The development of the syntax-discourse interface. In The acquisition of syntax in Romance languages 41 (p. 371).

    Google Scholar 

  • Montrul, S. A. (2004). The acquisition of Spanish: Morphosyntactic development in monolingual and bilingual L1 acquisition and adult L2 acquisition (Vol. 37). Holland: John Benjamins Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller Gathercole, V. C. (2007). Miami and North Wales, so far and yet so near: A constructivist account of morphosyntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(3), 224–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newmeyer, F. J. (2000). Language form and language function. Cambridge: Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paradis, J., & Crago, M. B. (2004). Dual language development & disorders: A handbook on bilingualism & second language learning (Vol. 11). Cambridge: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perdue, C., Benazzo, S., Giuliano, P. (2002). When finiteness gets marked: The relation between morphosyntactic development and use of scopal items in adult language acquisition (pp. 849–890). Baltimore: Linguistics, 40(4; ISSU 380).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(02), 169–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollock, J. Y. (1989). Verb movement, universal grammar, and the structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rooryck, J. (2003). The morphosyntactic structure of articles and pronouns in Dutch. Germania et alia. A linguistic webschrift for Hans den Besten, http://odur.let.rug.nl/~koster/DenBesten/contents.htm.

  • Salaberry, M. R. (2000). L2 morphosyntactic development in text-based computer-mediated communication. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 13(1), 5–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanjay, S. J. (2010). Smile please. India: Pratham Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, E. (1982). The syntax of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sengül, M. (2015). The opinions of instructors teaching Turkish to foreigners about the writing skills of Syrian students. Journal of Education and Training Studies, 3(5), 177–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shwayder, K. (2014). Morphosyntactic structure of phonological words. In Proceedings of the Annual Meetings on Phonology (1, 1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarone, E. (1988). Variation in interlanguage. London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, P. D. (2008). Teacher-and learner-led discourse in task-based grammar instruction: Providing procedural assistance for L2 morphosyntactic development. Language Learning, 58(2), 237–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ulum, & Kara. (2016). The effects of war on Syrian refugees’ academic achievement. The Journal of Academic Social Science Studies. Summer II, 48, 413–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Üredi, L. (2015). Evaluating the primary school teachers’ level of forming a constructivist learning environment according to chaos theory. In Chaos, complexity and leadership 2013 (pp. 537–566). Cham: Springer International Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, T. L. (2005). Basic classical ethnographic research methods. Cultural ecology of health change. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zec, D. (1993). Rule domains and phonological change. In S. Hargus & E. Kaisse (Eds.), Studies in lexical phonology (pp. 365–405). San Diego: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Zwicky, A. M. (1985). Heads. Journal of Linguistics, 21(01), 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lütfi Üredi .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this paper

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this paper

Üredi, L., Ulum, Ö.G. (2018). A Study on the Morpho-syntactic Profiles of Syrian Children Learning Turkish as a Second Language. In: Erçetin, Ş. (eds) Chaos, Complexity and Leadership 2016. ICCLS 2016. Springer Proceedings in Complexity. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-64554-4_34

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics