Skip to main content

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Animal Sciences ((BRIEFSANIMAL))

Abstract

Besides moths, the only other bat–prey interaction, with the potential for co-evolution, is that with frogs. Bats can eavesdrop on sounds inadvertently produced by frogs as they move through their habitat or they can eavesdrop on the mating calls emitted by male frogs to attract females. Bats using such passive acoustic detection have an additional field of increased sensitivity outside the range of their own echolocation calls which matches frequencies at which most frogs call. Most frogs use audition for intra-specific communication , but as yet there is no evidence that they can hear echolocation. Some frog species are able to detect bats visually, allowing them to deploy a defence strategy before they are detected—the frog may cease movement or stop calling, depriving the bat of an acoustic cue . Frog mating calls are therefore under at least two strong selection pressures—female mate choice and bat predation. This may pose an evolutionary trade-off , potentially indicative of co-evolution, because call cessation has a negative effect on reproduction opportunities. However, although frogs appear to have evolved anti-bat behaviour, there is no indication that bats have responded. For example, some frogs have evolved calls that are difficult to localize for bats despite these calls being less attractive to females. The fringe-lipped bat may have responded by using water ripples produced by the calling frog and which remain available for a short time even after the frog stops calling. However, besides the latter case, no evidence has yet emerged that indicates an iterative reciprocal process between bats and frogs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Akre KL, Ryan MJ (2010) Complexity increases working memory for mating signals. Curr Biol 20(6):502–505

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akre KL, Farris HE, Lea AM, Page RA, Ryan MJ (2011) Signal perception in frogs and bats and the evolution of mating signals. Science 333(6043):751–752

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arch VS, Grafe TU, Gridi-Papp M, Narins PM (2009) Pure ultrasonic communication in an endemic Bornean frog. PLoS ONE 4(4):e5413

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Balcombe J, Fenton MB (1988) Eavesdropping by bats: the influence of echolocation call design and foraging strategy. Ethology 79(2):158–166

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay RMR (1982) Interindividual use of echolocation calls: eavesdropping by bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10(4):271–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay RMR, Fenton M, Tuttle M, Ryan M (1981) Echolocation calls produced by Trachops cirrhosus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomatidae) while hunting for frogs. Can J Zool 59(5):750–753

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barth FG, Schmid A (2001) Ecology of sensing (1 ed). Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastian A, Jacobs DS (2015) Listening carefully: increased perceptual acuity for species discrimination in multispecies signalling assemblages. Anim Behav 101:141–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baugh AT, Ryan MJ (2010) Mate choice in response to dynamic presentation of male advertisement signals in tungara frogs. Anim Behav 79(1):145–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benedix J Jr, Narins PM (1999) Competitive calling behavior by male treefrogs, Eleutherodactylus coqui (Anura: Leptodactylidae). Copeia 1118–1122

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernal XE, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (2006) Acoustic preferences and localization performance of blood-sucking flies (Corethrella Coquillett) to tungara frog calls. Behav Ecol 17(5):709–715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal XE, Stanley Rand A, Ryan MJ (2007a) Sexual differences in the behavioral response of tungara frogs, Physalaemus pustulosus, to cues associated with increased predation risk. Ethology 113(8):755–763

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal XE, Page RA, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (2007b) Cues for eavesdroppers: do frog calls indicate prey density and quality? Am Nat 169(3):409–415

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal XE, Akre KL, Baugh AT, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (2009) Female and male behavioral response to advertisement calls of graded complexity in tungara frogs, Physalaemus pustulosus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63(9):1269–1279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonato V, Facure K (2000) Bat predation by the fringe-lipped bat Trachops cirrhosus (Phyllostomidae, Chiroptera). Mammalia-Paris 64(2):241–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruns V, Burda H, Ryan MJ (1989) Ear morphology of the frog-eating bat (Trachops cirrhosus, family: Phyllostomidae): apparent specializations for low-frequency hearing. J Morphol 199(1):103–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brush JS, Narins PM (1989) Chorus dynamics of a Neotropical amphibian assemblage: comparison of computer simulation and natural behaviour. Anim Behav 37:33–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchler E, Childs S (1981) Orientation to distant sounds by foraging big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). Anim Behav 29(2):428–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bulbert MW, Page RA, Bernal XE (2015) Danger comes from all fronts: Predator-dependent escape tactics of tungara frogs. PLoS ONE 10(4):12

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Caldart VM, Iop S, Cechin SZ (2014) Social interactions in a neotropical stream frog reveal a complex repertoire of visual signals and the use of multimodal communication. Behaviour 151(6):719–739

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capranica RR, Moffat AJ (1983) Neurobehavioral correlates of sound communication in anurans. In: Moffat AJ, Capranica RR, Ingle DJ (eds) Advances in vertebrate neuroethology, vol 56. Springer, New York, pp 701–730

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings ME, Rosenthal GG, Ryan MJ (2003) A private ultraviolet channel in visual communication. Proc Royal Soc London B: Biol Sci 270(1518):897–904

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dapper AL, Baugh AT, Ryan MJ (2011) The sounds of silence as an alarm cue in tungara frogs, Physalaemus pustulosus. Biotropica 43(3):380–385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies NB, Krebs JR, West SA (2012) An introduction to behavioural ecology. Wiley, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Delia J, Cisneros-Heredia DF, Whitney J, Murrieta-Galindo R (2010) Observations on the reproductive behavior of a neotropical Glassfrog, Hyalinobatrachium fleischmanni (Anura: Centrolenidae). S Am J Herpetol 5(1):1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denzinger A, Schnitzler H-U (2013) Bat guilds, a concept to classify the highly diverse foraging and echolocation behaviors of microchiropteran bats. Front Physiol 4:1–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Endler JA (1992) Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am Nat 139:S125–S153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng AS, Narins PM, Xu CH, Lin WY, Yu ZL, Qiu Q, Xu ZM, Shen JX (2006) Ultrasonic communication in frogs. Nature 440(7082):333–336

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenton MB (1990) The foraging behavior and ecology of animal-eating bats. Can J Zool-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 68(3):411–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenton MB, Gaudet CL, Leonard ML (1983) Feeding-behavior of the bats Nycteris grandis and Nycteris thebaica (Nycteridae) in captivity. J Zool 200:347–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenton MB, Cumming D, Hutton J, Swanepoel C (1987) Foraging and habitat use by Nycteris grandis (Chiroptera: Nycteridae) in Zimbabwe. J Zool 211(4):709–716

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenton MB, Rautenbach IL, Chipese D, Cumming MB, Musgrave MK, Taylor JS, Volpers T (1993) Variation in foraging behavior, habitat use, and diet of Large slit-faced bats (Nycteris grandis). Zeitschrift für Säugetierkunde Biol 58(2):65–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher RA (1930) The genetical theory of natural selection: a complete, variorum edn. Oxford University Press. Edition, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Forrest T (1994) From sender to receiver: propagation and environmental effects on acoustic signals. Am Zool 34(6):644–654

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fugere V, Teague O’Mara M, Page RA (2015) Perceptual bias does not explain preference for prey call adornment in the frog-eating bat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69(8):1353–1364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuzessery ZM, Buttenhoff P, Andrews B, Kennedy JM (1993) Passive sound localization of prey by the pallid bat (Antrozous p. pallidus). J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 171(6):767–777

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC (1994a) Reproductive character displacement of female mate choice in the grey treefrog, Hyla chrysoscelis. Anim Behav 47(4):959–969

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt CH (1994b) The evolution of vocalization in frogs and toads. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 25:293–324

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC, Huber F (2002) Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: common problems and diverse solutions. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC, Schwartz JJ (2001) Auditory tuning and frequency preferences in anurans. Anuran Commun 73–85

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillam E (2007) Eavesdropping by bats on the feeding buzzes of conspecifics. Can J Zool 85(7):795–801

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green AJ (1990) Determinants of chorus participation and the effects of size, weight and competition on advertisement calling in the tungara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus (Leptodactylidae). Anim Behav 39(4):620–638

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gridi-Papp M, Feng AS, Shen J-X, Yu Z-L, Rosowski JJ, Narins PM (2008) Active control of ultrasonic hearing in frogs. Proc Natl Acad Sci 105(31):11014–11019

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Halfwerk W, Jones PL, Taylor RC, Ryan MJ, Page RA (2014a) Risky ripples allow bats and frogs to eavesdrop on a multisensory sexual display. Science 343(6169):413–416

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halfwerk W, Dixon MM, Ottens KJ, Taylor RC, Ryan MJ, Page RA, Jones PL (2014b) Risks of multimodal signaling: bat predators attend to dynamic motion in frog sexual displays. J Exp Biol 217(17):3038–3044

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Höbel G (1999) Notes on the natural history and habitat use of Eleutherodactylus fitzingeri (Anura: Leptodactylidae). Amphibia-Reptilia 20(1):65–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Igaune K, Krams I, Krama T, Bobkova J (2008) White storks Ciconia ciconia eavesdrop on mating calls of moor frogs Rana arvalis. J Avian Biol 39(2):229–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger RG (1976) A possible prey-call window in anuran auditory perception. Copeia 1976(4):833–834

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone RA (1998) Conspiratorial whispers and conspicuous displays: games of signal detection. Evolution 1554–1563

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones G, Webb PI, Sedgeley JA, O’Donnell CF (2003) Mysterious Mystacina: how the New Zealand short-tailed bat (Mystacina tuberculata) locates insect prey. J Exp Biol 206(Pt 23):4209–4216

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones PL, Ryan MJ, Flores V, Page RA (2013a) When to approach novel prey cues? Social learning strategies in frog-eating bats. Proc Royal Soc B—Biol Sci 280(1772):2013–2330

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones PL, Farris HE, Ryan MJ, Page RA (2013b) Do frog-eating bats perceptually bind the complex components of frog calls? J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 199(4):279–283

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones PL, Ryan MJ, Page RA (2014) Population and seasonal variation in response to prey calls by an eavesdropping bat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 68(4):605–615

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koselj K, Siemers BM (2013) Horseshoe bats can use information in echoes of conspecific calls for spatial orientation. Paper presented at the International Bat Research Conference, Costa Rica

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahanas PN (1995) The function of near neighbors in decreasing call latency period by the tungara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus. Biotropica 27(2):262–265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang AB, Kalko EK, Römer H, Bockholdt C, Dechmann DK (2006) Activity levels of bats and katydids in relation to the lunar cycle. Oecologia 146(4):659–666

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68(4):619–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, O’Keefe JM (2013) Do predators influence the behaviour of bats? Biol Rev 88(3):626–644

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Llusia D, Márquez R, Beltrán JF (2010) Non-selective and time-dependent behavioural responses of common toads (Bufo bufo) to predator acoustic cues. Ethology 116(12):1146–1154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus-Hills JJ, Johnstone BM (1970) Auditory function, communication, and the brain-evoked response in anuran amphibians. J Acoust Soc Am 47(4B):1131–1138

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marimuthu G (1997) Stationary prey insures life and moving prey ensures death during the hunting flight of gleaning bats. Curr Sci 72(12):928–931

    Google Scholar 

  • Marimuthu G, Neuweiler G (1987) The use of acoustical cues for prey detection by the Indian false vampire bat, Megaderma lyra. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 160(4):509–515

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marimuthu G, Habersetzer J, Leippert D (1995) Active acoustic gleaning from the water-surface by the Indian false vampire bat, Megaderma lyra. Ethology 99(1):61–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Marimuthu G, Rajan KE, Kandula S, Parsons S, Jones G (2002) Effects of different surfaces on the perception of prey-generated noise by the Indian false vampire bat Megaderma lyra. Acta Chiropterologica 4(1):25–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narins PM, Feng AS, Lin WY, Schnitzler H-U, Denzinger A, Suthers RA, Xu CH (2004) Old World frog and bird, vocalizations contain prominent ultrasonic harmonics. J Acoust Soc Am 115(2):910–913

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuweiler G (1980) How bats detect flying insects. Phys Today 33(8):34–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuweiler G (1989) Foraging ecology and audition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evol 4(6):160–166

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuweiler G (1990) Auditory adaptations for prey capture in echolocating bats. Physiol Rev 70(3):615–641

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuweiler G, Singh S, Sripathi K (1984) Audiograms of a South Indian bat community. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 154(1):133–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norberg UM, Fenton MB (1988) Carnivorous bats? Biol J Linn Soc 33(4):383–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes VD (1988) Vocalizations of treefrogs (Smilisca sila) in response to bat predation. Herpetologica 44(1):8–10

    Google Scholar 

  • Page RA, Ryan MJ (2005) Flexibility in assessment of prey cues: frog-eating bats and frog calls. Proc Royal Soc Lond B: Biol Sci 272(1565):841–847

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Page RA, Ryan MJ (2006) Social transmission of novel foraging behavior in bats: frog calls and their referents. Curr Biol 16(12):1201–1205

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Page RA, Ryan MJ (2008) The effect of signal complexity on localization performance in bats that localize frog calls. Anim Behav 76:761–769

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Page RA, Schnelle T, Kalko EKV, Bunge T, Bernal XE (2012) Sequential assessment of prey through the use of multiple sensory cues by an eavesdropping bat. Naturwissenschaften 99(6):505–509

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Page RA, Ryan MJ, Bernal XE (2013) Be loved, be prey, be eaten. In K Yasukawa (ed) Animal behavior (vol 3, Case studies: integration and application of animal behavior pp 123–54). Praeger, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips CJ, Tandler B, Pinkstaff CA (1987) Unique salivary glands in two genera of tropical microchiropteran bats: an example of evolutionary convergence in histology and histochemistry. J Mammal 68(2):235–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popper AN, Fay RR (1995) Hearing by bats, vol 5. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Popper AN, Fay RR (2005) Sound source localization. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Poussin C, Simmons JA (1982) Low-frequency hearing sensitivity in the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Acoust Soc Am 72(2):340–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prado CPA, Haddad CF (2003) Testes size in leptodactylid frogs and occurrence of multimale spawning in the genus Leptodactylus in Brazil. J Herpetol 37(2):354–362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puechmaille SJ, Borissov IM, Zsebok S, Allegrini B, Hizem M, Kuenzel S, Schuchmann M, Teeling EC, Siemers BM (2014) Female mate choice can drive the evolution of high frequency echolocation in bats: a case study with Rhinolophus mehelyi. PLoS ONE 9(7):e103452

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe JM, Raghuram H, Marimuthu G, Fullard JH, Fenton MB (2005) Hunting in unfamiliar space: echolocation in the Indian false vampire bat, Megaderma lyra, when gleaning prey. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 58:157–164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhebergen F, Taylor RC, Ryan MJ, Page RA, Halfwerk W (2015) Multimodal cues improve prey localization under complex environmental conditions. Proc Royal Soc B: Biol Sci 282(1814)

    Google Scholar 

  • Römer H, Lang A, Hartbauer M (2010) The signaller’s dilemma: a cost–benefit analysis of public and private communication. PLoS ONE 5(10):e13325

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ron SR (2008) The evolution of female mate choice for complex calls in tungara frogs. Anim Behav 76(6):1783–1794

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rübsamen R, Neuweiler G, Sripathi K (1988) Comparative collicular tonotopy in two bat species adapted to movement detection, Hipposideros speoris and Megaderma lyra. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 163(2):271–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruczyński I, Kalko EKV, Siemers BM (2009) Calls in the forest: a comparative approach to how bats find tree cavities. Ethology 115(2):167–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (1983) Sexual selection and communication in a Neotropical frog, Physalaemus pustulosus. Evolution 37(2):261–272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (1986) Factors influencing the evolution of acoustic communication—biological constraints. Brain Behav Evol 28(1–3):70–82

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (1990) Sexual selection, sensory systems and sensory exploitation. Oxf Surv Evol Biol 7:157–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (2005) The evolution of behaviour, and integrating it towards a complete and correct understanding of behavioural biology. Anim Biol 55(4):419–439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (2011) Sexual selection: a tutorial from the tungara frog. In: Losos JB (ed) In the light of evolution: essays from the laboratory and field. Roberts and Company, Greenwood Village CO, pp 185–203

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Rand AS (2003) Mate recognition in tungara frogs: a review of some studies of brain, behavior, and evolution. Acta Zoologica Sinica 49(6):713–726

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD (1983) The ability of the frog-eating bat to discriminate among novel and potentially poisonous frog species using acoustic cues. Anim Behav 31:827–833

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD (1987) The role of prey-generated sounds, vision, and echolocation in prey localization by the African bat Cardioderma cor (Megadermatidae). J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 161(1):59–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Taft LK (1981) The costs and benefits of frog chorusing behavior. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8(4):273–278

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Rand AS (1982) Bat predation and sexual advertisement in a Neotropical anuran. Am Nat 119(1):136–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Barclay RMR (1983) Behavioral responses of the frog-eating bat, Trachops cirrhosus, to sonic frequencies. J Comp Physiol 150(4):413–418

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sales G (2012) Ultrasonic communication by animals. Chapman & Hall, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt S, Hanke S, Pillat J (2000) The role of echolocation in the hunting of terrestrial prey—new evidence for an underestimated strategy in the gleaning bat, Megaderma lyra. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 186(10):975–988

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt S, Yapa W, Grunwald J-E (2010) Echolocation behaviour of Megaderma lyra during typical orientation situations and while hunting aerial prey: a field study. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 97(5):403–412

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnitzler H-U, Kalko EKV (2001) Echolocation by insect-eating bats. Bioscience 51(7):557–569

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnitzler H-U, Moss CF, Denzinger A (2003) From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evol 18(8):386–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuchmann M, Siemers BM (2010) Behavioral evidence for community wide species discrimination from echolocation calls in bats. Am Nat 176:72–82

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuchmann M, Puechmaille SJ, Siemers BM (2012) Horseshoe bats recognise the sex of conspecifics from their echolocation calls. Acta Chiropterol 14(1):161–166

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ, Bee MA, Tanner SD (2000) A behavioral and neurobiological study of the responses of gray treefrogs, Hyla versicolor to the calls of a predator, Rana catesbeiana. Herpetologica 27–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Seamark EC, Bogdanowicz W (2002) Feeding ecology of the common slit-faced bat (Nycteris thebaica) in KwaZulu-Natal. S Afr Acta Chiropterol 4(1):49–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seeba F, Schwartz JJ, Bee MA (2010) Testing an auditory illusion in frogs: perceptual restoration or sensory bias? Anim Behav 79(6):1317–1328

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shen JX, Xu ZM, Feng AS, Narins PM (2011) Large odorous frogs (Odorrana graminea) produce ultrasonic calls. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 197(10):1027–1030

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shetty S, Sreepada KS (2013) Prey and nutritional analysis of Megaderma lyra guano from the west coast of Karnataka, India. Adv Biores 4(3):1–7

    Google Scholar 

  • Siemers BM, Stilz P, Schnitzler H-U (2001) The acoustic advantage of hunting at low heights above water: behavioural experiments on the European ‘trawling’ bats Myotis capaccinii, M. dasycneme and M. daubentonii. J Exp Biol 204:3843–3854

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siemers BM, Kriner E, Kaipf I, Simon M, Greif S (2012) Bats eavesdrop on the sound of copulating flies. Curr Biol 22(14):R563–R564

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JA, Stein RA (1980) Acoustic imaging in bat sonar: echolocation signals and the evolution of echolocation. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 135(1):61–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speakman J (1993) The evolution of echolocation for predation. Paper presented at the Symposia of the Zoological Society of London

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoddard PK (1999) Predation enhances complexity in the evolution of electric fish signals. Nature 400(6741):254–256

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surlykke A, Jakobsen L, Kalko EK, Page RA (2013) Echolocation intensity and directionality of perching and flying fringe-lipped bats, Trachops cirrhosus (Phyllostomidae). Front Physiol 4

    Google Scholar 

  • Suthers RA (1965) Acoustic orientation by fish-catching bats. J Exp Zool 158(3):319–347

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tandler B, Phillips CJ, Nagato T (1996) Histological convergent evolution of the accessory submandibular glands in four species of frog-eating bats. Eur J Morphol 34(3):163–168

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tandler B, Nagato T, Phillips CJ (1997) Ultrastructure of the unusual accessory submandibular gland in the fringe-lipped bat, Trachops cirrhosus. Anatomical Record 248(2):164–175

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas AJ, Jacobs DS (2013) Factors influencing the emergence times of sympatric insectivorous bat species. Acta Chiropterol 15(1):121–132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trillo PA, Athanas KA, Goldhill DH, Hoke KL, Funk WC (2013) The influence of geographic heterogeneity in predation pressure on sexual signal divergence in an Amazonian frog species complex. J Evol Biol 26(1):216–222

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tuttle MD, Ryan MJ (1981) Bat predation and the evolution of frog vocalizations in the Neotropics. Science 214(4521):677–678

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tuttle MD, Ryan MJ (1982) The role of synchronized calling, ambient light, and ambient noise, in anti-bat-predator behavior of a treefrog. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 11(2):125–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuttle MD, Taft LK, Ryan MJ (1981) Acoustical location of calling frogs by Philander opossums. Biotropica 13(3):233–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuttle MD, Taft LK, Ryan MJ (1982) Evasive behavior of a frog in response to bat predation. Anim Behav 30(MAY):393–397

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Usman KA, Habersetzer J, Subbaraj R, Gopalkrishnaswamy G, Paramanandam K (1980) Behaviour of bats during a lunar eclipse. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7(1):79–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan TA (1976) Nocturnal behavior of the African false vampire bat (Cardioderma cor). J Mammal 57(2):227–248

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (2007) The ecology and behavior of amphibians (1st ed). The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD, Schwartz JJ (1982) The effect of vegetation on the propagation of calls in the Neotropical frog Centrolenella fleischmanni. Herpetologica 38:449–455

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley RH, Richards DG (1978) Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 3(1):69–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuk M, Kolluru GR (1998) Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. Q Rev Biol 73(4):415–438

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Steve Jacobs .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Jacobs, D.S., Bastian, A. (2016). Co-evolution Between Bats and Frogs?. In: Predator–Prey Interactions: Co-evolution between Bats and Their Prey. SpringerBriefs in Animal Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-32492-0_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics