Keywords

1 Introduction

Our auditory world is by no means stationary. Natural sound sources often change position and also listeners move in their environment. Predators like the barn owl (Tyto alba) will routinely hunt moving prey, and they perform in-flight corrections (Hausmann et al. 2008). Thus, with respect to sound source localization, real world situations confront the auditory system with varying binaural cues. Possible motion-related dynamic binaural cues are the change in interaural time differences (ITDs), in interaural level differences (ILDs) or in spectral peaks and notches. Generally, ITD cues identify sound source azimuth, ILD cues azimuth and/or elevation and peaks and notches mostly elevation (e.g., Grothe et al. 2010). It has been suggested that auditory motion processing relies on specialized circuits composed of directionally tuned and motion sensitive neurons in the midbrain (reviewed by Wagner et al. 1997). Time constants for processing of non-stationary dynamic cues must be short to allow tracking of moving sound sources (Wagner 1991). However, time constants for binaural processing have found to depend on measuring procedures (e.g., Shackleton and Palmer 2010).

Auditory motion perception has been investigated simulating motion by switching between neighbouring free-field loudspeakers (e.g., Wagner and Takahashi 1992) or by varying interaural cues in headphone presentation (reviewed by Middlebrooks and Green 1991). Neurophysiological correlates to human auditory motion perception have been investigated in animal models with both free-field and headphone stimulation (e.g., McAlpine et al. 2000; Wagner et al. 1997). What is missing, however, is the direct comparison of neuronal and perceptual sensitivity for acoustic motion in the same model species.

Here we report auditory motion perception in the barn owl, a species for which data on the neuronal representation of motion stimuli in the inferior colliculus (IC) are available (e.g., Wagner and Takahashi 1992; Kautz and Wagner 1998; Wagner and von Campenhausen 2002; Wang and Peña 2013). Specifically, Wagner and Takahashi (1992) showed that the angular velocity and stimulus duration affected the neurons’ response. Here, we investigate how stimulus velocity, the size of the angle of sound incidence (i.e., the angular range), and stimulus duration affect the owls’ perceptual performance. Furthermore, we analyse whether the perception of changes in motion direction differs for stimuli presented in the frontal or in the lateral space. Finally, we will discuss the behavioural data with reference to the responses of motion direction sensitive neurons in the barn owl auditory midbrain.

2 Methods

We trained two barn owls to report a change in auditory motion within a series of repeating band-pass noise stimuli (500–6000 Hz) presented in the horizontal plane. Motion was simulated by sequentially activating up to eight loudspeakers (Vifa XT25TG30-04), fading in and out the sound between adjacent loudspeakers (spacing 12°; Fig. 1). The experiments were performed in a sound-attenuating echo-reduced chamber (IAC 1203-A, walls with sound absorbing foam). Signals were generated by a 24-bit sound card (Hammerfall DSP Multiface II, RME, Germany) and the loudspeakers were driven by a multichannel amplifier (RMB-1048, Rotel, Japan). The barn owls had learned to sit on a waiting perch during the repeated reference stimuli and to fly to a report perch when a test stimulus was presented (Go/NoGo paradigm). Within a trial the reference stimuli (repetition period 1300 ms) had the same direction, while the test was a single stimulus presented with opposite motion direction. Each stimulus started at a random position between ‑42° and + 42° in azimuth. This prevented the owls from using location per se rather than motion direction to solve the task. The owls were also trained to generally orient their head in the waiting position towards 0° in azimuth. Correct responses were rewarded. The waiting intervals before a test stimulus were randomized. For any trial the direction of the noise sweeps, the velocity (53–2400°/s) and the effective duration (30–225 ms, with 10 ms Hanning ramps) and angular range of the motion (12–72°) of the reference stimuli were randomly selected from a distribution of combinations of parameters. Each combination of parameters was presented 20 times. Experimental sessions with 44 trials each (36 test trials and 8 catch trials having no change in motion direction) were included in the analysis if the owls responded at least ten times (10 “Hits”).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Stimuli were classified as moving either “inward” (from lateral to frontal) or “outward” (from frontal to lateral). “Frontal” stimuli were moving at least 50 % of the time in the range between + and ‑15° from the midline. “Lateral” stimuli were moving more than 50 % of the time in one of the two hemispheres (including up to 15° of the other hemisphere)

We performed two experimental series. Series 1 had effective stimulus durations of 75, 150 and 225 ms and provided stimulus velocities of between 53 and 960°/s. Series 2 had effective stimulus durations of 30, 60 and 90 ms and provided stimulus velocities of between 133 and 2400°/s. Note that stimulus duration and stimulus velocity are interdependent variables. The relation between sensitivity and the parameters stimulus duration, angular range, and stimulus velocity was analysed by linear regression. We will use the regressions’ coefficient of determination (R2) and associated ANOVA results to describe the contribution of each of the three parameters to the perception of a change in motion direction.

We made the following assumptions for data analysis. (1) Noise sweeps moving from the owl’s lateral space toward the frontal space were classified as moving “inward”, noise sweeps moving from the frontal space toward the lateral space were classified as moving “outward”. (2) “Lateral” stimuli were noise sweeps that stayed in one hemisphere more the 50 % of the time, but were allowed to cross the midline up to 15° to the contralateral side, “frontal” stimuli were noise sweeps moving more the 50 % of the time in the range between plus and minus 15° from the midline (Fig. 1). The reasoning for this approach is that the owls’ space maps in the left and right IC overlap by about this amount (Knudsen and Konishi 1978a, b). For the lateral/frontal classification, we limited our analysis to noise sweeps of an extent of 36° and 48° to avoid floor and ceiling effects. We then compared the owl’s performance for inward and outward stimuli and for stimuli between the frontal and lateral space using Χ2 tests.

3 Results

In a behavioural task, barn owls show high sensitivity for auditory motion discrimination. Sensitivity was reduced only for small angular ranges. Furthermore auditory motion discrimination was still good at the shortest stimulus duration (30 ms) and at high velocities (up to 2400°/s).

3.1 Effects of Stimulus Velocity, Size of the Angular Range, and Stimulus Duration on Auditory Motion Discrimination

Our data show that the owls’ sensitivity for auditory motion discrimination mainly increased with increasing angular range. The amount of variance accounted for (R2) by “angular range” was between 0.70 and 0.80 for each of the conditions shown in Figs. 2 and 3. Stimulus duration and stimulus velocity had only a small, though significant effect on sensitivity. When comparing sensitivity at similar angular ranges, sensitivity moderately increased for longer stimuli and decreased for faster moving stimuli (R2 values for “duration” were between 0.03 and 0.13, R2 values for “velocity” were between 0.07 and 0.21, all P ≤ 0.001). The same pattern was represented by the ANOVA results: the factor “angular range” had F-values of at least 854, F-values for “duration” were between 12 and 55, F-values for “velocity” were between 25 and 95 (all P ≤ 0.001). The owls’ sensitivity was still rather high, if stimulus duration was further decreased in series 2 (Fig. 3). When comparing sensitivity for noise sweeps of similar velocities at different durations (Fig. 2, 3), we had expected to find them to be similar across conditions. In contrast to our expectation, stimulus velocity was a rather bad predictor of the owls’ behavioural performance. This can be explained, however, by the interaction of duration and angular range that determines stimulus velocity.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Series 1 with stimulus velocities up to 960°/s, results from two owls (A, B): Sensitivity increases with larger angular range and longer stimulus duration. Sensitivity index (d’) is plotted as a function of stimulus velocity, angular range and duration

Fig. 3
figure 3

Series 2 with stimulus velocities up to 2400°/s, results from two owls (A, B): Sensitivity is hardly affected when stimulus velocity is further increased. Sensitivity index (d’) is plotted as a function of stimulus velocity, angular range and duration

3.2 Effects of Stimulus Position on Auditory Motion Discrimination

Responses to inward and outward sweeps were equally frequent: the owls had relative hit rates of between about 35 and 43 % for each of the two directions (Χ2 tests n.s., Table 1). The owls responded more often to stimuli with a change in direction moving in the frontal range compared to those moving in the lateral range (Table 2): Owl “ba” had a relative Hit rate of about 67 % in the frontal space and of only 38 % in the lateral space (Χ2 = 15.7, P < 0.01) while owl “we” had relative hit rates of about 52 and 39 % for the frontal and lateral range, respectively (Χ2 = 3.1, P = 0.077).

Table 1 The absolute “Hit” and “Miss” rates combining Series 1 and 2. Test stimuli were moving either inward or outward.
Table 1 Table 2 The absolute "Hit" and "Miss" rates combining Series 1 and 2. Noise sweeps were presented from either lateral or frontal

4 Discussion

Using simulated acoustic motion stimuli, we have demonstrated how the sensitivity for discriminating auditory motion direction in the horizontal plane is related to stimulus velocity, stimulus duration, and the size of the angular range covered by the moving sound. Furthermore, we describe how motion discrimination is affected by the location in the owl’s auditory space. Here we relate these data to results from studies of motion-direction sensitive neurons in the barn owl.

Motion direction sensitive neurons have been observed in the barn owl IC and optic tectum (Wagner et al. 1997, Wagner and von Campenhausen 2002). Neurophysiological studies on directional sensitivity in auditory motion processing by the owl have applied a wide range of angular velocities. Witten et al. (2006) presented simulated motion based on ITD change at a speed of 32°/s observing shifts of auditory spatial receptive fields in relation to motion direction that could be explained by adaptation. Similarly, Wang and Peña (2013) showed that adaptation affected direction selectivity for simulated motion of 470°/s in free-field stimulation. Wagner and Takahashi (1992) observed a broad tuning of motion direction selective units with respect to stimulus velocity when presenting simulated motion with an angular velocity ranging between 125 and 1200°/s. They suggest that inhibition accounts for the directional sensitivity. Our behavioural data obtained with noise sweeps in the free field and with an angular velocity between 53 and 2400°/s are in accord with the observed broad angular velocity tuning. Stimulus velocity had a moderate effect on motion direction sensitivity provided that the stimulus duration was not too short.

Duration of the noise sweeps per se had only a moderate effect on the behavioural sensitivity. Sensitivity was lower for short than for long stimulus durations. We could observe the lowest sensitivity for stimuli of 30 ms duration. This result is in accord with data describing the barn owl’s binaural temporal window for ITD processing ranging from 4 to 42 ms (Wagner 1991). Inhibitory neuronal processing has been observed to be effective already between 20 and 40 ms after stimulus onset (Wagner 1990). Inhibition could thus contribute to the dynamic sharpening of the ITD representation in the barn owl’s IC and to the duration of the binaural temporal window.

The angular range covered by the noise sweeps in our experiments exerted the strongest effect on the owl’s sensitivity. To solve the task, the angular range had to be at least 24°. The angular range necessary for motion direction discrimination was much larger than the mean error for locating points in azimuth as indicated by the head orienting response towards the sound source elicited by broadband noise bursts (Knudsen et al. 1979) or by the owl’s minimum audible angle (Bala et al. 2007). Using a pupillary dilation response, Bala et al. (2007) measured a minimum audible angle of 3° in azimuth. In the horizontal plain, Knudsen et al. (1979) observed a localization error being as low as 2° for broadband sounds in the frontal direction (open-loop conditions). The azimuth localization error increased to 6° if the target location was 50° lateral. The small localization error in comparison to the minimum angular range needed for motion direction discrimination suggests that at least partially different neuronal circuits are involved in the location of stationary and of moving sound sources. Wagner and Takahashi (1992) suggested a motion detector circuit in the barn owl auditory midbrain that included inhibition and a delayed interaction between neurons representing different locations in space (Wagner et al. 1997; Kautz and Wagner 1998). This circuit is interwoven with the computational auditory space map in the barn owl midbrain and relies on the space map providing input (Wagner et al. 1997). Thus, the barn owl brain uses separate but intermingled systems for representing auditory motion and spatial location. Furthermore, the large difference between the localization accuracy and the minimum angular range needed for motion direction discrimination renders it unlikely that the owls solve the motion direction discrimination task by a “snapshot” mechanism. The snapshot hypothesis suggests that the processing of motion simply relies on comparing starting and endpoint of the moving stimulus (e.g., Perrot and Marlborough 1989). The snapshot hypothesis was also dismissed in human psychophysical studies determining the minimum audible movement angle (reviewed by Middlebrooks and Green 1991). The difference between the barn owl’s sensitivity for comparing angular locations of static sounds (Bala et al. 2007) and the large angular range needed for direction discrimination (present study) suggests that the snapshot hypothesis does not apply to barn owls.

Neurophysiological recordings in the barn owl midbrain have revealed an interaction between the starting point of the motion and the preferred direction of motion direction sensitive units (Wagner and von Campenhausen 2002; Wang and Peña 2013). However, the results from the two studies were contradictory. Wagner and von Campenhausen (2002) observed that neurons responded preferentially to outward motion whereas Wang and Peña (2013) observed a preference to inward motion. The behavioural data obtained in the present study are not suited to resolve the discrepancy. Our owls were equally sensitive in detecting inward and outward moving noise sweeps.

Previous barn owl studies indicated that the frontal space is represented by a larger population of neurons than the lateral space (e.g., as reflected by the distribution of best ITDs in the IC; Wagner et al. 2007). Furthermore, the space maps in each IC represent the contralateral space plus an angular range of about 15° from the midline in the ipsilateral space (Knudsen and Konishi 1978a, 1978b). This results in an overlap in the representation of the frontal space (‑15° to 15°) by IC neurons from both sides of the brain. Finally, neurons in the IC tuned to locations in the frontal space have more confined spatial receptive fields (Knudsen and Konishi 1978b). The neurophysiological results have prompted Knudsen and Konishi (1978b) to predict that localization accuracy should be larger in the frontal space than in the lateral space. As expected, Knudsen et al. (1979) observed a smaller localization error in the frontal field than in the lateral field when measuring the barn owls’ head orienting response towards a stationary sound source. Our results are consistent with these findings. The barn owls’ motion direction discrimination was more sensitive for stimuli being presented in the frontal space compared to stimuli presented in the lateral space.