Skip to main content

Neurobiological Perspectives on Agency: Ten Axioms and Ten Propositions

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Constraints of Agency

Part of the book series: Annals of Theoretical Psychology ((AOTP,volume 12))

  • 1290 Accesses

Abstract

Herein, I define agency broadly, ranging from primitive animal activity to consciously driven human activity. I take the broad view because much of human conscious agency is driven by primitive neural processes. At the simplest level, actions are intrinsic, automated, and unconscious. At slightly higher levels, such actions are responses to stimuli. At still higher levels, actions arise from such agency elements as inherent drives, perception, memory recall, expected utility, intent to act, decision, plans, and finally action. These elements of agency may be unconscious, conscious, or some mixture of both.

This chapter lists and explains ten “axioms of agency” that I believe would be widely accepted by many if not most neuroscientists. Axioms, however identified, need to be the starting point for understanding what new research is needed. Then, as perhaps a basis for new research, I identify ten “propositions of agency,” which are ideas open to debate. Two of those propositions, which I argue against, are that consciousness does not do anything and that there is no free will.

I explain the view that agency derives from the circuit connections of neurons, which are most fundamentally hardwired in simple brains with relatively few neurons. But in higher animals, the superabundance of networks within networks allows for dynamic adjustment of circuitry to enable adaptive adjustment of the elements of agency. In all circuits, the spatiotemporal patterns of nerve impulses are the carriers of information. Therefore, if we know the circuitry and their impulse patterns, we may know the cause of any element of agency.

Strategies and tactics are proposed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Banich, M. T., Mackiewicz, K. L., Depue, B. E., Whitmer, A. J., Miller, G. A., & Heller, W. (2009). Cognitive control mechanisms, emotions and memory: a neural perspective with implications for psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 613–630.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barik, J., Marti, F., Morel, C. et al. (2013). Chronic stress triggers social aversion via glucocorticoid receptor in dopaminoceptive neurons. Science, 339, 332–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, W. (2000). Irrational man. A study in existential philosophy (p. 279). New York: Anchor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 969–977.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benchenane, K., Peyrache, A., Khamassi, M., Tierney, P. L., Gioanni, Y., Battaglia, F. P., & Wiener, S. I. (2010). Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal-prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron, 66, 921–936.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boden, M. A. (2006). Mind as machine. A history of cognitive science (Vol. 2., pp. 1205–1210). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briggman, K. L. et al. (2006). Optical imaging of neuronal populations during decision-making. Science, 307, 896–901.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunton, B. W., Botvinick, M. M., & Brody, C. D. (2013). Rats and humans can optimally accumulate evidence for decision-making. Science, 340, 95–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buzsáki, G. (2006). Rhythms in the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Buzsáki, G., & Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science, 304, 1926–1929.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, C. E. (1993). Processing of temporal information in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 223–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Changeux, J.-P., Danchin A. (1976). Selective stabilization of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks. Nature 264, 705–712.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chrysikou, E. G., Hamilton, R. H., Coslett, H. B., Datta, A., Bikson, M., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2013). Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use. Cognitive Neuroscience. doi:10.1080/17588928.2013.768221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, Patricia S. (2002). Self-representation in nervous systems. Science, 296, 308–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Pisapia, N. (2013). Unconscious information processing in executive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, Jan. 31. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00021.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, R. J., & Martin, K. A. (2004). Neuronal circuits of the neocortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 419–451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evarts, E. V., & Tanji, J. (1976). Reflex and intended responses in motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons of monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 39, 1069–1080.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Filimon, F., Philiastides, M. G., Nelson, J. D., Kloosterman, N. A., & Heekeren, H. R. (2013). How embodied is perceptual decision making? Evidence for separate processing of perceptual and motor decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(5), 2121–2136. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2334-12.2013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. J. (2000). How brains make up their minds. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. J., Burke, B., & Holmes, M. (2003). Aperiodic phase resetting in scalp EEG of beta-gamma oscillations by state transition at alpha-theta-rates. Human Brain Mapping, 19, 248–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallivan, J. P., McLean, D. A., Flanagan, J. R., & Culham, J. C. (2013). Where one hand meets the other: Limb-specific and action-dependent movement plans decoded from preparatory signals in single human frontoparietal brain areas. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(5), 1991–2008. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0541-12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998). The mind’s past (201 p.). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J., (2004). Consciousness: Creeping up on the Hard Problem. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guitart-Masip, M., Barnes, G. R., Horner, A., Bauer, M., Dolan, R. J., & Duzel, E. (2013). Synchronication of medial temporal lobe and prefrontal rhythms in human decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(2), 442–451.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 382–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ham, T., Leff, A., de Boissezon, X., Joffe, A., & Sharp, D. J. (2013). Cognitive control and the salience network: An investigation of error processing and effective connectivity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(16), 7091–7098.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (1976). Physiological and behavioral significance of hippocampal rhythmic, slow activity (“theta rhythm”). Progress in Neurobiology, 6, 23–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (1990). The behavioral readiness response. In W. R. Klemm & R. P. Vertex (Eds.), Brainstem mechanisms of behavior (pp. 105–145). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R., Li, T. H., & Hernandez, J. L. (2000). Coherent EEG indicators of cognitive binding during ambiguous figure tasks. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 66–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2001). Behavioral arrest: In search of the neural control system. Progress in Neurobiology, 654, 453–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2010). Free will debates: Simple experiments are not so simple. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 6(6), 47–65.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2011a). Atoms of mind. The “ghost in the machine” materializes. New York: Springer International.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2011b). Neural representations of the sense of self. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7(7), 16–30.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2011c). Why does REM sleep occur? A wake-up hypothesis. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5(73), 1–12. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2011.00073.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2012). Sense of self and consciousness: Nature, origins, mechanisms, and implications. In A. E. Cavanna & A. Nani (Eds.), Consciousness: States, mechanisms and disorders. Hauppauge: Nova Science. https://www.novapublishers.com/catalog/product_info.php?products_id=38801.

  • Klemm, W. R. (2013). Just do it. Psychology Today. http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/memorymedic/201303/just-do-it-0.

  • Klemm, W. R. (2014). The brain’s odyssey. New York: Prometheus. (In press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lange, F. P., Rahnev, D. A., Donner, T. H., & Lau, H. (2013). Prestimullus oscillatory activity over motor cortex reflects perceptual expectations. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(4), 1400–1410. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1094-12.2013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B., & Commentators (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. (2006). The timing of brain events: Reply to the “special section” in this journal of September 2004. In Susan Pockett (Ed.), Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 540–547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luk, C.-H., & Wallis, J. D. (2013). Choice coding in frontal cortex during stimulus-guided or action-guided decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(5), 1864–1871.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, George (2003). Consciousness: Respectable, useful, and probably necessary. In B. J. Baars et al. (Eds.), Essential sources in the scientific study of consciousness (pp. 15–33). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. F., & Teitelbaum, P. (1977). New considerations in the neuropsychology of motivated behaviors. In Handbook of psychopharmacology (pp. 201–229). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McMains, S., & Kastner, S. (2011). Interactions of top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in human visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(2), 587–597.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melzack, R., (1989). Phantom limbs, the self and the brain. Canad. G. Psychol. 30, 1-16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich, M. M., Kaas, J. H., Wall, J. T., Sur, M., Nelson, R. J., & Felleman, D. J. (1983a). Progression of change following median nerve section in the cortical representation of the hand in areas 3b and 1 in adult owl and squirrel monkeys. Neuroscience, 10, 639–665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich, M. M., Kaas, J. H., Wall, J. T., Nelson, R. J., Sur, M., & Felleman, D. J. (1983b). Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentation. Neuroscience, 8, 33–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (1995). Faster than thought. Holism, homogeneity and temporal coding. In T. Metzinger (Ed.), Conscious experience (pp. 425–461). Thorverten: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, N., & Brown, W. S. (2007). Did my neurons make me do it? New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Niwa, M., Jaaro-Peled, H., Tankou, S. et al. (2013). Adolescent stress-induced epigenetic control of dopaminergic neurons via glucocorticoids. Science, 339, 335–339.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ogawa, T., & Komatsu, H. (2006). Neuronal dynamics of bottom-up and top-down processes in area V4 of macaque monkeys performing a visual search. Experimental Brain Research, 173, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paz-Alonso, P. M., Bunge, S. A., Anderson, M. C., & Ghetti, S. (2013). Strength of coupling within a mnemonic control network differentiates those who can and cannot suppress memory retrieval. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(11), 5017–5026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pockett, S., Banks, W. P., & Gallagher, S. (Eds.) (2009). Does consciousness cause behavior? Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, D. J., & Giambrone, S. (2001). Reasoning about beliefs: A human specialization? Child Development, 72, 691–695.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rivard, G., & Klemm, W. R. (1990). Sample contact required for complete bull response to oestrous pheromone in cattle. In D. W. MacDonald, D. Muller-Schwarze & S. E. Natynczuk (Eds.), Chemical signals in vertebrates V (pp. 627–633). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, R. T. (2012) Willed action, free will, and the stochastic neurodynamics of decision-making. Frontiers of Integrative Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnint.2012.00068.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salimpoor, V. N., van den Bosch, I., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science, 340, 216–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, S., Grahn, J., Hampshire, A., Mobbs, D., & Dalgleish, T. (2013). Training the emotional brain: improving affective control through emotional working memory training. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(12), 5301–5311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soon, Chun Siong et al. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 13 April. doi:10.1038/nn.2112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soutschek, A., Taylor, P. C. J., Müller, H. J., & Schubert, T. (2013). Dissociable networks control conflict during perception and response selection: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience, 3(13), 5647–5654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Studer, B., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., Robbins, T. W., & Clark, L. (2012). What are the odds? The neural correlates of active choice during gambling. Frontiers in Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00046.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sugihara, G., May, R., Ye, H., Hsieh, C., Deyle, E., Fogarty, M., Munch, S. (2012). Detecting causality in complex ecosystems. Science. 338, 496–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. G., & Alavi, F. N. (2003). A global competitive network for attention. In J. Baars et al. (Eds.), Essential sources in the scientific study of consciousness (pp. 1035–1058). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhagen, L., Dijkerman H. C., Medendorp, W. P., Toni, I. (2013) Hierarchical organization of parieto-frontal circuits during goal-directed action. J. Neurosci, 33(15), 6492–6503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Stein, A., Chiang, C., & König, P. (2000). Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(26), 14748–14753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (2005). Who is the controller of controlled processes? In R. Hassin et al. (Ed.), The new unconscious. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William R. Klemm .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Klemm, W. (2015). Neurobiological Perspectives on Agency: Ten Axioms and Ten Propositions. In: Gruber, C., Clark, M., Klempe, S., Valsiner, J. (eds) Constraints of Agency. Annals of Theoretical Psychology, vol 12. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10130-9_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics