Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Applied Clinical Psychology ((NSSB))

Abstract

Considered most broadly, a person’s behavior is social when its causes or effects include the behavior of others. From the wide range of everyday actions that fit this definition, social scientists have focused on particular, consequential forms of social behavior for experimental study. One of the longest experimental traditions has been the study of cooperation and competition, contrasting social behaviors that are ubiquitous in groups of various types and sizes and the basis of competing economic and organizational philosophies. Although cooperation and competition are often used in a general sense to connote activities that benefit others or gain advantage over them, respectively, definitions in the experimental tradition are more specific. Here cooperation and competition are behaviors that occur in the context of particular contingencies that specify behaviors and the criteria for their reinforcement. With a cooperative contingency, all participants receive a reinforcer if their responses collectively meet a specified performance criterion. With a competitive contingency, reinforcers are distributed unequally based on relative performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H., and Lindsley, O. R. (1956). The reinforcement of cooperation between children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 100–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., and Galizio, M. (1983). Instructional control of human operant behavior. Psychological Record, 33, 495–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, R. L., and Nielsen, J. M. (1974). An experimental analysis of some structural determinants of equitable and inequitable exchange relations. American Sociological Review, 39, 427–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buskist, W. F., Barry, A., Morgan, D., and Rossi, M. (1984). Competitive fixed interval performance in humans: Role of “orienting” instructions. Psychological Record, 34, 241–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buskist, W. F., and Morgan, D. (1987). Competitive fixed-interval performance in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 145–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cerutti, D. T. (1989). Discrimination theory of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 259–276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M. (1962). Effect of relative skill on the amount of competitive facilitation. Psychological Reports, 11, 603–614.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M. (1968). Applications of behavior theory in social psychology: Imitation and competition. In E. C. Simmel, R. A. Hoppe, and G. A. Milton (Eds.), Social facilitation and imitative behavior (pp. 135–167 ). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D. J. (1962). Justin and his peers: An experimental analysis of a child’s social world. Child Development, 33, 697–717.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D. J., and Lindsley, O. R. (1964). Catalysis of controlled leadership in cooperation by human stimulation. Journal of Child Psychological Psychiatry, 5, 119–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, D. M., and Cherek, D. R. (1994). Effects of social context, reinforcer probability, and reinforcer magnitude on humans’ choices to compete or not to compete. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 133–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., and Freed, D. E. (1993). The substitutability of reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 141–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., and Olvera, D. (1978). Cooperation, competition, and related social phenomena. In A. C. Catania (Ed.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis (pp. 208–245 ). New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., Olvera, D., and Bell, J. C. (1975). Switching from competition or cooperation at large response requirements: Competition requires more responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 24, 343–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., and Schmid, T. L. (1981). Acquisition and maintenance of trusting behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 109–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., and Vukelich, R. (1972). A classification and review of cooperation procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, 333–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., and Vukelich, R. (1973). Analysis of the control exerted by a complex cooperation procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 3–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., Vukelich, R., and Kaplan, S. J. (1973). Audit responses: Responses maintained by access to existing self or coactor scores during non-social parallel work, and cooperation procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 409–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., Vukelich, R., and Olvera, D. (1975). The measurement of sharing and cooperation as equity effects and some relationships between them. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 63–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1979). Personal relationships: Their structures and processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H., and Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesler, S., Sproull, L., and Waters, K. (1996). A prisoner’s dilemma experiment on cooperation with people and human-like computers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 47–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, O. R. (1966). Experimental analysis of cooperation and competition. In T. Verhave (Ed.), The experimental analysis of behavior (pp. 470–501 ). New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marwell, G., and Schmitt, D. R. (1972). Cooperation and interpersonal risk: Cross-cultural and cross-procedural generalizations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 594–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marwell, G., and Schmitt, D. R. (1975). Cooperation: An experimental analysis. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marwell, G., Schmitt, D. R., and Boyesen, B. (1973). Pacifist strategy and cooperation under interpersonal risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 12–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marwell, G., Schmitt, D. R., and Shotola, R. (1971). Cooperation and interpersonal risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 9–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B. A. (1977). Magnitudes of score differences produced within sessions in a cooperative exchange procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 331–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B. A. (1979). Effects of fixed and alternated payoff inequity on dyadic competition. The Psychological Record, 29, 329–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B. A., Kordonski, W. M., and Shimoff, E. (1983). Temptation and the maintenance of trust: Effects of bilateral punishment capability. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 27, 255–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B. A., and Shimoff, E. (1979). Monitoring trust levels in ongoing exchange relations. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 23, 538–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, R., and Bernstein, D. J. (1991). Transient effects of acquisition history on generalization in a matching-to-sample task. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 155–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, J. W., and Wiggins, J. A. (1976). Effects of mutual dependency and dependency asymmetry on social exchange. Sociometry, 39, 368–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mithaug, D. E. (1969). The development of cooperation in alternative task situations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 443–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mithaug, D. E., and Burgess, R. L. (1967). The effects of different reinforcement procedures in the establishment of a group response. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 5, 441–454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mithaug, D. E., and Burgess, R. L. (1968). The effects of different reinforcement contingencies in the development of cooperation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 6, 402–426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D. (1980). The effects of structural variations in social reinforcement contingencies on exchange and cooperation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 43, 269–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D. (1981a). Power use in the dyad: The effects of structure, knowledge, and interaction history. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 42–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D. (1981b). The conversion of power imbalance to power use. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 151–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D. (1984). The disruption and recovery of dyadic social interaction. In E. J. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group process (Vol. 1, pp. 183–227 ). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D. (1985). Relative effects of individual dependencies: Further tests of the relation between power imbalance and power use. Social Forces, 63, 810–837.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D. (1988). The structure and use of power: A comparison of reward and punishment power. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51, 108–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D. (1990). Structure, action, and outcomes: The dynamics of power in social exchange. American Sociological Review, 55, 427–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. D., and Cook, K. S. (1995). Social exchange and exchange networks. In K. S. Cook, G. A. Fine, and J. S. House (Eds.), Sociological perspectives on social psychology (pp. 209–235 ). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olvera, D. R., and Hake, D. F. (1976). Producing a change from competition to sharing: Effects of large and adjusting response requirements. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 321–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapoport, A., and Chammah, A. (1965). Prisoner’s dilemma. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, S. (1960). Cooperative behavior in dyads as a function of reinforcement parameters. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 318–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sally, D. (1995). Conversation and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-analysis of experiments from 1958 to 1992. Rationality and Society, 7, 58–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savastano, H. I., and Fantino, E. (1994). Human choice in concurrent ratio-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 453–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, T. L., and Hake, D. F. (1982). Fast acquisition of cooperation and trust: A two-stage view of trusting behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R. (1976). Some conditions affecting the choice to cooperate or compete. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 165–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R. (1986). Competition: Some behavioral issues. The Behavior Analyst, 9, 27–34.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R. (1987). Interpersonal contingencies: Performance differences and cost-effectiveness. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 221–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R., and Marwell, G. (1968). Stimulus control in the experimental study of cooperation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 571–574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R., and Marwell, G. (1971a). Taking and the disruption of cooperation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 405–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R., and Marwell, G. (1971b). Avoidance of risk as a determinant of cooperation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, 367–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R., and Marwell, G. (1972). Withdrawal and reward re-allocation as responses to inequity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 207–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. R., and Marwell, G. (1977). Cooperation and the human group. In R. L. Hamblin and J. H. Kunkel (Eds.), Behavioral theory in sociology (pp. 171–191 ). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., and Matthews, B. A. (1975). Unequal reinforcer magnitudes and relative preference for cooperation in the dyad. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 24, 1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidowski, J. B., Wyckoff, L. B., and Tabory, L. (1956). The influence of reinforcement and punishment in a minimal social situation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 115–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiga, R., Cherek, D. R., Grabowski, R., and Bennett, R. H. (1992). Effects of inequity on human free-operant cooperative responding: A validation study. The Psychological Record, 42, 29–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shibaut, J. W., and Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn, M. (1989). Rule-governed behavior in behavior analysis: A theoretical and experimental history. In S. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 97–118 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vukelich, R., and Hake, D. F. (1974). Effects of the difference between self and coactor scores upon the audit responses that allow access to these scores. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 61–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weingarten, R., and Mechner, F. (1966). The contingency as an independent variable of social interaction. In T. Verhave (Ed.), The experimental analysis of behavior (pp. 447–459 ). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willer, D. (1992). Predicting power in exchange networks: A brief history and introduction to the issues. Social Networks, 14, 187–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schmitt, D.R. (1998). Social Behavior. In: Lattal, K.A., Perone, M. (eds) Handbook of Research Methods in Human Operant Behavior. Applied Clinical Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1947-2_15

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1947-2_15

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-1949-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-1947-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics