Skip to main content

Lies and Decision Making

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
  • 154 Accesses

Abstract

Within the economics and law fields, many decisions must be based on what is reported by another person, who can deceive the decision maker by lying. Thus, discovering if our interlocutor is sincere or not is crucial in order to make good decisions. Research highlighted that the spontaneous strategies that we use to identify possible lies are often misleading. Our moods and personality, together with the level of trust between speakers, are all factors that can influence the detection of lies. However, the ability to discover lies may increase with appropriate training and experience of dealing with people in contexts where the probability of being deceived is quite high. Regardless of our actual skill in discovering lies, our attitude toward lying can influence the decision-making process. For example, when we are aware of the possibility that a lie occurs, a suspicious attitude can lead to a wrong judgment. Similarly, perceiving an alleged lie, whether it is real or not, can prompt the use of emotional heuristics linked to the perceived feelings of antipathy, anxiety, or anger. This can lead to decisions aimed at creating disadvantages for the partner. Under these circumstances, it is necessary to take into account the variety of human behavior, not relying on stereotypes to identify a lie. Being used to interact in particular contexts where the risk of being deceived is high may definitely help to sharpen the ability to find out who is lying to us, increasing the likelihood of taking rational choices based on reliable cues.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

References

  • Antal A, Terney D, Poreisz C, Paulus W (2007) Towards unravelling task-related modulations of neuroplastic changes induced in the human motor cortex. Eur J Neurosci 26:2687–2691

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldi PL, Iannello P, Riva S, Antonietti A (2013) Socially biased decisions are associated to individual differences in cognitive reflection. Stud Psychol 55:265–271

    Google Scholar 

  • Bembich S, Clarici A, Vecchiet C, Baldassi G, Cont G, Demarini S (2014) Differences in time course activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex associated with low or high risk choices in a gambling task. Front Hum Neurosci 24(8):464

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell S (2000) Romantic deception – the six signs he’s lying. Adams Media Corp., Holbrook

    Google Scholar 

  • Camden C, Motley MX, Wilson A (1984) White lies in interpersonal communication: a taxonomy and preliminary investigation of social motivations. West J Speech Commun 48:309–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colombo B, Rodella C, Riva S, Antonietti A (2013) The effects of lies on economic decision making. An eye-tracking study. Res Psychol Behav Sci 1(3):38–47

    Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo BM (1994) Spotting lies: can humans learn to do better? Psychol Sci 3(3):83–86

    Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo BM, Kashy DA (1998) Everyday lies in close and casual relationships. J Pers Soc Psychol 74:63–79

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo BM, Pfeifer RL (1986) On-the-job experience and skill at detecting deception. J Appl Soc Psychol 16:249–267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo BM, Kashy DA, Kirkendol SE, Wyer MM, Epstein JA (1996) Lying in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol 70:979–995

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding WN, Sun JH, Sun YW, Chen X, Zhou Y, Zhuang ZG, Li L, Zhang Y, Xu JR, Du YS (2014) Trait impulsivity and impaired prefrontal impulse inhibition function in adolescents with internet gaming addiction revealed by a Go/No-Go fMRI study. Behav Brain Funct 30:10–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman P (2009) Telling lies: clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage. WW Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman P, Frank MG (1993) Lies that fail. In: Lewis M, Saarni C (eds) Lying and deception in everyday life. Guilford Press, New York, pp 184–200

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman P, O’Sullivan M (1991) Who can catch a liar? Am Psychol 46:913–920

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman P, O’Sullivan M, Frank MG (1999) A few can catch a liar. Psychol Sci 10(3):263–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etcoff NL, Ekman P, Magee JJ, Frank MG (2000) Lie detection and language comprehension. Nature 405(6783):139–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice HP (1989) Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Hample D (1980) Purposes and effects of lying. South Speech Commun J 46:33–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartwig M, Bond CF Jr (2011) Why do lie-catchers fail? A lens model meta-analysis of human lie judgments. Psychol Bull 137:643–659

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee CK, Rottenstreich Y, Xiao Z (2005) When is more better? On the relationship between magnitude and subjective value. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 24:234–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iannello P, Antonietti A (2008) Reciprocity in financial decision making: intuitive and analytical mind-reading strategies. Int Rev Econ 55:167–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iannello P, Colombo B, Antonietti A (2014) Non-invasive brain stimulation techniques in the study of intuition. In: Sinclair M (ed) Handbook of research methods on intuition. Edward Elgar, Northampton, pp 130–143

    Google Scholar 

  • Iannello P, Balconi M, Antonietti A (2014) Analytical versus intuitive decision making modulates trust in e-commerce. Neuropsychol Trends 16:31–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen LA, Arnett JJ, Feldman SS, Cauffman E (2004) The right to do wrong: lying to parents among adolescents and emerging adults. J Youth Adolesc 33:101–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D (1994) New Challenges to the Rationality Assumption. J Inst Theor Econ, 150(1):18–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraut RE, Poe D (1980) Behavioral roots of person perception; the deception judgments of customs inspectors and laypersons. J Pers Soc Psychol 39:784–796

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo HI, Bikson M, Datta A, Minhas P, Paulus W, Kuo MF, Nitsche MA (2013) Comparing cortical plasticity induced by conventional and high-definition 4 × 1 ring tDCS: a neurophysiological study. Brain Stimul 6:644–648

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang N, Siebner HR, Ernst D, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Lemon RN, Rothwell JC (2004) Preconditioning with transcranial direct current stimulation sensitizes the motor cortex to rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation and controls the direction of after-effects. Biol Psychiatry 56:634–639

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee I, Byeon JS (2014) Learning-dependent changes in the neuronal correlates of response inhibition in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Exp Neurobiol 23:178–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCornack SA, Levine TR (1990) When lies are uncovered: Emotional and relational outcomes of discovered deception. Communication Monographs 57:119–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nitsche MA, Paulus W (2001) Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57:1899–1901

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Sullivan M (2009) Why most people parse palters, fibs, lies, whoppers, and other deceptions poorly. In: Harrington B (ed) Deception: from ancient empires to internet dating. Stanford University Press, Berkeley, pp 74–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell K (2003) Economy of the mind. PLoS Biol 3:312–315

    Google Scholar 

  • Riva S, Monti M, Iannello P, Pravettoni G, Schulz PJ, Antonietti A (2014) A preliminary mixed-method investigation of trust and hidden signals in medical consultations. PLosONE 9(3):e90941. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0090941

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal R, DePaulo BM (1979) Sex differences in eavesdropping on nonverbal cues. J Pers Soc Psychol 37:273–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz N (2002) Feeling as information: moods influence judgments and processing strategies. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D (eds) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 534–547

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, McGregor DG (2002) The affect heuristic. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D (eds) Heuristics and bias: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 397–420

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich KE, West RF (2000) Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate? Behav Brain Sci 23:645–665

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toris C, DePaulo BM (1984) Effects of actual deception and suspiciousness of deception on interpersonal perceptions. J Pers Soc Psychol 47:1063–1073

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner RE, Edgley C, Olmstead G (1975) Information control in conversations: honesty is not always the best policy. Kans J Sociol 11:69–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Vrij A, Semin GR (1996) Lie experts’ beliefs about nonverbal indicators of deception. J Nonverbal Behav 20:65–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alessandro Antonietti .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this entry

Cite this entry

Antonietti, A., Colombo, B., Rodella, C. (2014). Lies and Decision Making. In: Backhaus, J. (eds) Encyclopedia of Law and Economics. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-7883-6_582-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-7883-6_582-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4614-7883-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Economics and FinanceReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics