Introduction

Developing an animal model of mental disorders is controversial due to the human nature of the symptoms such as hallucinations, delusions and poverty of speech. These symptoms can only be adequately assessed by psychological assessments and therefore cannot be modulated in animals. A way to try to circumvent these problems is studying certain psychological or psychophysiological aspects of mental disorders such as latent inhibition, prepulse inhibition or P50 gating (Ellenbroek and Cools 1990, 1995; Geyer and Markou 1995; Ellenbroek et al. 2004). However, it is still unclear how (and if) these abnormalities are linked to the symptoms of mental illness. In rodent models prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle response is a model of sensorimotor gating mechanisms in the brain, while an equivalent reaction in humans is eye blinking (Braff et al. 1978; Ellenbroek et al. 1998). It has been shown that rat pups that underwent 24 h maternal separation on postnatal day (PND) 6 or 9 expressed reduced prepulse inhibition (PPI) on postnatal day 69 and had hyperactivity of the dopamineric system involving the dopamine neurotransmitter system via the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Ellenbroek and Cools 1995). Further, the PPI deficits could be reversed with typical antipsychotic drugs like haloperidol and were not detected prior to puberty (Ellenbroek et al. 1998). Due to the changes seen in the HPA axis, the dopamine system, hippocampus and long-term behavioural effects modelling deficits seen in mental patients has lead Ellenbroek and co-workers to hypothesis the 24-h maternal deprivation model to be a “schizophrenia-like” neurodevelopmental animal model (Ellenbroek and Cools 1998, 2002; Ellenbroek et al. 1998, 2004, 2005).

One of the striking characteristics of the developing neuroendocrine stress system in the mouse and rat is a period of reduced stress-responsiveness, the so-called stress hypo-responsive period (SHRP) (Schapiro et al. 1962; Cirulli et al. 1994; Schmidt et al. 2002). From about postnatal day (PND) 4–14 in the rat, and PND 1–12 in the mouse, the animals neuroendocrine system is characterized by a low basal corticosterone level and by the inability of a mild stressor, e.g. exposure to novelty, to induce a corticosterone response (Cirulli et al. 1994; Schmidt et al. 2003). In this study, we used the C57BL/6 inbred mouse. Due to a similar postnatal neurodevelopmental course in the mouse and rat (Clancy et al. 2001), we used PND 9 as the day of separation equivalent to Ellenbroek and co-workers day of choice in the rat. We tested if a 24-h maternal separation on PND 9 can cause an adult phenotype characterized by altered levels of activity and anxiety, learning and memory dysfunction, deficits in behavioural flexibility (reversal deficits) as well as changes in number of neurons in the hippocampus and its subregions in the mouse brain.

Material and methods

Animals

The offspring of 4 male and 8 female C57BL/6J mice (8 weeks old) obtained from Taconic Europe (Taconic Farms Inc., DK) were used in this study. The animals were acclimatized to the animal facility for 1 week. Multiparous females were used, since there is a higher rate of offspring survival. The animals were housed under a 12:12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 6 a.m.) with constant temperature (21 ± 2°C) and humidity (52 ± 2%) in Macrolon type III cages with environmental enrichment in the form of wood splints bedding (aspen 4HV), wood shavings, 1 piece of Aspen Corner 15, 1 standard mouse house made of recycled cardboard, 1 aspen chewing stick size medium, and pads of nesting material (all obtained from Brogaarden, DK). Food (Altromin pills NR 1324) and tap water were available ad libitum. Two females were placed in a male’s cage for a period of 1 week to ensure conception, followed by separation of the two females to their own cages.

Maternal separation

Pregnant females were checked for litters daily at 09:00 a.m. If litters were found, the day of birth was defined as PND 0 for that litter. On PND 0, litters were randomly assigned to maternal separation (MS) (N = 16, 9 males and 7 females), or to standard facility rearing (SFR). For the behavioural testing, 16 MS animals (4 males and 6 females) and 5 SFR animals (2 males and 3 females) were included, while 12 of the MS animals (2 males and 10 females) and 7 control animals (2 males and 5 females) were used for cell counting. Litters were not culled or sexed at birth to minimize the handling of the pups, but male and female pups were separated at weaning (PND 28) and group housed with their siblings, which resulted in 2–5 mice per cage. The environmental enrichment applied to the mothers was also applied to the MS and control animals.

The 24-h deprivation was carried out on PND 9 starting at 8 a.m. The pups remained in the home cage but were placed in a separate room with the same temperature, humidity and lighting conditions as the home stable. The cage was placed on a heating pad, which had a constant temperature of 31°C. No food or water was available during the separation. The dam was placed in a cage with similar facilities as the home cage in the home stable. The pups were checked every 3 h, using a red light during the night. Body weights were recorded before and after separation. Immediately after 24 h the dams were returned to the home cage and reunited with the pups.

Test for anxiety and fear related behaviour

When the pups reached 10 weeks of age they were subjected once to the open field test (Hall 1934) and the elevated plus maze test, which is based on the procedure used by Montgomery (1955) and later validated by Pellow et al. (1985). Behaviour was analysed using EthoVision (Noldus, Groeningen, The Netherlands). All behavioural testing took place between 10 a.m. and 3 p.m.

Open field test (OFT)

The open field consisted of a circular wooden platform (diameter 90 cm) surrounded by a 43 cm high wall with a camera mounted directly above. A central circle of 31 cm diameter was defined in the behaviour analysis software. Three 60-W light bulbs illuminated the arena. On the day of testing each animal was transported in a cardboard box to the centre of the open field and behaviour was recorded for 10 min. After the trial the maze was cleaned with a solution of acetic acid and soap water and faecal boli were counted.

The following parameters were calculated; total distance moved (cm), time spent in central circle and time spent in peripheral zone (expressed as % of session duration).

Elevated plus maze

The plus maze was elevated 50 cm above the ground and consisted of two opposing open arms (21 cm × 8 cm) connected by a central square (8 cm × 8 cm) to two opposing enclosed arms of the same size with 32 cm high walls. A video camera placed above the maze recorded the animals’ behaviour. On the day of testing, the animal was placed in the centre of the maze and behaviour was recorded for 10 min. Between trials the maze was cleaned as described for the OFT. The following parameters were calculated; total duration (s) in open and closed arms, the number of entries into the open and closed arms and the total distance moved (cm). From these parameters the ratio of entries into the open arms to the total number of arm entries, and the ratio of time spent in the open arms to time spent in both open and closed arms was calculated.

Test for spatial memory; Barnes maze

The Barnes maze (Barnes 1975) consisted of a circular, white-coated platform 90 cm in diameter and elevated 50 cm over the ground. Sixteen 5 cm wide holes were evenly distributed around the perimeter, 2.5 cm from the edge. A pair of rails was placed under two opposing holes to hold the hidden escape box. The escape box was a dark plastic storage box with a 5 cm diameter hole in the lid. A dark cylindrical cardboard tube (7.5 cm × 7 cm high) with a lid was used as the transport and start chamber.

Three 60-W bulbs illuminating the maze and high irregular rock and techno music played from a computer in a random manner provided the aversive stimuli. As with the previous test, a digital video camera mounted above the maze recorded animal’s behaviours.

Shaping

For 2 days before testing commenced, the animals were trained to enter the hidden escape box. Using the transport cylinder, the mouse was placed near the edge of the target hole with the hidden escape box underneath. Two cardboard walls blocked entry to adjacent holes. Only dim lightning and no noise was used during this phase of the experiment. The animal was allowed 5 min to enter the escape box and if this failed, it was placed manually inside. When the animal entered the box it was quickly carried to the home cage.

Acquisition trials

Six consecutive trials were given, one per day. The animal was placed in the transport cylinder, oriented in a random direction, in the centre of the maze. The aversive stimuli were turned on and the cylinder removed. Recording in the behaviour-observation software began immediately after the experimenter had left the room. The trial ended after 5 min or when the animal entered the hidden escape box. If the animal failed to enter the box or re-entered the maze after recording was stopped, the aversive stimuli were turned back on and the animal was allowed 5 min more to enter the escape box. If that also failed, the animal was manually placed in the box. After completion of each trial, the box was placed in the home cage.

Reversal trials

Three days after acquisition trials, the escape box was placed underneath the hole opposite to the hole that had been the target during acquisition training. Reversal training was conducted for seven consecutive days as described above.

Parameters

Each of the 16 escape holes in the Barnes maze was defined as a separate zone-of-interest in the behaviour analysis software. The following parameters were analysed: latency to target [time from start of the trial to first entry into the target hole zone (s)], total distance moved (cm) and error frequency (number of visits to other holes than the target hole). During reversal training two additional parameters were analysed: the mean number of visits to the old target hole over the seven trials (the hole where the escape box was located during acquisition training) and the mean number of visits to the two holes adjacent to the old target hole.

Three different search strategies could be distinguished and were scored manually. The random search strategy is characterized by a non-systematic exploration of the maze with many centre maze crossings and some perseverations. Perseverations were defined as repeatedly searching the same hole or two adjacent holes (Bach et al. 1995). Secondly, there is the serial search strategy, defined as systematic consecutive hole searching in a clockwise or counter clockwise manner and finally a spatial search strategy, defined as searching less than three holes from the location of the goal (Barnes 1979; Bach et al. 1995; Inman-Wood et al. 2000; Zhang et al. 2002; Raber et al. 2004) (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Three different search strategies could be applied: Random, a pattern that include many hole examinations in a random manner; Serial, a relative systematic search either clockwise or counterclockwise; Spatial, a pattern where the target box is found within relative short time and with high accuracy

Fixation and embedding

Approximately 1 week after all tests were concluded, the animals were scarified by CO2 asphyxiation and within 1 h the brains were dissected out and placed in a 37% formalin solution (Fixatin). The brains were split through corpus callosum, separating the two hemispheres. After systematic random assignment of left or right hemisphere they were coloured on the outer surface to preserve a coded sequence and embedded in paraffin with an automatic vacuum tissue processor (Leica ASP300). The hemispheres were then mounted horizontally balancing on needles and embedded in a paraffin block with up to four hemispheres in one block

Sectioning

The whole hemisphere was cut horizontally with a Leica (model SM 2400) microtome with a microtome setting of 40 μm thickness. All sections were mounted on silicone-coated glass (Frost +). After a minimum of 24 h in a 40°C heating cupboard, the sections were sampled systematic uniformly randomly (SURS). The first section was randomly selected from a random number table, hereafter every 6th section was sampled systematically for staining and counting. This provided a total of 8–10 sections containing the hippocampus per specimen. To be able to account for block advance (BA) the block height was measured for every 100th section. The BA determines the hitting probability of the particles within the block (see later for calculation). Furthermore, the paraffin shrinkage effect was calculated and amounted to about 70%. The sections were stained with a modified Vogt’s Cresyl violet acetate (Armed forces).

Optical design equipment

The optical design equipment consisted of an Olympus BH-2 microscope with an oil immersion 100× objective of high numerical aperture (NA = 1.40), which allows focusing in a thin focal plane inside a thick section. A camera transmits the image to a computer screen where a counting frame is superimposed using the computer-assisted stereological (CAST)-Grid software (Visiopharm, Hørsholm, DK). A motorized automatic stage was used to control movement in the x,y- plane via a connected joystick. Movement in the z-axis was controlled manually with the focus button on the microscope and the distance between the upper and lower surfaces of the sections was measured with a Heidenhein microcator (Heidenhain, Germany) with a precision of 0.5 μm.

Definitions and divisions of the hippocampus

The hippocampus was subdivided into five regions; the dentate gyrus (DG), the hilus of the dentate gyrus (CA4), regio inferior (CA3/2), region superior (CA1) and subiculum.

Neurons were counted manually in the sub-sampled sections containing the hippocampus, using the optical disector design. The layers comprising the hippocampus were defined consistently in all sections using the terminology of Blackstad (1956). It was not considered too difficult to differentiate the neuronal layers in horizontal sections (Fig. 2). The granule cell layer and the pyramidal cell layers also contain the cell bodies of basket cells and glial cells. The glial cells can be easily identified and counted separately, but the basket cells are so similar in appearance to the granule and pyramidal cells that they are included in the estimates. It was previously shown that they comprise less than 1% of the neurons in these layers (review West et al. 1991).

Fig. 2
figure 2

A schematic drawing of the hippocampus with the five subregions identified in this study. Dg, the granule cell layer of the dentate gyrus; h/CA4, hilus of the dentate gyrus; ri, regio inferior; CA3/2, rs, region superior; CA1, s, subiculum

Stereological equations

Estimation of the total neuron number, N

For the estimation of total neuron numbers, neurons were counted in optical disectors and sampled according to the so-called fractionator principles. In a fractionator, cells are counted directly in a known fraction of the different hippocampal subdivisions and the total neuron number, N, is estimated by multiplying the number of particles counted with the reciprocal sampling fractions:

$$ N = \frac{1} {\text{ssf}} \times \frac{1} {\text{asf}} \times \frac{1} {\text{hsf}} \times \sum {Q^{ - } } \times 2 $$

where ssf is the section sampling fractions, asf the area sampling fraction, and hsf the height sampling fraction. The bilateral cell number is estimated by multiplying the unilateral number ∑Q by 2. This is admissible when the right or left hippocampus is sampled systematically randomly.

The asf is known as the area of the counting frame of the disector relative to the area associated with each x,y-step movement of the disector:

$$ \text{asf} = \frac{a({\text{frame}})\,\upmu{\text{m}}^{2} } xy\,\upmu{\text{m}}^{2} $$

After having ascertained that the cell density was constant within the disector height, the height sampling fraction was defined as:

$$ {\text{hsf}}: = \frac{h} {{{{\mathop t \limits^ -}_{{q^{ - } } } }}}\quad {\text{where}}\ {\mathop t\limits^ - }_{{q^{ - } }} = \frac{{{\sum\limits_i {{\left( {t_{i} q^{ - }_{i} } \right)}} }}} {{{\sum\limits_i {q^{ - }_{i} } }}} $$

is the q weighted mean section thickness (Gundersen et al. 1988; Dorph-Petersen 2001).

Estimation of total volume—the Cavalieri estimator

Besides estimating total neuron numbers, it was also decided to estimate total volume of the different compartments of the hippocampus although the main purpose was estimation of cell numbers. Estimates of volume were obtained according to the principles of Cavalieri’s basic estimator (Gundersen and Jensen 1987) and corrected for shrinkage. Notice that these volumes were not used for the estimation of total neuron number. The estimates of total neuron number obtained by the fractionator design are independent of the containing volume. Further, when total number and volume is estimated, the density, N V (cells/mm3) can be obtained as well without extra work.

Error predictions

The precision of the two different estimates (neuron number and volume) can be expressed by the coefficient of error, CE. The CE for the Cavalieri estimation of volume was first formulated and described by Gundersen and Jensen (1987) revised in Gundersen et al. (1999). Table 1 gives an example of how CE for both volume and neuron estimates are calculated in this study (see also Gundersen et al. 1999).

Table 1 Example of how the CE is calculated for neuron number and volume respectively

When an appropriate number of sections have been chosen (10 or a little less), it is the number of points counted (the noise) which decides the precision of the estimate. To count about 200 points per sample is usually enough to obtain a CE around 5–8%, unless the object is very irregular (Gundersen and Jensen 1987; Gundersen et al. 1988; West and Gundersen 1990). When CE is estimated for the cell counting, Noise is equal to ∑Q .

Statistical analyses

All data were analysed with the statistical software packages SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 14.00) or SigmaStat (version 2.0). If Levene’s test for equality of variances did not fail, a t test was applied to test for differences between the two experimental groups. For data where normality tests failed, the non-parametric tests Mann–Whitney U test and Kruskal–Wallis test were applied. The animals performance over the six acquisition trials and the seven reversal trials in the Barnes maze was analysed by mixed-model analysis of variance with trial as within-subject factor and experimental group as between-subjects factor. Homogeneity of the variance-difference scores was determined by Mauchly’s test of sphericity [SPSS Inc. Chicago, IL, USA. SPSS Base version 11.5 User Manual; 2004]. When the assumption of sphericity was violated, degrees of freedom were adjusted with the Huynh–Feldt correction. Group differences in strategy selection in the Barnes maze were analysed with the likelihood-ratio chi-square test for each trial separately. Group differences were considered significant when P < 0.05.

Results

Only significant behavioural results are depicted and statistically elaborated.

Body weights

A significant weight loss was found in the pups after separation (P = 0.019, paired t test). However, 8 weeks after separation the body weights of MS and SFR animals were similar (P = 0.61, unpaired t test).

Open field test

Both experimental groups spent more time in the periphery of the open field than the centre (P < 0.001, t test). There was no significant difference between MS and SFR groups in either distance moved or frequency of visits to and time spent in the centre or periphery of the open field (data not shown).

Elevated plus maze

The MS animals spent more time in the open arms than the SFR animals and allotted a greater percentage of time spent in both open and closed arms to the open arms (P = 0.035 and P = 0.046, t test, respectively; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The mean (+SEM) time spent in both the open and closed arms. *There was a significant difference between MS 24 (N = 16) and SFR 24 (N = 5) in time spent in open arms (P < 0.05, ANOVA). ** Time spent in closed arms compared with time spent in open arms (P < 0.001, t test)

In trial progress there was a significant difference in both frequency and duration in closed and open arms [Closed arms frequency: (F (9,171) = 3.544, P < 0.0001) and duration: (Wilk’s λ F (9,11) = 6.045, P = 0.004), Open arms frequency: (Wilk’s λ F (9,11) = 8.086, P = 0.001) and duration: (F (9,171) = 6.253, P < 0.0001)].

There was no significant difference in any of the other parameters analysed.

Barnes maze

Acquisition trials

Latency to reach the target hole, total distance moved on the maze, and error frequency all decreased significantly over the course of the six acquisition trials (F (4.3,77.4) = 3.88, P = 0.005; F (5,90) = 2.56, P = 0.033; F (3.9,70.1) = 2.49, P = 0.05; respectively), indicating that the animals indeed learned the task. There was, however, no significant difference between the two experimental groups.

Reversal trials

Latency to reach the new target hole (Fig. 4 left) total distance moved on the maze (Fig. 5), and error frequency (Fig. 6) all decreased significantly over the course of the seven reversal trials (F (3.1;55.95) = 3.08, P < 0.05; F (1.85;33.41) = 16.24, P < 0.001; F (3.43,61.68) = 18.79, P < 0.001, respectively). The MS24 animals made significantly more errors over the course of reversal training (F (1, 18) = 4.79, P < 0.05; Fig. 6) and travelled longer distances (Fig. 5; P = 0.008, Mann–Whitney U-test). This might indicate that the MS animals did not learn the reversal task as fast as the controls.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Left: Mean (±SEM) latency to “Goal”. There was a significant difference in Trial progress and a significant Trial x Group interaction (P < 0.05). Right: Mean (±SEM) frequency of visits to “Old Goal”. There was * a significant difference between the two groups (P < 0.05, Mann–Whitney U test) and a significant decrease over time (P < 0.05, RM ANOVA). MS 24 (N = 16) and SFR (N = 5)

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean (±SEM) distance moved in reverse trials. There was a significant decrease over time (P < 0.05, RM ANOVA) and * a significant difference between the two treatment groups (P < 0.05, Mann–Whitney U test) but no significant Trial x Group interaction. MS 24 (N = 16) and SFR (N = 5)

Fig. 6
figure 6

Mean (±SEM) frequency of errors in MS 24 (N = 16) and SFR (N = 5) * there was a significant difference between the two treatment groups and a significant decrease in Trial progress (P < 0.05). MS 24 (N = 16) and SFR (N = 5)

The MS24 animals made significantly more visits to the old target hole (Fig. 4 right) and the two adjacent holes over the course of the seven reversal trials (t (17.5) = 3.72, P < 0.01; t (17.5) = 4.34, P < 0.001).

This tendency to venture to the old goal and the adjacent holes could be an indication of perseveration, a behavioural pattern also found in other animal models and in schizophrenic patients (Bleuler 1950; Bach et al. 1995; review Crider 1997)

Search strategies

The animal’s search pattern evolved over time (Fig. 7). The usual behaviour sequence was a progression from a random to a serial, and finally to a spatial search strategy. This sequence indicates that the animals became more accurate and more efficient in locating the correct hole. Likelihood-ratio chi-square tests showed that the experimental groups differed in their choice of search strategy on days 2 and 3 of acquisition training (χ(2) = 10.5, P < 0.01; χ(2) = 6.6, P < 0.05, respectively). Already at this early time the SFR animals adopted a serial search strategy while the MS animals maintained a random search strategy. On all other acquisition and reversal training days the two experimental groups did not differ significantly with respect to their choice of search strategy.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Overview of mean (%) search strategies applied in the acquisition and reverse trial sessions. The search pattern evolved over time from a random to a serial, and finally to a spatial search strategy. From acquisition days 2 and 3, the SFR animals adopted a serial search strategy, while the MS animals maintained a random search strategy. On all other acquisition and reversal training days the two experimental groups did not differ significantly with respect to their choice of search strategy. SFR (N = 5) animals to the left and MS (N = 16) animals to the right

Neuron number

The stereological sampling is shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Overview of the stereological sampling used in this study

There was no significant difference in the total bilateral neuron number in hippocampus (Table 3). However, in the five subregions of hippocampus, there was a significant difference in the dentate gyrus (P = 0.029, Students t test), but none in the four other regions (see Fig. 8; Table 3). The neuron loss in the dentate gyrus is equivalent to a 20% reduction in the maternal separated animals compared with controls. The results should be interpreted with caution due to the low number of subjects.

Table 3 Total estimated neuron number in five subregions of the hippocampus
Fig. 8
figure 8

The total neuron number for the two treatment groups in five sub-regions of the mouse hippocampus for the pooled data in MS (N = 12) and SFR (N = 7). * MS ≠ SFR, P < 0.05, Student´s t test in the dentate gyrus. filled circle MS; open circle SFR; Sub Subiculum, CA 4 Hilus, DG Dentate Gyrus

Volume

After Cavalieri estimation of volume and correction for shrinkage (67.7%) a Student’s t test did not show any significant difference in either the total hippocampal volume or in the five subregions of the hippocampus between groups (Table 4).

Table 4 Total estimated volume in five subregions of the hippocampus

Discussion

In the present study, 24 h maternal separation on PND 9 in mice pups resulted in a 20% neuron decrease in the dentate gyrus and behavioural perseverations in the Barnes maze. The anxiety and thus stress related behavioural tests conducted did not show any indications of an elevated anxiety level. On the contrary, the MS mice showed a reduced sign of anxiety, since they ventured more often onto the open arms than the equivalent control group. This could indicate that the SHRP was not repressed, in spite of our expectations. The results were thus surprising and contradicted some but not all other findings (Pihoker et al. 1993; Plotsky and Meany 1993; Cirulli et al. 1994; Wigger and Neuman 1999; Boccia and Pedersen 2001; Parfitt et al. 2004). A study by Lehman et al. (1999) found that MS 24 at PND 9 did not result in the anxiety/fear response predicted by the group (Lehmann et al. 1999). Others have also found that effects of a manipulation of the HPA axis cannot always be used to predict effects of the same manipulation of fear/anxiety expression at the behavioural level (see Lehmann et al. 1999; Parfitt et al. 2004). Furthermore, Parfitt et al. (2004) reported that maternally separated C57BL/6 male mice had a prolonged increase in plasma CORT after an acute stressor, but in adulthood showed no increased fear/anxiety behavioural response (Parfitt et al. 2004). The lack of fear response does evidently not necessarily mean that the corticosterone plasma level is not elevated in MS animals but since the corticosterone level was not measured this question remains unanswered. Finally, a study by Francis et al. (2002) concluded that MS rats subjected to an enriched environment could reverse the effect from MS on the HPA function and anxiety behaviour (Francis et al. 2002). In conclusion, we found that the MS mice in our study did not show behavioural indications on a repressed SHRP, which might be explained by the enriched environment the mice were kept in. The corticosterone plasma level was not measured.

In the data presented for the Barnes maze, there were no significant differences between the two treatment groups during the acquisition period. Latency to reach the target hole, total distance moved on the maze, and error frequency all decreased significantly over the course of the six acquisition trials and seven reversal trials, indicating that the animals were able to learn the task with time, which is in agreement with other studies (e.g. Pompl et al. 1999).

One consequence of hippocampal dysfunction is perseveration (Devenport et al. 1988). For the MS animals the group differences in the number of errors made, the goal parameter and the distance moved were therefore of interest. The differences found in the parameter “Close to Old Goal” the significantly higher frequency of “Errors” and visits to “Old Goal” by the MS 24 animals indicate that the MS animals were more perseverant which could point to a hippocampal lesion (Devenport et al. 1988; Bach et al. 1995). However, since perseveration is also a feature of prefrontal cortical dysfunction and there was no hippocampal dysfunction in the acquisition trials, we can only conclude that the behaviour in reversal trial may be related to hippocampus, but we cannot exclude that it is was caused by, e.g. a prefrontal cortical deficit.

One of the advantages of the Barnes maze is its ability to reveal the search strategies applied by the mice. The search strategies can be an indication of how well the cognitive abilities in the mice are due to the use of extra-maze cues (Barnes 1979; Holmes et al. 2002). In this study, an overall search pattern evolved with time. On initial trials the mice tended to use a random pattern and explore many incorrect holes, often returning to the centre after investigating the edge of the platform. With more experience the number of centre crossings decreased and a more systematic search was applied. Thus in later trials many mice went directly to the correct hole or one or two holes away before locating the hidden box. The mice learned the task better after a previous introduction to the platform and used the “Spatial” search strategy earlier. Furthermore, the SFR mice used the search strategy “Serial” significantly faster than the MS mice in the acquisition trials, but this was not true in the reversal trials. The serial search strategy requires the mouse to use the multiple relationships among extra-maze stimuli to find the escape tunnel (Barnes 1979; Bach et al. 1995). So apparently the MS mice needed more time to acquire to the spatial search strategy when learning the task, which could indicate a learning disability. However, since the MS mice did not show difficulties in using the spatial search strategy in the reverse trials, no firm conclusions can be made on this point.

Over 85% of granule cell neurogenesis are previously described to occur postnatally in the rodent with peak neurogenesis between PND 5 and 7 and total cell number increasing throughout the first year and continuing throughout life (Altman and Das 1965; Schlessinger et al. 1975; Bayer et al. 1982; Kuhn et al. 1996; Kempermann et al. 1998). Much research has focused on the neurogenesis in both the adult and newborn hippocampus of mammals (Kempermann et al. 1997; Gould et al. 1997; Eriksson et al. 1998; Tanapat et al. 1998, 2001; Gould et al. 1999; Lemaire et al. 2000; Malberg et al. 2000; Raber et al. 2004; Mirescu et al. 2004; Greisen et al. 2005) but only a few of these studies have used modified stereological methods (Kempermann et al. 1997; Eriksson et al. 1998; Gould et al. 1999; Lemaire et al. 2000; Malberg et al. 2000; Greisen et al. 2005). None of these previous studies have quantified the total cell numbers in the hippocampus using stereology in early trauma animal models or in humans. The model can apparently induce a neuron change in the hippocampus but whether the lower neuron number is a decrease due to a neuron loss or because neurogenesis has been affected in the peak period could not be determined in this study. Secondly, the impact of the neuron loss is not immediately linked to a possible memory deficit. Even though the MS animals had a reduced emotional responds in the elevated plus maze and showed perseverance in the Barnes maze, the cognitive deficits could just as well be due to a prefrontal damage and thus not a result of the decreased neuron numbers in DG.

We tested if a 24-h maternal separation on PND 9 can cause an adult phenotype characterized by altered levels of activity and anxiety, learning and memory dysfunction, deficits in behavioural flexibility (reversal deficits) as well as changes in number of neurons in the hippocampus and its subregions in the mouse brain. We found that a single 24 h maternal separation on PND 9 could elicit a reduced stress response in the elevated plus maze and induce perseveration behaviour in the Barnes maze. Further, a 20% reduction in total neuron numbers was found in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus.

Ethics

The experiment was carried out in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609/EEC) and the Danish legislation regulating animal experiments (Animal care and housing BEK nr 687 from 25/07/2003). The Danish Animal Experiments Inspectorate approved the protocols (Journal No. 2003/561–781).