Introduction

The recent recognition that birds are able to see in the near-ultraviolet (UV) part of the spectrum (~300–400 nm) has crucial implications for the interpretation of avian colours and transformed our understanding of avian sexual signalling (Bennett et al. 1997). Moreover, since some bird predators have poor UV sensitivity, the UV waveband has been considered a “private communication channel” for birds (Guilford and Harvey 1998; but see Hunt et al. 2001 and more recently Stevens and Cuthill 2007). Accordingly, UV reflecting plumage patches are found in most bird species (Eaton and Lanyon 2003) and UV reflectance is often associated with body parts used in sexual signalling (Hausmann et al. 2003). In the eastern bluebird (Sialia sialis) for example, higher UV reflectance may signal male competitive ability and more UV individuals are more successful at obtaining nestboxes (Siefferman and Hill 2005). Female choice may also select for exaggerated UV reflectance as shown in some blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus) populations (Sheldon et al. 1999; Limbourg et al. 2004), and producing highly UV reflecting feathers may only be achieved by birds in top condition (Jacot and Kempenaers 2007). Finally, differences in UV reflectance have been hypothesised to aid in reproductive isolation between closely related species (Bleiweiss 2004), and UV reflectance in chicks and eggs may increase their detectability to parents (Aviles et al. 2006; Hunt et al. 2003).

UV reflectance in bird plumage is caused by physical interference of the incident light with tissue microstructure (Prum 2006). It is also affected by pigment deposition (Shawkey and Hill 2005; Prum 2006), and by substances such as dirt and fat (Örnborg et al. 2002; Zampiga et al. 2004). UV reflectance is particularly sensitive to such external influences (Örnborg et al. 2002) and it has been suggested that birds may modify UV reflectance of their plumage by applying uropygial gland secretion (Piersma et al. 1999). Thus, birds may be able to rapidly, and perhaps also reversibly, adjust UV reflectance of the plumage.

The secretion produced by the uropygial gland consists of a complex mixture of waxes, (Jacob and Ziswiler 1982) and is actively applied onto the feathers during preening. Sometimes the secretion is visibly coloured, and its application changes the colour of the plumage thus acting as a “cosmetic” (for example in the great hornbill, Buceros bicornis Vevers 1964; Kemp 2001). The function of cosmetic colour changes is unknown although it has been hypothesised that they may enhance sexual attractiveness or reduce conspicuousness to predators (see reviews in Montgomerie 2006b; Delhey et al. 2007). Generally, the secretion is colourless to our eyes, but if it affects UV reflectance (either increasing or decreasing it) it could act as an avian “UV-cosmetic” (Piersma et al. 1999). This possibility was recently tested in the red knot (Calidris canutus), where the chemical composition of the uropygial secretion changes dramatically from mono-ester to di-ester waxes prior to breeding (Reneerkens et al. 2002; Reneerkens et al. 2006). Despite differences in the optical properties of both types of secretion (di-ester waxes absorbed more UV light in vitro), their application did not change plumage reflectance (Reneerkens and Korsten 2004). However, given that the rusty red breeding plumage of the red knot reflects very little in the UV, the potential UV-absorbing effects of the secretion cannot be expressed. Therefore, even if a UV cosmetic function of the uropygial secretion is unlikely in this species, this might not be the case in other birds with UV-reflective plumage or if in other species the secretion increases, rather than decreases, UV reflectance. Thus, the aim of this study is to assess inter-specific variability in the optical properties of the uropygial secretion in the UV and visible spectrum and to experimentally test how its application affects UV-reflective plumage.

Materials and methods

We sampled uropygial gland secretion of 181 individuals, belonging to 51 species from 12 orders. We sampled on average four individuals per species (range, 1–20; see Table 1). Samples were obtained from wild or captive live birds during spring and summer or from undated frozen specimens (Table 1). To assess whether freezing affected the optical properties of the secretion (effects on UV chroma and brightness, see below) we compared frozen and fresh secretions for the six species (Ciconia ciconia, Tetrao urogallus, Dendrocopos major, Turdus merula, Parus major and Sitta europaea) for which we had both frozen and fresh samples. Although frozen samples reduced UV chroma and brightness slightly more than fresh uropygial secretion (UV chroma, meanfresh = −1.1, SE = 0.32; meanfrozen = −1.5, SE = 0.46; Brightness, meanfresh = −21.4, SE = 2.3; meanfrozen = −23.9, SE = 2.1), this difference was not significant (Wilcoxon Matched-Pairs Signed Rank test, UV chroma, Z = −0.94, p = 0.35; brightness, Z = −0.52, p = 0.60). However, note that sample sizes are very small.

Table 1 List of all species included in this study, including sample size, source of the birds, and average values representing the change in brightness and UV chroma (ΔBrightness and ΔUV chroma) due to the uropygial secretion of each studied species

The uropygial gland opens to the exterior through a papilla which can be either feathered or naked, depending on the species (Jacob and Ziswiler 1982). From naked papillae, a small drop (1–2 mm in diameter) of secretion was gently expressed, picked up with a plastic spatula and smeared onto the measuring surface (see below). From feathered papillae, a few of the feathers, soaked in uropygial secretion, were collected and pressed on the measuring surface. In both cases the sampled secretion covered the measuring surface completely, and roughly similar amounts of secretion per unit of surface were applied in each case.

The effect of applying uropygial secretion on reflectance was measured using a Avaspec 2048 spectrometer connected to a deuterium-halogen light source (Avalight-DHS, Avantes, Eerbek, Netherlands) through a bifurcated fibre optics cable fitted at the end with a plastic cylinder to standardise measuring distance and shield out ambient light. The probe was held perpendicular to the surface of the feathers hence illumination and recording angles were both 90°. Reflectance was computed relative to Teflon™ tape or relative to a WS-2 white standard using the program Avasoft 6.2.1.

First, we tested the effect of the uropygial substance on the reflectance spectrum of white Teflon™ tape. Applying the uropygial secretion resulted in a small (3–4 mm diameter) but visible ‘greasy’ spot on the tape. We let the spot air dry before taking five reflectance measurements between 300 and 700 nm. The small size of the spot meant that the five reflectance readings were taken on the same spot (lifting the probe between measurements) and these five measurements were averaged to obtain one reflectance spectrum per individual. Reflectance spectra of the stains were computed relative to the reflectance of an adjacent spot of clean tape, which can be used as a white standard to calibrate reflectance measurements (Andersson and Prager 2006). From the average spectra we derived brightness, computed as the average reflectance between 300 and 700 nm (R300–700), and UV chroma, which is the percentage of reflectance in the near-UV range (R300–400/R300–700 ×100; Andersson et al. 1998; Montgomerie 2006a). We computed the effect of the secretion on the reflectance of the Teflon™ tape as: change in brightness = brightnessTeflon tape + secretion − brightnessTeflon tape, change in UV chroma = UV chromaTeflon tape + secretion − UV chromaTeflon tape. Per definition the average reflectance (brightness) and UV chroma of the white standard (clean Teflon™ tape) are 100% and 25% respectively.

Second, we assessed the effect of uropygial secretions on feather reflectance by comparing reflectance spectra before and after the application of the secretion. We sampled uropygial gland secretion and one feather type from one non-passerine (mallard, Anas platyrhynchos, the white tip of the feathers of the wing speculum, from five captive individuals) and one passerine (blue tit, C. (formerly Parus) caeruleus, UV/blue crown feathers, from four frozen specimens). This covered the two main types of secretion (see below). White feathers were chosen for their relatively uniform reflectance curve while blue tit crown feathers constitute a good example of a UV-rich sexually selected plumage patch (Andersson et al. 1998). We applied both types of secretion on different spots of both types of feathers. Reflectance of the same feather patch was measured five times before and five times after the application of uropygial secretion, using the same procedure as described above, but using a conventional white standard (WS-2, Avantes, Eerbek, Netherlands). We also quantified whether spectral differences would be visible to birds using a colour discrimination model (Vorobyev and Osorio 1998; Vorobyev et al. 1998). We used cone capture functions and visual parameters of UV-sensitive and violet-sensitive birds (from Endler and Mielke (2005)) to compute the chromatic distance (ΔS) in avian visual space between the average reflectance spectra of the feather before and after the application of the uropygial secretion. The unit for ΔS is the jnd (just noticeable difference) and values of ΔS > 1 can be discriminated by birds, whereas those below this threshold cannot. Note that ΔS only refers to chromatic contrast, and disregards differences in brightness or luminance (achromatic contrast). For more details on visual modelling see the Appendix.

Results

In general the application of uropygial secretion had little effect on the reflectance at medium and longer wavelengths but considerably reduced reflectance of shorter wavelengths (Fig. 1, Table 1, S1). Although intermediates exist, we identified two main types of secretions based on how they affected reflectance at shorter wavelengths. Secretions that caused a decrease in reflectance in the UV (Fig. 1a–c), and secretions that, besides the decrease in reflectance in the UV range, also caused a discrete dip in reflectance at around 400–410 nm (Fig. 1d–f). This second type was only found among passerines.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Inter-specific variability in the optical properties of uropygial gland secretion. Shown are the average effects of applying uropygial secretion of different species on the reflectance of white Teflon™ tape used as white standard. The dashed line at 100%, represents the reflectance of the intact Teflon™ tape The six species (ac non-passerines, df passerines) were chosen to illustrate the range of variation. Vertical bars represent ±1 SD. Average values (and SD’s) are calculated using the average values for each individual (based on the five repeated measurements, see “Materials and methods”). Thus, sample size equals the number of individuals. For a complete overview of sampled species and reflectance spectra see Table 1 and S1

Application of uropygial secretion reduced the brightness of the white surface (Fig. 1). This reduction was more marked among non-passerines (−22%, SE = 1.34, n = 19 species) than among passerines (−17%, SE = 0.99, n = 32 species; t 49 = −3.09, p = 0.003). Similarly, uropygial secretion reduced UV chroma of the Teflon™ tape more strongly in non-passerines (−1.41, SE = 0.3) than in passerines (−0.60, SE = 0.1; t 27.4 = −2.37, p = 0.027). The effect on UV chroma also seemed more variable among non-passerines than among passerines (95% confidence interval, non-passerines, −2.1 to −0.81, passerines, −0.97 to −0.33; values from Table 1; also compare the reflectance spectra in S1 of the electronic supplementary material).

Application of the two main secretion types (mallard and blue tit) significantly reduced brightness of white mallard feathers, but not of blue tit feathers (Fig. 2; Table 2). UV chroma of the feathers was reduced by both types of secretion, but the effect was statistically significant only for mallard secretion applied on white mallard feathers (Table 2). Although some of the differences in UV chroma were statistically significant differences in spectral shape were rather subtle (Fig. 2). Analyses based on models of avian colour vision revealed that most of the differences in colouration caused by the secretion were below or at the detection limit (Table 2). As expected, ΔS values were higher for highly UV-sensitive eyes (U-type eyes as found in rheas, gulls, parrots and Passerida; Endler and Mielke (2005)). However, even in these species, only the effects of the secretion on white feathers would be detectable (ΔS > 1; Table 2). The more noticeable effect of applying the secretion on white mallard feathers is possibly due to the latter’s higher absolute brightness. This implies that the magnitude of the effect depends on the type of secretion and feather. Note, however, that our sample sizes are small, reducing the power to detect small differences in reflectance.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Effects of uropygial gland secretion on feather reflectance. Depicted are the effects of mallard and blue tit secretion on white mallard feathers and on UV/blue crown feathers of blue tits. Thick lines represent intact feathers, thin lines represent feathers with secretion applied and dashed lines are the difference in reflectance between intact feathers and feathers with secretion. Vertical bars represent ±1 SE

Table 2 Effects of mallard and blue tit uropygial gland secretion on white mallard and UV/blue blue tit feathers

Discussion

Our survey of the optical properties of the uropygial gland secretion from 51 species suggests that the variability in uropygial secretion reflectance is largely restricted to UV and violet wavelengths (300–420 nm, Fig. 1). Interestingly, based on their optical properties, there seem to exist two types of secretion among the sampled birds. Secretion from non-passerine birds was characterised on average by moderate (but variable between species) reduction of overall reflectance and in particular UV reflectance. Passerine secretion on the other hand, showed less extreme effects but was characterised in most species by a discrete dip in reflectance around 400–410 nm (Fig. 1). Similar dips in this wavelength range have been found in reflectance spectra of structurally coloured bird integuments (Prum and Torres 2003), the ocular medium of bird eyes (Hart 2002) and urine of Australian marsupials (Kellie et al. 2004), but what causes these dips remains unclear.

The previous results suggest that uropygial secretion can only reduce, and not increase, feather UV reflectance and that it could possibly act as a UV cosmetic as proposed by Piersma et al. (1999). However, the application of both main types of secretion on feathers of mallards and blue tits revealed only subtle effects on feather reflectance that were barely discriminable by birds (Table 2, Fig. 2). Moreover, given that the amount of applied secretion probably exceeds the amount that birds normally preen onto their feathers, the changes in reflectance due to uropygial secretion reported here may be larger than those observed in nature.

We conclude that, at least among the sampled species, it is unlikely that secretions from the uropygial gland act as a UV cosmetic. However, note that seasonal and possibly also sexual variation in the optical properties of the uropygial secretion (e.g. Reneerkens et al. 2007) have not been specifically explored here and may need more attention before completely dismissing the UV-cosmetics hypothesis. Meanwhile, the lack of UV-cosmetic effects may simply reflect the fact that the optical properties of the uropygial secretions have been selected to interfere as little as possible with visual signalling through plumage colours. In favour of this hypothesis, most of the passerines included in this study have presumably highly UV-sensitive U-type eyes (except for the Jackdaw, Corvus monedula; Ödeen and Hastad 2003), whereas most of the non-passerines have violet-sensitive V-type eyes (except for the Lovebird, Agapornis sp.; Ödeen and Hastad 2003). It is therefore tempting to speculate that there is a link between UV sensitivity and uropygial secretion type. If UV signalling is more prevalent among birds with highly UV-sensitive eyes (Mullen and Pohland 2008), selection may favour uropygial secretions with increased UV transparency, as seen among passerines. However, this hypothesis must await rigorous testing through further sampling and phylogenetically controlled comparative analyses.