Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitive load and mental rotation: structuring orthographic projection for learning and problem solving

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive load theory was used to generate a series of three experiments to investigate the effects of various worked example formats on learning orthographic projection. Experiments 1 and 2 investigated the benefits of presenting problems, conventional worked examples incorporating the final 2-D and 3-D representations only, and modified worked examples with several intermediate stages of rotation between the 2-D and 3-D representations. Modified worked examples proved superior to conventional worked examples without intermediate stages while conventional worked examples were, in turn, superior to problems. Experiment 3 investigated the consequences of varying the number and location of intermediate stages in the rotation trajectory and found three stages to be superior to one. A single intermediate stage was superior when nearer the 2-D than the 3-D end of the trajectory. It was concluded that (a) orthographic projection is learned best using worked examples with several intermediate stages and that (b) a linear relation between angle of rotation and problem difficulty did not hold for orthographic projection material. Cognitive load theory could be used to suggest the ideal location of the intermediate stages.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bethel-Fox, C.E. & Shepard, R.N. (1988). Metal rotation: Effects of stimulus complexity and familiarity.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14: 12–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, P.A. & Just, M.A. (1986). Spatial ability: An information processing approach to psychometrics, in R.J. Sternberg, ed.,Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence, Vol. 3 (pp. 221–253). New Jersey: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., Glaser, R. & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving, in R.J. Sternberg, ed.,Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence, Vol. 1 (pp. 7–75). New Jersey: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G.A. & Sweller, J. (1987). The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer.Journal of Educational Psychology 79: 347–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L.A. (1988). The role of spatial representations in complex problem solving, in S. Schiffer & S. Steele, eds.,Cognition and Representation (pp. 53–86). Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L.A. & Shepard, R.N. (1982). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images, in R.N. Shepard & L.A. Cooper, eds.,Mental Images and Their Transformation. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. (1993).Children's Understanding: Development of Mental Models. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. (1985). Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability.Psychological Review 92: 137–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon, D. & Simon, H. (1980). Models of competence in solving physics problems.Cognitive Science 4: 317–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzler, J. & Shepard, R.N. (1982). Transformational studies of the internal representation of three-dimensional objects, in R.N. Shepard & L.A. Cooper, eds.,Mental Images and Their Transformation. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D.R. & Bialystok, E. (1983).Spatial Cognition: The Structure and Development of Mental Representations of Spatial Relations (pp. 145–181 & 233–259). New Jersey: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I., Wheeler, D. & Tudor, L. (1989). Can we imagine how objects look from other viewpoints?Cognitive Psychology 21: 185–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R.N. & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects.Science 171: 701–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1983). Control mechanisms in problem solving.Memory and Cognition 11, 32–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning.Cognitive Science 12: 257–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. & Cooper, G.A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra.Cognition and Instruction 2: 59–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Chandler P., Tierney, P. & Cooper, M. (1990). Cognitive load as a factor in the structuring of technical material.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 119: 176–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarmizi, R. & Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematical problem solving.Journal of Educational Psychology 80: 424–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, M. & Sweller, J. (1990). Structuring effective worked examples.Cognitive and Instruction 7: 1–39.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Pillay, H.K. Cognitive load and mental rotation: structuring orthographic projection for learning and problem solving. Instr Sci 22, 91–113 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00892159

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00892159

Keywords

Navigation