Scolaris Content Display Scolaris Content Display

Pericyazine for schizophrenia

Collapse all Expand all

Abstract

Background

Pericyazine is a 3‐cyano‐10 (3‐4'‐hydroxypiperidinopropyl) phenothiazine. It is overall pharmacologically similar with chlorpromazine, though particularly sedating. Dopamine receptor subtype analysis has not been performed for pericyazine, but the drug appears to induce greater noradrenergic than dopaminergic blockade. Compared to chlorpromazine, pericyazine reportedly has more potent antiemetic, antiserotonin, and anticholinergic activity.

Objectives

To evaluate the clinical effects and safety of pericyazine in comparison with placebo, typical and atypical antipsychotic agents and standard care for people with schizophrenia.

Search methods

We searched the Cochrane Schizophrenia Group Trials Register (February 2013) which is based on regular searches of CINAHL, EMBASE, MEDLINE and PsycINFO. We inspected references of all identified studies for further trials.

Selection criteria

All relevant randomised controlled trials focusing on pericyazine for schizophrenia and other types of schizophrenia‐like psychoses (schizophreniform and schizoaffective disorders). We excluded quasi‐randomised trials.

Data collection and analysis

Data were extracted independently from included papers by at least two review authors. Risk ratios (RR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) of homogeneous dichotomous data were calculated. We assessed risk of bias for included studies and used GRADE to judge quality of evidence.

Main results

We could only include five studies conducted between 1965 and 1980. Most of the included studies did not report details of randomisation, allocation concealment, details of blinding and we could not assess the impact of attrition due to poor reporting.

For the primary outcome of Global state ‐ not improved, the confidence interval was compatible with a small benefit and increased risk of not improving with pericyazine compared with typical antipsychotics (2 RCTs, n = 122, RR 1.24 CI 0.93 to 1.66, very low quality of evidence) or atypical antipsychotics (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.97 CI 0.67 to 1.42, very low quality of evidence).

When compared with typical antipsychotics relapse was only experienced by one person taking pericyazine (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 2.59 CI 0.11 to 61.75, very low quality of evidence).

Pericyazine was associated with more extrapyramidal side effects than typical antipsychotics (3 RCTs, n = 163, RR 0.52 CI 0.34 to 0.80, very low quality of evidence) and atypical antipsychotics (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 2.69 CI 1.35 to 5.36, very low quality of evidence).

The estimated risk of leaving the study early for specific reasons was imprecise for the comparisons of pericyazine with typical antipsychotics (2 RCTs, n = 71, RR 0.46 CI 0.11 to 1.90, very low quality of evidence), and with atypical antipsychotics (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.13 CI 0.01 to 2.42, very low quality of evidence).

Authors' conclusions

On the basis of very low quality evidence we are unable to determine the effects of pericyazine in comparison with typical or atypical antipsychotics for the treatment of schizophrenia. However, there is some evidence that pericyazine may be associated with a higher incidence of extrapyramidal side effects than other antipsychotics, and again this was judged to be very low quality evidence. Large, robust studies are still needed before any firm conclusions can be drawn.

PICOs

Population
Intervention
Comparison
Outcome

The PICO model is widely used and taught in evidence-based health care as a strategy for formulating questions and search strategies and for characterizing clinical studies or meta-analyses. PICO stands for four different potential components of a clinical question: Patient, Population or Problem; Intervention; Comparison; Outcome.

See more on using PICO in the Cochrane Handbook.

Plain language summary

Pericyazine for the treatment of schizophrenia

People with schizophrenia often experience symptoms such as hearing voices or seeing things (hallucinations) and have strange beliefs (delusions). The first line and mainstay of treatment for the symptoms of schizophrenia is antipsychotic drugs. These antipsychotic drugs can be grouped into older drugs (typical or first generation) and newer drugs (atypical or second generation). Pericyazine is a relatively old antipsychotic formulated in 1961. Some studies have suggested that it may be of more benefit than other antipsychotic drugs.

This review aims to assess the effectiveness of pericyazine in the treatment of schizophrenia compared to older and newer antipsychotics.A search for studies was carried out in 2013 and five studies conducted between 1965 and 1980 were found and included in the review. The quality of evidence was rated by the authors to be very low, and their results were imprecise for many outcomes where they compared pericyazine and other older and newer antipsychotic drugs. The evidence is inadequate to determine whether pericyazine is better than other antipsychotics.

The results of the analysis for the outcome of improvement were imprecise and the authors could not be certain that more people who took pericyazine were found to have not improved compared with those who took typical antipsychotics. More side effects, such as involuntary shaking, tremors, restlessness and spasms, were experienced by people who took pericyazine than other typical or atypical antipsychotics. These side effects are very unpleasant and the increased occurrence of them compared to other antipsychotics is an important finding considering pericyazine may not have additional benefits for the symptoms of schizophrenia.

No studies reported outcomes on satisfaction of treatment or cost effectiveness, which require attention. This lack of evidence leaves people with schizophrenia, mental health professionals and policy makers with little information on the benefits, hazards or problems of pericyazine. Outcomes on the cost of care and satisfaction with treatment should be included in future trials which should also be larger, better conducted, and fully reported.

(This plain language summary has been written by Ben Gray from Rethink Mental Illness).

Authors' conclusions

Implications for practice

1. For people with schizophrenia

This review has identified evidence of very low quality. We are unable to determine whether pericyazine is superior to other antipsychotics. The estimated increase in the number of people who took pericyazine who did not improve compared with those who took typical antipsychotics was imprecise. More extrapyramidal symptoms were experienced by people who took pericyazine than typical or atypical antipsychotics. These symptoms are unpleasant and the increased occurrence of them compared to other antipsychotics featured in this review is an important finding considering pericyazine may not have additional benefits for the symptoms of schizophrenia. No studies reported outcomes on satisfaction of treatment, so studies should be conducted including this outcome to asses whether people feel the benefit of pericyazine outweighs the likelihood of side effects that may be experienced.

2. For clinicians

There is very low quality evidence to inform the decision to use pericyazine or other antipsychotics in the treatment of schizophrenia. Clinicians will still have to make pragmatic decisions until further trials involving pericyazine are conducted. Clinicians should be aware of the likelihood of higher incidence of side effects, especially extrapyramidal side effects when prescribing pericyazine.

3. For managers/policy makers

There are no data on service outcomes or cost effectiveness for the studies included in this review. This lack of evidence leaves policy makers with difficult decisions to make. Cost effectiveness studies should be conducted to assess the usefulness of pericyazine compared with other antipsychotics in clinical settings.

Implications for research

1. General

Allocation concealment is essential for the results of a trial to be considered valid and gives the assurance that the selection bias is kept to the minimum. All studies included in this review were conducted many years before the CONSORT statement (Moher 2001) was proposed, so it is not surprising that a lot of information used to grade the quality of studies were missing. Six out of seven studies excluded from this review were excluded due to no useable data being reported. Had these studies used standardised outcomes, preferably validated binary outcomes as they are easier to interpret, and reported results including means and standard deviations in full, it would have facilitated the possibility of more robust conclusions being drawn. Missing data and poor reporting resulted in the studies included in this review being judged to be of low quality, which in turn makes it difficult to interpret and apply the findings.

2. Specific

We feel that the question as to whether pericyazine is a better alternative to typical and atypical antipsychotics for schizophrenia is unanswered. We advocate future trials and these should not only be adequately powered and of long duration, but should also be pragmatic in order to increase applicability. Methods should involve adequate concealment of allocation and follow‐up. The outcomes to be measured should be predefined and should be adhered to. If scales are to be used, then validated and clinically meaningful outcomes should also be predefined. Outcomes on the cost of care and satisfaction of treatment should also be included in future studies (see Table 1 for suggested design for future study).

Open in table viewer
Table 1. Suggested design for future study

Methods

Allocation: randomised, describing method of randomisation and allocation concealment.
Blinding: double blind.
Duration: 12 months.

Participants

Diagnosis: schizophrenia.
n: 300.
Sex: male and female.
Age: > 18 years old.

Intervention

1. Pericyazine: any dose and mode or pattern of administration.

2. Other antipsychotic: any dose and mode or pattern of administration.

Outcomes

Global state.

Leaving the study early.
Mental state.
General functioning.
Quality of life.
Adverse effects.
Economic outcomes.

Notes

Any outcomes measured using scale‐derived data should be interpreted in such a way as to make clear the real‐life relevance of changes in scale score.

Summary of findings

Open in table viewer
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Pericyazine versus Typical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Pericyazine compared with typical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Patient or population: patients with schizophrenia
Settings: Inpatient
Intervention: Pericyazine
Comparison: Typical antipsychotic

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Typical antipsychotic

Pericyazine

Global state ‐ not improved
Follow‐up: 6‐12 weeks

Low risk population1

RR 1.24
(0.93 to 1.66)

122
(2 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low2,3,4

200 per 1000

248 per 1000
(186 to 332)

Medium risk population1

500 per 1000

620 per 1000
(465 to 830)

High risk population1

800 per 1000

992 per 1000
(744 to 1000)

Global state ‐ relapse
Follow‐up: mean 3 months

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

80
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low5,6,7

Adverse events ‐ for specific reasons ‐ Extrapyramidal side effect
Follow‐up: 6‐12 weeks

Low risk population1

RR 0.59
(0.38 to 0.89)

163
(3 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low2,3,4

200 per 1000

118 per 1000
(76 to 178)

Medium risk population1

400 per 1000

236 per 1000
(152 to 356)

High risk population1

500 per 1000

295 per 1000
(190 to 445)

Mental state

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Leaving the study early for specific reasons
Follow‐up: 9‐12 weeks

Low risk population1

RR 0.46
(0.11 to 1.9)

71
(2 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low7,8,9

100 per 1000

46 per 1000
(11 to 190)

Medium risk population1

150 per 1000

69 per 1000
(16 to 285)

High risk population1

200 per 1000

92 per 1000
(22 to 380)

Behaviour

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Economic outcomes

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported useable data for this outcome.

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Moderate risk approximately equates to that of the control group in the studies.
2 Risk of bias: 'very serious' ‐ randomisation and allocation concealment was generally unclear in all trials. Blinding was achieved in two of the trials, but were untested. Seven people left the Leitch 1965 study early, but were unaccounted for in the final analysis.
3 Imprecision: 'very serious' ‐ both sample size and effect size are small. Meta‐anlaysis result was not significant.
4 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ no explanation was provided.
5 Risk of bias: 'serious' ‐ allocation concealment was unclear; drop out was not included in final analysis.
6 Imprecision: 'serious' ‐ only one trial contributed data to this outcome.
7 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ only one trial was identified.
8 Risk of bias: 'serious' ‐ randomisation and allocation concealment were not described. Blinding of participants was untested. Loss to follow up were unaccounted for in final analysis.
9 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ only a limited small number of trials were identified. Most of the trials either favoured (or has a tendency to favour) the investigated intervention.

Open in table viewer
Summary of findings 2. Pericyazine versus Atypical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Pericyazine compared with atypical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Patient or population: patients with schizophrenia
Settings: Inpatient
Intervention: Pericyazine
Comparison: Atypical antipsychotic

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Atypical antipsychotic

Pericyazine

Global state ‐ not improved
Follow‐up: mean 6 weeks

Low

RR 0.97
(0.67 to 1.42)

93
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2,3

450 per 1000

437 per 1000
(302 to 639)

Moderate

550 per 1000

534 per 1000
(369 to 781)

High

650 per 1000

631 per 1000
(436 to 923)

Adverse Effects ‐ Extrapyramidal symptoms
Follow‐up: mean 6 weeks

Low

RR 2.69
(1.35 to 5.36)

93
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2,3

100 per 1000

269 per 1000
(135 to 536)

Moderate

200 per 1000

538 per 1000
(270 to 1000)

High

300 per 1000

807 per 1000
(405 to 1000)

Mental state

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Leaving the study early for specific reasons
Follow‐up: 9‐12 weeks

Low4

RR 0.46
(0.11 to 1.9)

93
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low3,5,6

50 per 1000

23 per 1000
(5 to 95)

Moderate4

100 per 1000

46 per 1000
(11 to 190)

High4

150 per 1000

69 per 1000
(16 to 285)

Behaviour

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Economic outcomes

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Limitations of design: 'serious' ‐ lack of blinding, randomisation unclear.
2 Imprecision: 'serious' ‐ only one trial was identified and the effect size of which is small with wide confidence intervals.
3 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ only one trial was identified.
4 Moderate risk approximately equates to that of the control group in the studies.
5 Risk of bias: 'serious' ‐ randomisation and allocation concealment were not described. Blinding of participants was untested. Loss to follow up were unaccounted for in final analysis.
6 Imprecision: 'serious' ‐ only one trial was identified.

Background

Description of the condition

Schizophrenia is a chronic, severe and disabling illness which affects approximately 1% of the population. It is a worldwide illness that crosses all cultures and socioeconomic groups (Fortinash 2000). The severe and long‐lasting symptoms of schizophrenia cause considerable disability.

Description of the intervention

Pericyazine, also known as periciazine, is a relatively old antipsychotic. It was formulated in 1961 at the same laboratories where chlorpromazine was developed (Rhone‐Poulenc Laboratories, Rasch 1966). Pericyazine has been marketed as Neulactil in Europe and Australia while in the US it is under the brand name Neuleptil.

The first reports concerning the clinical application of pericyazine were from France and these showed that pericyazine had a beneficial effect in 'character' disorders (Chanoit 1962; Chanoit 1964; Deshaies 1962). However, treatment of people with schizophrenia was attempted later (Astrup 1965) and this was followed by trials and gave the impression that pericyazine had similar effects as other antipsychotic drugs of the day. Most investigators seemed to find that pericyazine was most valuable in case of restlessness, aggression, destructive tendencies, and severe hallucinations, and that the effect on paranoid delusions seemed to be less pronounced.

Different investigators reached somewhat varying conclusions as to the effect and the adequate dosage of pericyazine (Ban 1965; Bartlet 1965; Boardman 1965; Grant 1965 a). In 1965 it was thought that pericyazine was "particularly superior in the treatment of aggressive chronic schizophrenics" and that it seemed to be of equal value to other antipsychotics of the time (Bartlet 1965). Leitch 1965, on the other hand, concluded that the adverse effects were fewer and less pronounced when using pericyazine in comparison with other phenothiazines, such as chlorpromazine. Additionally, pericyazine was thought to be of value in long‐term therapy and was well‐accepted by patients. In particular, the somnolence was thought to be less pronounced than with other drugs of its class (Rasch 1966). More latterly, pericyazine has been found to be more effective in treating aggression and hostility in comparison with chlorpromazine (Becker 1981).

How the intervention might work

Pericyazine is a 3‐cyano‐10 (3‐4'‐hydroxypiperidinopropyl) phenothiazine (Figure 1). It is overall, pharmacologically similar to chlorpromazine, though more sedating (Anonymous 1967). Dopamine receptor subtype analysis has not been performed for pericyazine, but the drug appears to induce greater noradrenergic than dopaminergic blockade (Nishikawa 1989). Compared to chlorpromazine, pericyazine reportedly has more potent antiemetic, antiserotonin, and anticholinergic activity (Jenner 1970; Rodgers 1996). More potent cataleptic activity compared to prochlorperazine has been demonstrated (Rodgers 1996). There is little information about plasma concentrations, distribution and excretion in humans. Finally, the initial dose for pericyazine for schizophrenia is 75 mg/day, increased slowly if necessary to a maximum of 300 mg/day. For the short‐term treatment of severe anxiety, agitation, and violent or dangerously impulsive behaviour the dose is initially 15 to 30 mg/day adjusted according to response (BNF 2008).


10‐[3‐(4‐hydroxypiperidino)propyl] phenothiazine‐2‐carbonitrile)]

10‐[3‐(4‐hydroxypiperidino)propyl] phenothiazine‐2‐carbonitrile)]

Why it is important to do this review

Although we have not found precise data on how much pericyazine is being used for treating schizophrenia, it seems to be especially popular in the Nordic countries of Europe, as well as Russia and Japan (Nishikawa 1989). Currently, the literature supports its use as chief or adjuvant therapy for anxiety in the elderly, irritability and tension in late luteal phase dysphoria, obsessive‐compulsive disorder, and temporal lobe epilepsy (BNF 2008). Despite the lack of modern scientific data concerning the effects of this antipsychotic, pericyazine seems surprisingly popular with patient‐targeted information online resources.

Objectives

To evaluate the clinical effects and safety of pericyazine in comparison with placebo, typical and atypical antipsychotic agents and standard care for people with schizophrenia.

Methods

Criteria for considering studies for this review

Types of studies

We included all relevant randomised controlled trials (RCTs). If a trial was described as 'double‐blind' but it was implied that the study was randomised and the participants' demographic details in each group were similar, it was included. Quasi‐randomised studies, in which treatment allocation was not concealed, such as those allocating by using alternate days of the week or toss of a coin, were excluded. This is because prior knowledge of treatment allocation may lead to biased patient allocation (Schulz 1995). Randomised cross‐over studies were eligible but only data up to the point of first cross‐over because of the instability of the problem behaviours and the likely carryover effects of all treatments.

Types of participants

We included people with schizophrenia and other types of schizophrenia‐like psychoses (schizophreniform and schizoaffective disorders) however diagnosed, irrespective of age, sex ethnicity or severity of illness. There is no clear evidence that the schizophrenia‐like psychoses are caused by fundamentally different disease processes or require different treatment approaches (Carpenter 1994). If possible, we planned to exclude people with dementing illnesses, depression and primary problems associated with substance misuse.

Types of interventions

  1. Pericyazine: any dose and mode or pattern of administration

  2. Placebo or no intervention

  3. Typical antipsychotic drugs: any dose and mode or pattern of administration ‐ examples of such drugs are chlorpromazine and haloperidol

  4. Atypical antipsychotic drugs: any dose and mode or pattern of administration ‐ examples of such drugs are clozapine and olanzapine

  5. 'Standard care' as defined by treating physician

Types of outcome measures

Where possible, outcomes were made dichotomous by dividing them into two categories ‐ 'clinically significant change' and 'no clinically significant change'. All outcomes were reported for the short term (up to six weeks), medium term (seven to 26 weeks) and long term (over 26 weeks).

Primary outcomes
1. Global state (medium term) ‐ not improved or worsened
2. Adverse effects ‐ specific (medium term) ‐ extrapyramidal symptoms (parkinsonian symptoms, dystonia, akathisia, and tardive dyskinesia)
Secondary outcomes
1. Global state

1.1 Relapse
1.2 Satisfaction with treatment ‐ participant/carer
1.3 Death (suicide or natural causes)

2. Mental state

2.1 General symptoms
2.2 Specific symptoms
2.2.1 Positive symptoms (delusions, hallucinations, disordered thinking)
2.2.2 Negative symptoms (avolition, poor self‐care, blunted affect)
2.2.3 Mood ‐ depression

3. Adverse effects

3.1 General
3.2 Specific
3.2.1 Others

4. Leaving the study early

4.1 For specific reasons
4.2 For general reasons

5. Behaviour

5.1 General behaviour
5.2 Specific behaviours (e.g. aggressive or violent behaviour)
5.2.1 Social functioning
5.2.2 Employment status during trial (employed/unemployed)
5.2.3 Occurrence of violent incidents (to self, others or property)

6. Economic

6.1 Average change in total cost of medical and mental health care
6.2 Total indirect and direct costs.
6.3 Direct resource use:
6.3.1 Outpatients ‐ number of contacts (GP consultation, psychiatrist, psychologists, psychiatric nurse, counsellor, social worker)
6.3.2 Hospitalisation (taking battery of tests, patients’ physical, psychiatric and psychological profile and psychological assessment, number of days, relapse)
6.3.3 Medication (different types of antipsychotics to include dose and frequency, treatment of side effects)
6.3.4 Psychological therapies (different types of psychological therapies to include session numbers and frequency)
6.3.5 Other resources (day centres, night shelter) and transportation for medical care visits
6.4 Indirect resource use:
6.4.1 Family, relative and friends resources
6.4.2 Police, criminal justice system
6.4.3 Benefits paid, social security payments
6.4.4 Employment agency workers, absence from work, loss of productivity
6.5 Cost‐effectiveness ratios represented by ICER (incremental cost effectiveness ratio)
6.6 Cost‐utilities represented by incremental costs per QALY (quality‐adjusted life‐years) or DALYs (disability‐adjusted life years)
6.7 Cost benefit represented by net Benefit Ratio, others.

7. 'Summary of findings' tables

We used the GRADE approach to interpret findings (Schünemann 2008) and used GRADE profiler (GRADEPRO) to import data from RevMan 5 (Review Manager) to create 'Summary of findings' tables. These tables provide outcome‐specific information concerning the overall quality of evidence from each included study in the comparison, the magnitude of effect of the interventions examined, and the sum of available data on all outcomes we rated as important to patient‐care and decision making. We selected the following main outcomes for inclusion in the 'Summary of findings' table.

  1. Global state

  2. Adverse effect

  3. Mental state

  4. Leaving the study early

  5. Behaviour

  6. Economic outcomes

However, the included studies provided no data towards mental state, behaviour or economic outcomes so they do not appear in the 'Summary of findings' tables. In future updates, if new included studies report data for these outcomes the 'Summary of findings' tables will be updated.

Search methods for identification of studies

Electronic searches

1. 1. The Cochrane Schizophrenia Group Trials Register

The CSzG Trials Register was searched (May 2008, February 2013) using the phrase:

[((*peric?azin* or *aolept* or *nemactil* or *neulactil* or *neulept?l* or *neuperil* or propericiazine or RP 8909 or SKF 20716) in title, abstract and index fields in REFERENCE) OR (*peric?azin* or propericiazine or RP 8909 or SKF 20716) in interventions field in STUDY]

This register is compiled by systematic searches of major databases, handsearches and conference proceedings (see Group Module).

Searching other resources

References of all identified relevant studies were inspected for more trials.

Data collection and analysis

We have updated the data collection and analysis section since publication of the protocol ‐ for previous text please see Appendix 1.

Selection of studies

Review authors HEM and MQA independently inspected citations from the searches and identified relevant abstracts. A random 20% sample were independently re‐inspected by SM to ensure reliability. Where disputes arose, the full report was acquired for more detailed scrutiny. Full reports of the abstracts meeting the review criteria were obtained and inspected by HEM and MQA. Again, a random 20% of reports were re‐inspected by SM in order to ensure reliable selection. Where it was not possible to resolve disagreement by discussion, we had planned to contact the authors of the studies for clarification, however, as the included studies were all. completed over 30 years ago, we did not attempt this.

Data extraction and management

1. Extraction

Review author HEM extracted data from all included studies. In addition, to ensure reliability, MQA independently extracted data from a random sample of these studies, comprising 10% of the total. Again, any disagreement was discussed and decisions documented. With remaining problems SM helped clarify issues and these final decisions were documented. Data presented only in graphs and figures were extracted whenever possible, but included only if two review authors independently had the same result. We had intended to contact authors through an open‐ended request in order to obtain missing information or for clarification whenever necessary but as all included studies were completed over 30 years ago, this was not attempted. If studies were multi‐centre, where possible, we extracted data relevant to each component centre separately.

2. Management
2.1 Forms

We extracted data onto standard, simple forms.

2.2 Scale‐derived data

We did not identify any continuous data to include in this review. For future updates, we will only include continuous data from rating scales if:

a) the psychometric properties of the measuring instrument had been described in a peer‐reviewed journal (Marshall 2000); and
b) the measuring instrument had not been written or modified by one of the trialists for that particular trial.

Ideally, the measuring instrument should either be i. a self‐report or ii. completed by an independent rater or relative (not the therapist). We realise that this is not often reported clearly, therefore, we will note if this is the case or not in the Description of studies.

2.3 Endpoint versus change data

There are advantages of both endpoint and change data. Change data can remove a component of between‐person variability from the analysis. On the other hand, calculation of change needs two assessments (baseline and endpoint) which can be difficult in unstable and difficult to measure conditions such as schizophrenia. We did not encounter such data in this review, but for future updates, we have decided primarily to use endpoint data, and only use change data if the former are not available. Endpoint and change data will be combined in the analysis using weighted mean differences (MD) throughout (Higgins 2011).

2.4 Skewed data

Even though this review did not identify any outcomes with skewed data, we are aware that continuous data on clinical and social outcomes are often not normally distributed. To avoid the pitfall of applying parametric tests to non‐parametric data, for future updates we aim to apply the following standards to all data before inclusion:

a) standard deviations (SDs) and means are reported in the paper or obtainable from the authors;
b) when a scale starts from the finite number zero, the SD, when multiplied by two, is less than the mean (as otherwise the mean is unlikely to be an appropriate measure of the centre of the distribution, (Altman 1996);
c) if a scale started from a positive value (such as the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), (Kay 1986)), which can have values from 30 to 210), the calculation described above will be modified to take the scale starting point into account. In these cases skew is present if 2 SD > (S‐S min), where S is the mean score and 'S min' is the minimum score.

Endpoint scores on scales often have a finite start and end point and these rules can be applied. Skewed data pose less of a problem when looking at means if the sample size is large (> 200), we will enter these into the syntheses and present skewed endpoint data from studies of less than 200 participants in 'Additional tables' rather than enter such data in analyses.

When continuous data are presented on a scale that includes a possibility of negative values (such as change data), it is difficult to tell whether data are skewed or not. We will present and enter change data into the analyses.

2.5 Common measure

To facilitate comparison between trials, we intended to convert variables that can be reported in different metrics, such as days in hospital (mean days per year, per week or per month) to a common metric (e.g. mean days per month). Although common measure was not an issue in this review, the above procedures will be used in future updates.

2.6 Conversion of continuous to binary

We did not identify any continuous data in this review, therefore, conversion from continuous to binary was not performed. If we had found such data, we would have made the effort to convert outcome measures to dichotomous data. This can be done by identifying cut‐off points on rating scales and dividing participants accordingly into 'clinically improved' or 'not clinically improved'. It is generally assumed that if there is a 50% reduction in a scale‐derived score such as the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS, Overall 1962) or the PANSS (Kay 1986), this could be considered as a clinically significant response (Leucht 2005; Leucht 2005a). If data based on these thresholds are not available, we will use the primary cut‐off presented by the original authors.

2.7 Direction of graphs

Where possible, we entered data in such a way that the area to the left of the line of no effect indicates a favourable outcome for pericyazine. Where keeping to this made it impossible to avoid outcome titles with clumsy double‐negatives (e.g. 'Not improved') we would have reported data where the left of the line indicates an unfavourable outcome. This would have been noted in the relevant graphs.

Assessment of risk of bias in included studies

Again, review authors HEM and MQA worked independently to assess risk of bias by using criteria described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systemic reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011) to assess trial quality. This set of criteria is based on evidence of associations between overestimate of effect and high risk of bias of the article such as sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding, incomplete outcome data and selective reporting.

Where the raters disagreed, the final rating was made by consensus, with the involvement of another member of the review group. Where inadequate details of randomisation and other characteristics of trials were provided, we had planned to contact authors of the studies in order to obtain further information but as all included studies were completed over 30 years ago, this was not attempted. There was no non‐concurrence in quality assessment, but if disputes had occurred as to which category a trial is to be allocated, we would have resolved these by discussion.

The level of risk of bias was noted in both the text of the review and in the 'Summary of findings' tables.

Measures of treatment effect

1. Binary data

For binary outcomes, we calculated a standard estimation of the risk ratio (RR) and its 95% confidence interval (CI). It has been shown that RR is more intuitive (Boissel 1999) than odds ratios and that odds ratios tend to be interpreted as RR by clinicians (Deeks 2000). The Number Needed to Treat/Harm (NNT/H) statistic with its confidence intervals is intuitively attractive to clinicians but is problematic both in its accurate calculation in meta‐analyses and interpretation (Hutton 2009). For binary data presented in the 'Summary of findings' tables, where possible, we calculated illustrative comparative risks.

2. Continuous data

We were unable to extract any continuous data for this review. If we had found such data, we would have estimated mean difference (MD) between groups. We prefer not to calculate effect size measures (standardised mean difference SMD). In future updates, if scales of very considerable similarity are used, we will presume there is a small difference in measurement, and we will calculate effect size and transform the effect back to the units of one or more of the specific instruments.

Unit of analysis issues

1. Cluster trials

Studies increasingly employ 'cluster randomisation' (such as randomisation by clinician or practice) but analysis and pooling of clustered data poses problems. Firstly, authors often fail to account for intra‐class correlation in clustered studies, leading to a 'unit of analysis' error (Divine 1992) whereby P values are spuriously low, confidence intervals unduly narrow and statistical significance overestimated. This causes type I errors (Bland 1997; Gulliford 1999).

Our search did not reveal any cluster‐randomised trials, otherwise, for these kind of data, where clustering is not accounted for in primary studies, we would have presented data in a table, with a (*) symbol to indicate the presence of a probable unit of analysis error. If we find cluster‐randomised trial data in subsequent versions of this review, we will seek to contact first authors of studies to obtain intra‐class correlation coefficients (ICCs) for their clustered data and to adjust for this by using accepted methods (Gulliford 1999). If clustering has been incorporated into the analysis of primary studies, we will present these data as if from a non‐cluster randomised study, but adjust for the clustering effect.

We sought statistical advice during the protocol state of this review and have been advised that the binary data as presented in a report should be divided by a 'design effect'. This is calculated using the mean number of participants per cluster (m) and the ICC [Design effect = 1+(m‐1)*ICC] (Donner 2002). If the ICC is not reported, it will be assumed to be 0.1 (Ukoumunne 1999).

If cluster studies have been appropriately analysed taking into account ICCs and relevant data documented in the report, synthesis with other studies will be possible using the generic inverse variance technique.

2. Cross‐over trials

A major concern of cross‐over trials is the carry‐over effect. It occurs if an effect (e.g. pharmacological, physiological or psychological) of the treatment in the first phase is carried over to the second phase. As a consequence, on entry to the second phase the participants can differ systematically from their initial state despite a wash‐out phase. For the same reason, cross‐over trials are not appropriate if the condition of interest is unstable (Elbourne 2002). Both effects are very likely in severe mental illness. We did not identify any studies of this type but only planned to use data from the first phase of cross‐over trials.

3. Studies with multiple treatment groups

All of the included studies in this review had parallel treatment arms. In future updates, where a study involves more than two treatment arms, if relevant, the additional treatment arms will be presented in comparisons. If data are binary they will simply be added and combined within the two‐by‐two table. If data are continuous we will combine data following the formula in section 7.7.3.8  (Combining groups) of the Cochrane Handbook for Systemic reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). Where the additional treatment arms are not relevant, we will not use these data.

Dealing with missing data

1. Overall loss of credibility

At some degree of loss of follow‐up, data must lose credibility (Xia 2009). Most of the studies included in this review either had no missing data or the missing data were of an acceptable proportion of the total (< 50%). For future updates, we choose that, for any particular outcome, should more than 50% of data be unaccounted for, we will not reproduce these data or use them within the analyses. If, however, more than 50% of those in one arm of a study are lost, but the total loss is less than 50%, this will be addressed within the 'Summary of findings' tables by down‐rating quality. Finally, we will also downgrade quality within the 'Summary of findings' tables should loss be 25% to 50% in total.

2. Binary

In the case where attrition for a binary outcome is between 0% and 50% and where these data are not clearly described, we presented data on a 'once‐randomised‐always‐analyse' basis (an intention‐to‐treat analysis). Those leaving the study early are all assumed to have the same rates of negative outcome as those who completed, with the exception of the outcome of death and adverse effects. For these outcomes, the rate of those who stay in the study ‐ in that particular arm of the trial ‐ will be used for those who did not. We planned to undertake a sensitivity analysis to test how prone the primary outcomes were to change when data only from people who completed the study to that point are compared to the intention‐to treat analysis using the above assumptions, however, we did not identify studies with the appropriate data to enable us to do this.

3. Continuous
3.1 Attrition

In the case where attrition for a continuous outcome is between 0% and 50%, and data only from people who complete the study to that point are reported, we would have reproduced these. However, we did not have any continuous data in this review.

3.2 Standard deviations

For future updates of this review, if standard deviations (SDs) are not reported, we will first try to obtain the missing values from the authors. If not available, where there are missing measures of variance for continuous data, but an exact standard error (SE) and confidence intervals (CIs) available for group means, and either a 'P' value or 't' value available for differences in mean, we can calculate them according to the rules described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systemic reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011): When only the SE is reported, SDs are calculated by the formula SD = SE * square root (n). Chapters 7.7.3 and 16.1.3 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systemic reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011) present detailed formulae for estimating SDs from P values, t or F values, CIs, ranges or other statistics. If these formulae do not apply, we will calculate the SDs according to a validated imputation method, which is based on the SDs of the other included studies (Furukawa 2006). Although some of these imputation strategies can introduce error, the alternative would be to exclude a given study's outcome and thus to lose information. We nevertheless will examine the validity of the imputations in a sensitivity analysis excluding imputed values.

Assessment of heterogeneity

1. Clinical heterogeneity

We considered all included studies initially, without seeing comparison data, to judge clinical heterogeneity. We simply inspected all studies for clearly outlying people or situations which we had not predicted would arise. We did not find any clinical heterogeneity amongst the included studies.

2. Methodological heterogeneity

We considered all included studies initially, without seeing comparison data, to judge methodological heterogeneity. We simply inspected all studies for clearly outlying methods which we had not predicted would arise. We did not identify any methodological outliers in the included studies.

3. Statistical heterogeneity
3.1 Visual inspection

We visually inspected graphs to investigate the possibility of statistical heterogeneity.

3.2 Employing the I2 statistic

Heterogeneity between studies was investigated by considering the I2 method alongside the Chi2 'P' value. The I2 provides an estimate of the percentage of inconsistency thought to be due to chance (Higgins 2003). The importance of the observed value of I2 depends on i. magnitude and direction of effects and ii. strength of evidence for heterogeneity (e.g. 'P' value from Chi2  test, or a confidence interval for I2). An I2 estimate greater than or equal to around 50% accompanied by a statistically significant Chi2 statistic, will be interpreted as evidence of substantial levels of heterogeneity (Section 9.5.2 ‐ Higgins 2011). We did not find any heterogeneity in the analysis of this review, but for future updates, when substantial levels of heterogeneity are found in the primary outcome, we will explore reasons for heterogeneity (Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity).

Assessment of reporting biases

1. Protocol versus full study

Reporting biases arise when the dissemination of research findings is influenced by the nature and direction of results. These are described in section 10.1 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systemic reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). We tried to locate protocols of included randomised trials. If the protocol was available, we planned to compare outcomes in the protocol and in the published report. However, the studies included in this particular review were conducted before the initiation of clinical trial registration, therefore, the protocols were not available. Thus, we compared outcomes listed in the methods section of the trial report with actually reported results.

2. Funnel plot

Reporting biases arise when the dissemination of research findings is influenced by the nature and direction of results (Egger 1997). These are again described in Section 10 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systemic reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). We are aware that funnel plots may be useful in investigating reporting biases but are of limited power to detect small‐study effects. We did not use funnel plots for outcomes as there are less than 10 included studies. In future updates, where funnel plots are possible, we will seek statistical advice in their interpretation.

Data synthesis

We understand that there is no closed argument for preference for use of fixed‐effect or random‐effects models. The random‐effects method incorporates an assumption that the different studies are estimating different, yet related, intervention effects. This often seems to be true to us and the random‐effects model takes into account differences between studies, even if there is no statistically significant heterogeneity. There is, however, a disadvantage to the random‐effects model. It puts added weight onto small studies, which often are the most biased ones. Depending on the direction of effect, these studies can either inflate or deflate the effect size. We used a fixed‐effect model for all analyses. The reader is, however, able to choose to inspect the data using the random‐effects model.

Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

1. Subgroup analyses
1.1 Primary outcomes

We had planned to conduct a subgroup analysis for the primary outcomes for pericyazine compared with placebo, however, we did not identify any studies that compared pericyazine with placebo so this analysis will be carried out if relevant studies are identified in the future.

1.2 Clinical state, stage or problem

In future updates if further studies become available, we will investigate whether pericyazine is differentially effective compared with placebo for the following subgroups.

  • Men and women

  • People who are under 18 years of age, between 18 and 64, or over 65 years of age

  • People who became ill recently (i.e. acute episode approximately less than one month's duration) as opposed to people who have been ill for a longer period

  • People who are given low doses (75‐ 200 mg/day), and those given high doses (over 200 mg/day)

  • People who have schizophrenia diagnosed according to any operational criterion i.e. a pre‐stated checklist of symptoms/problems/time periods/exclusions) as opposed to those who have entered the trial with loosely defined illness

  • People treated earlier (pre‐1990) and people treated in recent years (1990 to 2006)

2. Investigation of heterogeneity

We did not find any unacceptable level of inconsistency when combining data for this review. However, for future updates, if inconsistency is high, this will be reported. First, we will investigate whether data have been entered correctly. Second, if data are correct, we will visually inspect the graph and remove outlying studies to see if homogeneity is restored. Should this occur with data contributing to the summary finding of no more than around 10% of the total weighting, data will be presented. If not, data will not be pooled and issues will be discussed. We know of no supporting research for this 10% cut‐off but are investigating use of prediction intervals as an alternative to this unsatisfactory state.

When unanticipated clinical or methodological heterogeneity are obvious, we will simply state hypotheses regarding these for future reviews or versions of this review. We do not anticipate undertaking analyses relating to these.

Sensitivity analysis

1. Implication of randomisation

We did not identify any trials with implied randomisation in this review. For future updates, we aim to include trials in a sensitivity analysis if they are described in some way as to imply randomisation. For the primary outcomes, we will include these studies and if there is no substantive difference when the implied randomised studies are added to those with better description of randomisation, then all data will be employed from these studies.

2. Assumptions for lost binary data

We did not have to make assumptions with people lost to follow‐up in this review, but for future updates, where assumptions have to be made regarding people lost to follow‐up (see Dealing with missing data), we will compare the findings of the primary outcomes when we use our assumptions and when we use data only from people who complete the study to that point. If there is a substantial difference, we will report results and discuss them but will continue to employ our assumption.

For future updates, where assumptions have to be made regarding missing SDs data (see Dealing with missing data), we will compare the findings of the primary outcomes when we use our assumptions and when we use data only from people who complete the study to that point. A sensitivity analysis will be undertaken to test how prone results are to change when completer‐only data only are compared with the imputed data using the above assumption. If there is a substantial difference, we will report results and discuss them, but will continue to employ our assumption.

3. Risk of bias

We had planned to analyse the effects of excluding trials that are judged to be at high risk of bias across one or more of the domains of randomisation (implied as randomised with no further details available), allocation concealment, blinding and outcome reporting for the meta‐analysis of the primary outcome. However, we were unable to perform this comparison due to the fact that only limited data were available for primary outcomes. For instance, there were only two studies contributed data on global state, both had high risk rating on one of the above domains. In future updates, if there are more data available, we hope to carry out the sensitivity analysis. Data from trials with high risk of bias ratings will only be included in the analysis, if the exclusion of trials at high risk of bias does not substantially alter the direction of effect or the precision of the effect estimates.

4. Imputed values

We also planned to undertake a sensitivity analysis to assess the effects of including data from trials where we used imputed values for ICC in calculating the design effect in cluster‐randomised trials, however, we did not impute any data in this version of the review.

Should this become the case in future updates, if substantial differences are noted in the direction or precision of effect estimates in any of the sensitivity analyses listed above, we will not pool data from the excluded trials with the other trials contributing to the outcome, but present them separately.

5. Fixed and random effects

All data were synthesised using a fixed‐effect model, however, we also synthesised data for the primary outcome using a random‐effects model to evaluate whether this alters the significance of the results.

Results

Description of studies

For substantive descriptions of the studies, please see Characteristics of included studies and Characteristics of excluded studies tables.

Results of the search

1. The Cochrane Schizophrenia Group Trials Register (2008, 2013)

The electronic search found 14 citations. After removing duplicate publications, 13 potentially eligible studies were identified, of which five were included and eight were excluded (Figure 2).


Study flow diagram.

Study flow diagram.

Included studies

Details of the five included studies are provided in the Characteristics of included studies tables.

1. Design

The studies were randomised controlled trials and were conducted from between 1965 to 1980. All but one study (Leitch 1965) was described as double blind, however this study reported that great care was taken so that clinician would not know which drug was being given and the trial was blind as far as the doctor was concerned.

2. Study duration

Study duration varied from six weeks (Iwanaga 1980) to 12 weeks (Ananth 1977; Leitch 1965; Rasch 1966). The other study (Grant 1965 ) lasted eight weeks.

3. Settings

All studies were conducted in single inpatient settings apart from Iwanaga 1980, which was a multi‐centre study.

4. Interventions

Pericyazine was compared with chlorpromazine in three studies (Ananth 1977; Grant 1965; Rasch 1966). One study compared pericyazine with trifluoperazine (Leitch 1965) and one with zotepine (Iwanaga 1980). Four of the studies compared pericyazine with typical antipsychotics (Ananth 1977; Grant 1965; Leitch 1965; Rasch 1966) and one with an atypical antipsychotic (Iwanaga 1980).

5. Participants

The majority of participants had a diagnosis of chronic schizophrenia (Grant 1965; Iwanaga 1980; Leitch 1965; Rasch 1966) and newly diagnosed schizophrenia (Ananth 1977). Most participants were aged between 18 to 77 but two studies (Iwanaga 1980; Rasch 1966) did not report this information. The number of participants ranged from 30 (Ananth 1977) to 93 (Iwanaga 1980) participants.

6. Outcomes

The primary outcome of global state ‐ not improved was reported by three studies (Iwanaga 1980; Leitch 1965; Rasch 1966). Global state ‐ relapse was only reported by one study (Leitch 1965). Specific adverse effects of extrapyramidal symptoms were reported by four studies (Grant 1965; Iwanaga 1980; Leitch 1965; Rasch 1966). The secondary outcome of adverse effects were reported by all of the studies. Leaving the study early was reported by Ananth 1977; Grant 1965; Iwanaga 1980. No study reported outcomes or useable data for the following outcomes: mental state, behaviour or cost of care.

Excluded studies

We excluded eight studies. Two studies were not randomised trials (Deutsch 1971; St Jean 1967). Another study (Nishikawa 1984) involved symptom‐free schizophrenic patients and did not report usable data. One study (Weir 1968) involved mentally disturbed patients and it was unclear if they were schizophrenia patients and hence was excluded. Ananth 1972 and Barker 1969 and Denef 1971 and Heller 1965 were excluded as they reported no usable data.

1. Awaiting assessment

There are no trials currently awaiting assessment.

2. Ongoing trials

At the time of writing, we know of no ongoing trials.

Risk of bias in included studies

The 'Summary of findings' tables (summary of findings Table for the main comparison; summary of findings Table 2) describe bias by outcome, an overview is reported below and graphical impressions are also available (Figure 3; Figure 4)


'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.


'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

Allocation

All five studies were reported as randomised, but only one (Leitch 1965) reported the method of randomisation (allocated to a randomised list). None of the studies stated how allocation concealment was conducted or ensured.

Blinding

All the studies were described as double blind except Leitch 1965, who used a single‐blind methodology, however it is reported that great care was taken that clinician would not know which drug was being given and the trial was blind as far as the doctor was concerned.

No study reported the blinding of outcome assessors.

Two studies (Ananth 1977) and (Iwanaga 1980) did not report the methods of blinding. Grant 1965 used matched appearing tablets and syrup, whereas Rasch 1966 used identical tablets. Leitch 1965, a single‐blind study, reported that identical tablets were not obtained.

Incomplete outcome data

'As‐treated' analyses were undertaken for all studies apart from Iwanaga 1980 who used the Last Observastion Carried Forward (LOCF) method. There was high attrition bias identified for Leitch 1965 and Rasch 1966. Both these studies had people leave the study early, however it was not reported which intervention they had received.

Selective reporting

All the studies reported data on one of the primary outcomes. However,for some outcomes data were unusable. It is difficult to judge whether this is bias, inadvertent or poor reproduction of data. 

Other potential sources of bias

With poor reporting of randomisation methods and allocation concealment, all the studies are prone to at least a moderate degree of bias. No study reported information on funding, but this is not itself necessarily indicative of bias.

Effects of interventions

See: Summary of findings for the main comparison Pericyazine versus Typical antipsychotic for schizophrenia; Summary of findings 2 Pericyazine versus Atypical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

We calculated risk ratio (RR) for binary data with 95% confidence intervals (CIs).All data were synthesised using a fixed‐effect model, however, we also synthesised data for the primary outcomes using a random‐effects model.

1. Comparison: Pericyazine versus Typical Antipsychotics

This comparison included four studies (Ananth 1977; Grant 1965; Leitch 1965; Rasch 1966).

1.1. Global state ‐ not improved

No significant difference between pericyazine and typical antipsychotics in the clinical change of global state was found (2 RCTs, n = 122, RR 1.24 CI 0.93 to 1.66, Analysis 1.1). The significance did not change when analysed using the random‐effects method.

1.2. Global state ‐ relapse

One person who took pericyazine experienced a relapse of symptoms while no one who took typical antipsychotics experienced a relapse, this difference was not found to be significant (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 2.59 CI 0.11 to 61.75, Analysis 1.1).

1.3. Adverse effects: 1.Specific (primary outcome)

Significantly more people taking pericyazine experienced extrapyramidal symptoms (3 RCTs, n = 163, RR 0.52 CI 0.34 to 0.80), parkinsonism symptoms (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 0.49 CI 0.28 to 0.86) and akathisia (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 0.22 CI 0.05 to 0.95) compared to those taking pericyazine (Analysis 1.2). The significance did not change when analysed using the random‐effects method.

1.4. Adverse effects: 2. Specific (secondary outcome)

Significant differences were not found for nausea (1 RCT, n = 41, RR 5.24 CI 0.27 to 102.81), facial pallor (1 RCT, n = 41, RR 3.14 CI 0.14 to 72.92), drowsiness (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 0.86 CI 0.06 to 13.28), hypotension (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 2.59 CI 0.11 to 61.75), ataxia (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 0.86 CI 0.06 to 13.28), faintness (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 2.59 CI 0.11 to 61.75), depression (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 0.86 CI 0.30 to 2.44) or epileptic fit (1 RCT, n = 80, RR 0.29 CI 0.01 to 6.86, Analysis 1.2).

1.5 Adverse effects: 3. General

Although not statistically significant less people taking typical antipsychotics experienced general adverse events than those taking pericyazine (1 RCT, n = 30, RR 1.18 CI 0.82 to 1.70, Analysis 1.3).

1.6. Leaving the study early ‐ for specific reasons

Less people in the pericyazine group left the study due to deterioration in their condition (1 RCT, n = 30, RR 0.20 CI 0.03 to 1.51). One person taking pericyazine left the study due to side effects whereas no one taking typical antipsychotics did (1 RCT, n = 41, RR 3.14 CI 0.14 to 72.92, Analysis 1.4).

2. Comparison: Pericyazine versus Atypical Antipsychotics

This comparison included only one study (Iwanaga 1980).

2.1. Global state ‐ not improved

A similar proportion of people showed no improvement in both the pericyazine and atypical antipsychotic groups, therefore no significant difference was found (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.97 CI 0.67 to 1.42, Analysis 2.1). The significance did not change when analysed using random‐effects method.

2.2. Adverse effects: 1. Specific (primary outcome)

Significantly more people in the pericyazine group experienced akathisia (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 3.29 CI 0.98 to 11.04), parkinsonism (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 4.49 CI 1.39 to 14.48) and extrapyramidal symptoms (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 2.69 CI 1.35 to 5.36, Analysis 2.2). No significant differences were found for other events associated with movement disorders including dyskinesia (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 3.59 CI 0.42 to 30.93). The significance did not change when analysed using the random‐effects method.

2.3 Adverse effects: 2. Specific (secondary outcome)

Cardiac‐related events did not reveal any significant differences between those taking pericyazine or atypical antipsychotics for palpitations (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.90 CI 0.06 to 13.93), chest tightness (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 2.70 CI 0.11 to 64.61) or lightheadedness (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 1.80 CI 0.58 to 5.55, Analysis 2.2).

Various states of arousal were experienced by people taking both pericyazine and atypical antipsychotics but no significant differences were found for insomnia (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 1.91 CI 0.91 to 3.98), fatigue (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.64 CI 0.22 to 1.88), feeling of weariness of the lower extremity (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 1.80 CI 0.58 to 5.55) or somnolence (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.60 CI 0.18 to 1.98).

Many physical symptoms were reported but the events were quite often experienced by both of the groups so no significant differences were found for headache (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 3.59 CI 0.42 to 30.93), ataxia (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.18 CI 0.01 to 3.65), dysuria (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.45 CI 0.04 to 4.78), dysarthria (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.30 CI 0.01 to 7.18), diarrhoea (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 1.80 CI 0.35 to 9.33), constipation (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.45 CI 0.09 to 2.33), nausea (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 1.50 CI 0.59 to 3.78), joint pain (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.30 CI 0.01 to 7.18), ophthalmic symptoms (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 8.10 CI 0.45 to 146.31), dry mouth (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 2.10 CI 0.58 to 7.61) or dyshidrosis (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 2.69 CI 0.29 to 24.96).

Other adverse events reported included experiencing an abnormal sensation (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 6.30 CI 0.33 to 118.66), agitation (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 2.69 CI 0.78 to 9.32) and appetite loss (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 1.23 CI 0.55 to 2.79) although no significant differences were found.

2.4 Leaving the study early

People taking pericyazine were no more likely to leave the study early than those taking atypical antipsychotics (1 RCT, n = 93, RR 0.13 CI 0.01 to 2.42, Analysis 2.3).

Discussion

Summary of main results

1. Comparison: Pericyazine versus Typical Antipsychotics

The summary of findings Table for the main comparison shows that despite four trials providing data for this comparison, the quality of evidence was quite low. No study reported data on satisfaction with treatment, death, mental state, behaviour or economic outcomes.

1.1. Global state ‐ not improved

Two trials reported the number of people who had not improved, and although more people who took pericyazine had not improved, the difference was not significant and this was only based on 122 people. No study reported any participants got worse during the trials.

1.2. Relapse

One study reported data on relapse and only one person who took pericyazine experienced a relapse of symptoms, therefore no significant difference was found. One other study (Ananth 1977) reported that people left the study early due to a deterioration in symptoms but this was not classified as a relapse, but even if the data from the five people who took typical antipsychotics and the one person who took pericyazine who left early due to relapse were combined in this comparison, the difference between groups was still not significant.

1.3. Adverse effects ‐ 1. Specific (primary outcome)

Overall, people taking pericyazine were more likely to experience extrapyramidal symptoms than those taking typical antipsychotics. This was based on three studies with 163 people, however in one study two people from each intervention experienced extrapyramidal symptoms. In the other two studies, two people taking pericyazine experienced these symptoms whereas in one nobody taking typical antipsychotics did, so this significant finding depends on the larger study for this comparison (Leitch 1965) where these symptoms were experienced in high numbers by both groups, but more by people taking typical antipsychotics.Two of the three studies were comparing pericyazine with chlorpromazine, one with trifluoperazine. Therefore, these results may depend more on the individual drug used as the comparator rather than typical antipsychotics in general.

1.4. Adverse effects ‐ 2. Specific (secondary outcome)

Although there were eight specific adverse effects reported that were not extrapyramidal symptoms, no significant difference between the pericyazine and typical antipsychotic groups were found. Overall, more side effects were experienced by people taking typical antipsychotics rather than pericyazine.

1.5. Adverse events ‐ general

More people who took typical antipsychotics experienced general adverse events than those taking pericyazine but this was not found to be significant and was only based on one study that did not report individual figures for specific adverse effects.

1.6. Leaving the study early ‐ for specific reasons

More people who took typical antipsychotics left the study early due to experiencing a deterioration in their condition and only one person who took pericyazine left the study early due to the side effects they experienced despite the number of side effects that were reported. Neither difference was found to be significant.

2. Comparison: Pericyazine versus Atypical Antipsychotics

This comparison included only one study Iwanaga 1980 and as summary of findings Table 2 shows, there are many outcomes that are absent and the ones that have been reported, again lack quality and have been graded as very low. No study reported data on relapse, satisfaction with treatment, death, mental state, behaviour or economic outcomes.

2.1. Global state ‐ not improved

A similar proportion, roughly half, of people showed no improvement in both the pericyazine and atypical antipsychotic groups, therefore no significant difference was found.

2.2. Adverse effects ‐ 1. Specific (primary outcome)

Significantly more people in the pericyazine group experienced akathisia, parkinsonism and extrapyramidal symptoms. However, no significant difference was found for dyskinesia, which is another commonly occurring movement disorder side effect, although more people who took pericyazine reported these effects than those who took atypical antipsychotics. Again, this finding is based on one small trial, and findings may have been more pronounced had other larger, good quality studies been conducted.

2.3. Adverse effects ‐ 2. Specific (secondary outcome)

Although a total of 21 different adverse effects were reported, no other significant differences were found. The adverse events that were reported were often experienced by small numbers of people from both the pericyazine and atypical antipsychotic groups and are often reported with the use of antipsychotics.

2.4. Leaving the study early

Although three people who took atypical antipsychotics left the study early, not one person from the pericyazine group left the study early so although pericyazine was technically more favourable, this finding was not significant.

Overall completeness and applicability of evidence

1. Completeness

There were only five relevant studies identified for this review, all of which were small with less than 50 people per arm, therefore none of the outcomes included data from large numbers of people. Also, only three typical and one atypical antipsychotics were compared with pericyazine whereas, many more are listed in the BNF 2008. There were no data on satisfaction with treatment, relapse, general symptoms or economic outcomes. Poor reporting adds to this problem as there were many outcomes that were reported but simply had no useable data. There was considerable inconsistency in the way that the outcomes were measured and reported. Diagnoses and other participant characteristics were also often poorly described. The studies identified would therefore seem insufficient to address all the objectives of the review to allow strong conclusions to be drawn based on the available evidence. 

2. Applicability

The studies included in this review were conducted several decades ago and all in inpatient settings, although this does not make findings irrelevant, we have no knowledge of the effectiveness of pericyazine for people treated in the community or for long‐‐term outcomes as all studies were of short duration. Reporting of the studies was poor and there are many important outcomes that are missing, therefore the extent of the applicability of findings is unclear.

Quality of the evidence

Overall, the quality of reporting was poor and this is reflected in the 'Summary of findings'[ tables for both of the comparisons. Only five studies were identified and all the studies were conducted between 1965 to 1980, therefore, there may be difficulty in applying results as the studies are judged against the standards of today in which studies follow guidelines such as the CONSORT statement (Moher 2001) for reporting. The information provided was often insufficient to allow a valid judgement to be made on study quality and this is reflected in the Risk of bias in included studies and the summary of findings Table for the main comparison and summary of findings Table 2. Most included studies did not report the method of randomisation, allocation concealment or details of blinding of personnel, participants or outcome assessors and the loss of participants to follow‐up was often not described. The studies included in this review were all relatively small and studies of this size are unlikely to detect subtle but important differences with any confidence. No study was an international multi‐centre trial, only one was multi‐centre and all were conducted in inpatient settings. Two of the five included studies were conducted in the UK and the three remaining studies were conducted in Canada, Japan and Denmark respectively. Until further large, good quality, multi‐centre randomised studies are undertaken, the evidence for using pericyazine for schizophrenia will remain unclear.

Potential biases in the review process

We are not aware of biases in the review process. We may have failed to identify small studies because of a degree of publishing bias (Egger 1997), but do not think we would have not found large relevant studies. We did not contact authors of included studies as they were all completed over 30 years ago.

Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews

No other systematic review covering the exact specification of this protocol has been identified.

10‐[3‐(4‐hydroxypiperidino)propyl] phenothiazine‐2‐carbonitrile)]
Figures and Tables -
Figure 1

10‐[3‐(4‐hydroxypiperidino)propyl] phenothiazine‐2‐carbonitrile)]

Study flow diagram.
Figures and Tables -
Figure 2

Study flow diagram.

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.
Figures and Tables -
Figure 3

'Risk of bias' graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.

'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.
Figures and Tables -
Figure 4

'Risk of bias' summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 1 Global state.
Figures and Tables -
Analysis 1.1

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 1 Global state.

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 2 Adverse effects: 1. Specific.
Figures and Tables -
Analysis 1.2

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 2 Adverse effects: 1. Specific.

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 3 Adverse effects: 2. General.
Figures and Tables -
Analysis 1.3

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 3 Adverse effects: 2. General.

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 4 Leaving the study early.
Figures and Tables -
Analysis 1.4

Comparison 1 PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 4 Leaving the study early.

Comparison 2 PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 1 Global state ‐ not improved.
Figures and Tables -
Analysis 2.1

Comparison 2 PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 1 Global state ‐ not improved.

Comparison 2 PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 2 Adverse effects: Specific.
Figures and Tables -
Analysis 2.2

Comparison 2 PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 2 Adverse effects: Specific.

Comparison 2 PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 3 Leaving the study early.
Figures and Tables -
Analysis 2.3

Comparison 2 PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC, Outcome 3 Leaving the study early.

Table 1. Suggested design for future study

Methods

Allocation: randomised, describing method of randomisation and allocation concealment.
Blinding: double blind.
Duration: 12 months.

Participants

Diagnosis: schizophrenia.
n: 300.
Sex: male and female.
Age: > 18 years old.

Intervention

1. Pericyazine: any dose and mode or pattern of administration.

2. Other antipsychotic: any dose and mode or pattern of administration.

Outcomes

Global state.

Leaving the study early.
Mental state.
General functioning.
Quality of life.
Adverse effects.
Economic outcomes.

Notes

Any outcomes measured using scale‐derived data should be interpreted in such a way as to make clear the real‐life relevance of changes in scale score.

Figures and Tables -
Table 1. Suggested design for future study
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Pericyazine versus Typical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Pericyazine compared with typical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Patient or population: patients with schizophrenia
Settings: Inpatient
Intervention: Pericyazine
Comparison: Typical antipsychotic

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Typical antipsychotic

Pericyazine

Global state ‐ not improved
Follow‐up: 6‐12 weeks

Low risk population1

RR 1.24
(0.93 to 1.66)

122
(2 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low2,3,4

200 per 1000

248 per 1000
(186 to 332)

Medium risk population1

500 per 1000

620 per 1000
(465 to 830)

High risk population1

800 per 1000

992 per 1000
(744 to 1000)

Global state ‐ relapse
Follow‐up: mean 3 months

See comment

See comment

Not estimable

80
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low5,6,7

Adverse events ‐ for specific reasons ‐ Extrapyramidal side effect
Follow‐up: 6‐12 weeks

Low risk population1

RR 0.59
(0.38 to 0.89)

163
(3 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low2,3,4

200 per 1000

118 per 1000
(76 to 178)

Medium risk population1

400 per 1000

236 per 1000
(152 to 356)

High risk population1

500 per 1000

295 per 1000
(190 to 445)

Mental state

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Leaving the study early for specific reasons
Follow‐up: 9‐12 weeks

Low risk population1

RR 0.46
(0.11 to 1.9)

71
(2 studies)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low7,8,9

100 per 1000

46 per 1000
(11 to 190)

Medium risk population1

150 per 1000

69 per 1000
(16 to 285)

High risk population1

200 per 1000

92 per 1000
(22 to 380)

Behaviour

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Economic outcomes

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported useable data for this outcome.

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Moderate risk approximately equates to that of the control group in the studies.
2 Risk of bias: 'very serious' ‐ randomisation and allocation concealment was generally unclear in all trials. Blinding was achieved in two of the trials, but were untested. Seven people left the Leitch 1965 study early, but were unaccounted for in the final analysis.
3 Imprecision: 'very serious' ‐ both sample size and effect size are small. Meta‐anlaysis result was not significant.
4 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ no explanation was provided.
5 Risk of bias: 'serious' ‐ allocation concealment was unclear; drop out was not included in final analysis.
6 Imprecision: 'serious' ‐ only one trial contributed data to this outcome.
7 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ only one trial was identified.
8 Risk of bias: 'serious' ‐ randomisation and allocation concealment were not described. Blinding of participants was untested. Loss to follow up were unaccounted for in final analysis.
9 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ only a limited small number of trials were identified. Most of the trials either favoured (or has a tendency to favour) the investigated intervention.

Figures and Tables -
Summary of findings for the main comparison. Pericyazine versus Typical antipsychotic for schizophrenia
Summary of findings 2. Pericyazine versus Atypical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Pericyazine compared with atypical antipsychotic for schizophrenia

Patient or population: patients with schizophrenia
Settings: Inpatient
Intervention: Pericyazine
Comparison: Atypical antipsychotic

Outcomes

Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI)

Relative effect
(95% CI)

No of Participants
(studies)

Quality of the evidence
(GRADE)

Comments

Assumed risk

Corresponding risk

Atypical antipsychotic

Pericyazine

Global state ‐ not improved
Follow‐up: mean 6 weeks

Low

RR 0.97
(0.67 to 1.42)

93
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2,3

450 per 1000

437 per 1000
(302 to 639)

Moderate

550 per 1000

534 per 1000
(369 to 781)

High

650 per 1000

631 per 1000
(436 to 923)

Adverse Effects ‐ Extrapyramidal symptoms
Follow‐up: mean 6 weeks

Low

RR 2.69
(1.35 to 5.36)

93
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low1,2,3

100 per 1000

269 per 1000
(135 to 536)

Moderate

200 per 1000

538 per 1000
(270 to 1000)

High

300 per 1000

807 per 1000
(405 to 1000)

Mental state

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Leaving the study early for specific reasons
Follow‐up: 9‐12 weeks

Low4

RR 0.46
(0.11 to 1.9)

93
(1 study)

⊕⊝⊝⊝
very low3,5,6

50 per 1000

23 per 1000
(5 to 95)

Moderate4

100 per 1000

46 per 1000
(11 to 190)

High4

150 per 1000

69 per 1000
(16 to 285)

Behaviour

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

Economic outcomes

not reported

See comment.

See comment.

Not estimable

See comment.

No study reported this outcome.

*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;

GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect.
Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate.
Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate.
Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate.

1 Limitations of design: 'serious' ‐ lack of blinding, randomisation unclear.
2 Imprecision: 'serious' ‐ only one trial was identified and the effect size of which is small with wide confidence intervals.
3 Publication bias: 'strongly suspected' ‐ only one trial was identified.
4 Moderate risk approximately equates to that of the control group in the studies.
5 Risk of bias: 'serious' ‐ randomisation and allocation concealment were not described. Blinding of participants was untested. Loss to follow up were unaccounted for in final analysis.
6 Imprecision: 'serious' ‐ only one trial was identified.

Figures and Tables -
Summary of findings 2. Pericyazine versus Atypical antipsychotic for schizophrenia
Comparison 1. PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Global state Show forest plot

2

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

1.1 not improved

2

122

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.24 [0.93, 1.66]

1.2 relapse

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.59 [0.11, 61.75]

2 Adverse effects: 1. Specific Show forest plot

3

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

2.1 cardiovascular ‐ facial pallor

1

41

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.14 [0.14, 72.92]

2.2 cardiovascular ‐ faintness

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.59 [0.11, 61.75]

2.3 cardiovascular ‐ hypotension

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.59 [0.11, 61.75]

2.4 central nervous system ‐ depression

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.86 [0.30, 2.44]

2.5 central nervous system ‐ drowsiness

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.86 [0.06, 13.28]

2.6 central nervous system ‐ epileptic fit

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.29 [0.01, 6.86]

2.7 gastrointestinal ‐ nausea/vomiting

1

41

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

5.24 [0.27, 102.81]

2.8 movement disorder ‐ akathesia

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.22 [0.05, 0.95]

2.9 movement disorder ‐ ataxia

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.86 [0.06, 13.28]

2.10 movement disorder ‐ extrapyramidal side effect

3

163

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.52 [0.34, 0.80]

2.11 movement disorder ‐ parkinsonism

1

80

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.49 [0.28, 0.86]

3 Adverse effects: 2. General Show forest plot

1

30

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.18 [0.82, 1.70]

4 Leaving the study early Show forest plot

2

71

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.46 [0.11, 1.90]

4.1 due to deterioration of condition

1

30

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.2 [0.03, 1.51]

4.2 due to side effects

1

41

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.14 [0.14, 72.92]

Figures and Tables -
Comparison 1. PERICYAZINE versus TYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC
Comparison 2. PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC

Outcome or subgroup title

No. of studies

No. of participants

Statistical method

Effect size

1 Global state ‐ not improved Show forest plot

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.97 [0.67, 1.42]

2 Adverse effects: Specific Show forest plot

1

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

2.1 anticholinergic ‐ dry mouth

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.10 [0.58, 7.61]

2.2 anticholinergic ‐ dyshidrosis

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.69 [0.29, 24.96]

2.3 anticholinergic ‐ opthalmic symptoms

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

8.1 [0.45, 146.31]

2.4 cardiovascular ‐ chest tightness Feeling

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.7 [0.11, 64.61]

2.5 cardiovascular ‐ lightheadedness

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.80 [0.58, 5.55]

2.6 cardiovascular ‐ palpitation

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.90 [0.06, 13.93]

2.7 central nervous system ‐ abnormal sensation

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

6.3 [0.33, 118.66]

2.8 central nervous system ‐ fatigue

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.64 [0.22, 1.88]

2.9 central nervous system ‐ headache/heaviness of head

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.59 [0.42, 30.93]

2.10 central nervous system ‐ insomnia

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.91 [0.91, 3.98]

2.11 central nervous system ‐ somnolence

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.60 [0.18, 1.98]

2.12 gastrointestinal ‐ appetite loss

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.23 [0.55, 2.79]

2.13 gastrointestinal ‐ constipation

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.45 [0.09, 2.33]

2.14 gastrointestinal ‐ diarrhoea

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.80 [0.35, 9.33]

2.15 gastrointestinal ‐ nausea/ vomiting

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.50 [0.59, 3.78]

2.16 movement disorder ‐ akathisia

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.29 [0.98, 11.04]

2.17 movement disorder ‐ ataxia

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.18 [0.01, 3.65]

2.18 movement disorder ‐ dysarthria

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.3 [0.01, 7.18]

2.19 movement disorder ‐ dyskinesia

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.59 [0.42, 30.93]

2.20 movement disorder ‐ extrapyramidal symptoms

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.69 [1.35, 5.36]

2.21 movement disorder ‐ joint pain/rigor

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.3 [0.01, 7.18]

2.22 movement disorder ‐ parkinsonism

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

4.49 [1.39, 14.48]

2.23 other ‐ agitation

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.69 [0.78, 9.32]

2.24 other ‐ dysuria

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.45 [0.04, 4.78]

2.25 other ‐ feeling of weariness of lower extremity

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.80 [0.58, 5.55]

3 Leaving the study early Show forest plot

1

93

Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.13 [0.01, 2.42]

Figures and Tables -
Comparison 2. PERICYAZINE versus ATYPICAL ANTIPSYCHOTIC