Skip to main content
Log in

Reading ability and memory span: Long-term memory contributions to span for good and poor readers

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A number of studies have shown that childrenwith reading difficulties perform poorly ontests of verbal memory span. The extent towhich differences in memory span for good andpoor readers can be explained by differences ina long-term memory component to span as well asby differences in short-term memory processeswas investigated in this study. Memory spanand rehearsal rate were measured for high andlow frequency words and nonwords. Althoughmemory span performance for high frequencywords was comparable for all reading abilitygroups, good readers had better memory spanperformance for low frequency words. This wasattributable to differences in both short-termand long-term memory contributions to spanperformance. Differences between readingability groups also emerged when memory spanfor nonwords was measured. In this case,differences between groups also appeared to bethe result of difficulties which poor readersencountered in learning newphonologically-based materials (i.e. nonwords). The nature of the inter-relationships betweenmemory span, reading and measures ofphonological awareness are discussed in thelight of these findings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read-thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In: G.H. Bower (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 8 (pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D., Thomson, N. & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of shortterm memory, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14: 575-589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D., Lewis, V.J. & Vallar, G. (1984). Exploring the articulatory loop, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 36: 233-252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaton, A., McDougall, S. & Singleton, C. (1997). Humpty Dumpty grows up?-diagnosing dyslexia in adulthood, Journal of Research in Reading 20: 1-12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beech, J.R. & Harding, L.M. (1984). Phonemic processing from a developmental lag viewpoint, Reading Research Quarterly 14: 357-366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisanz, G.L., Das, J.P. & Mancini, G. (1984). Children's memory for phonetically confusable and nonconfusable letters: Changes with age and reading ability, Child Development 55: 1845-1854.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. (1986). Short-term memory, phonological processing and reading ability, Annals of Dyslexia 36: 138-153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. & Shankweiler, D. (1991). Phonological processes in literacy. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits, Developmental Psychology 28: 874-886.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J., Davies, P. & Richman, B. (1971). The American Heritage Word Frequency Book. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cormier, P. & Dea, S. (1997). Distinctive patterns of relationship of phonological awareness and working memory with reading development, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 9: 193-206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Day, L. Saults, J.S., Keller, T.A., Johnson, T. & Flores, L. (1992). The role of verbal output time in the effects of word length on immediate memory, Journal of Memory and Language 34: 1-17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Keller, T.A., Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., McDougall, S. & Rack, J. (1994). Verbal memory span in children: Speech timing clues to the mechanisms underlying age and word length effects, Journal of Memory and Language 33: 234-250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Wood, N.L., Nugent, L.D & Treisman, M. (1997). There are two word-length effects in verbal short-term memory: Opposed effects of duration and complexity, Psychological Science 8: 290-295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, R., Hulme, C. & Brown, G. (1992). STM Experimenter: Using Hypercard and MacRecorder in short-term memory experiments, Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers 24: 575-579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S., Shankweiler, D., Macaruso, P. & Bar-Shalom, E. (1990). Working memory and comprehension of spoken sentences: Investigations of children with reading disorders. In: G. Vallar & T. Shallice (eds.), Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory (pp. 447-508). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dark, V.J. & Benbow, C.P. (1991). Differential enhancement of working memory with mathematical versus verbal precocity, Journal of Educational Psychology 83: 48-60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Neilsen, I. & Petersen, D.K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in nonword reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items, Annals of Dyslexia 44: 205-226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, C.A. (1983). The British Ability Scales. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D.J., Shaywitz, S.E., Stuebing, K.K., Shaywitz, B.A. & Fletcher, J.M. (1996). Developmental lag verus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal, individual growth curves analysis, Journal of Educational Psychology 88: 3-17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. (1997). Models of verbal short-term memory. In: M.A. Conway (ed.), Cognitive models of memory (pp. 13-45). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. & Adams, A-M. (1994). Children's phonological working memory: Contributions of long-term knowledge and rehearsal, Journal of Memory and Language 33: 672-688.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S.E. & Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language 29: 336-360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G.J., Service, E., Martin, A.J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children, Developmental Psychology 33: 966-979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S., Willis, C.S., Emslie, H & Baddeley, A.D. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and word-likeness on children's repetition of nonwords, Applied Psycholinguistics 12: 349-367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Siegel, L.S. & Stanovich, K.E. (1997). The assessment of adults with reading disabilities: What can we learn from experimental tasks? Journal of Research in Reading 20: 42-54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A, Stanovich, K.E. & Siegel, L.S. (1996). The relationship between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 63: 563-582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregg, V.H., Freedman, C.M. & Smith, D.K. (1989). Word frequency, articulatory suppression and memory span, British Journal of Psychology 80: 363-374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, J. & Bowey, J.A. (1994). Phonological analysis skills, verbal working memory and reading ability in second-grade children, Child Development 65: 938-950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animals. In: J.F. Delfresnaye (ed.), Brain mechanisms and learning (pp. 37-46). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry, L.A. & Millar, S. (1991). Memory span increase with age: A test of two hypotheses, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 51: 459-484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hitch, G.J., Halliday, M.S. & Littler, J.E. (1989). Item identification time and rehearsal as predictors of memory span in children, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 41A: 321-338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holligan, C. & Johnston, R.S. (1988). The use of phonological information by good and poor readers in memory and reading tasks, Journal of Memory and Cognition 16: 522-532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C. (1981). Reading retardation and multi-sensory teaching. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Maughan, S. & Brown, G.D.A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span, Journal of Memory and Language 30: 685-701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S, Schweikert, R., Brown, G.D.A., Martin, S. & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 23: 1217-1232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, R.B., Healy, A.F. & Shankweiler, D. (1983). Phonetic coding and order memory in relation to reading proficiency: A comparison of short-term memory for temporal and spatial order information, Applied Psycholinguistics 4: 229-250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leather, C.V. & Henry, L.A. (1994). Working span and phonological awareness tasks as predictors of early reading ability, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 58: 88-111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I.Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A.M., Fowler, C. & Fischer, F.W. (1977). Phonetic segmentation and recoding in the beginning readers. In: A.S. Reber & D.L. Scarborough (eds.), Toward a psychology of reading: The proceedings of the CUNY Conferences (pp. 207-225). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V.A., Liberman, I.Y. & Shankweiler, D. (1980). Children's memory for sentences and word strings in relation to reading ability, Memory and Cognition 5: 623-629.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDougall, S., Hulme, C., Ellis, A. & Monk, A. (1994). Learning to read: The role of shortterm memory and phonological skills, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 58: 112-133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michas, I.C. & Henry, I.C. (1994). The link between phonological memory and vocabulary acquisition, British Journal of Developmental Psychology 12: 147-163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V. & Snowling, M. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills, Reading Research Quarterly 33: 320-337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. & Kyle, F. (2000). The relation between phonological awareness and working memory, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 75: 152-164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R.K., Davidson, B.J., Kleigl, R. & Davies, S.E. (1984). Development of phonetic memory in disabled and normal readers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 37: 187-206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J., Snowling, M. & Olson, R. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review, Reading Research Quarterly 27: 29-53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapala, M. M. & Brady, S. (1990). Reading ability and short-term memory: The role of phonological processing, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 1-25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohl, M. & Pratt, C. (1995). Phonological awareness, verbal working memory and the acquisition of literacy, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 327-360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C. & Brown, G.D.A. (1993). The development of short-term memory span: Separable effects of speech rate and long-term memory, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 56: 431-442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., Alban, J. & Ellis, A.W. (1994). Effects of word frequency and age of acquisition on short-term memory span, Memory and Cognition 22: 695-701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sattler, J. (1982). Assessment of children's intelligence and special abilities. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P., Taylor, H.G., Friel, J. & Fletcher, J.M. (1978). Some developmental and predictive precursors of reading disabilities: A six year follow-up. In: A.L. Benton & D. Pearl (eds.), Dyslexia: An appraisal of current knowledge (pp. 313-347). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I.Y, Mark, L.S., Fowler, C.A. & Fischer, F.W. (1979). The speech code and learning to read, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 5: 531-545.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D.L. & Stanovich, K.E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition, Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology 1: 1-57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S. & Linder, B.A. (1984). STM processes in children with reading and arithmetic learning disabilities, Developmental Psychology 20: 200-207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Goulandris, N., Bowlby, M. & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41: 489-507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Hulme, C., Smith, A. & Thomas, J. (1994). The effects of phonetic similarity and list length on children's sound categorisation performance, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 58: 160-180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M.A. (1993). Implicit serial learning: Questions inspired by Hebb (1961), Memory and Cognition 21: 819-827.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy, Research Research Quarterly 21: 360-407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor readers: The phonological-core variable-difference model, Journal of Learning Disabilities 21: 590-612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1991). Discrepancy definitions of reading ability: Has intelligence led us astray? Reading Research Quarterly 26: 7-29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In: P. Gough, L. Ehri & R.Treiman (eds.), Reading Acquisition (pp. 307-342). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1993). Dysrationalia: A new specific learning disability, Journal of Learning Disabilities 26: 501-515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. & Siegel, L.S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model, Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 24-53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, G. & Hulme, C. (2000). The effects of word co-occurrence on short-term memory: Associative links in long-term memory affect short-term memory performance, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 26: 796-802.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H.L. (1999). Reading comprehension and working memory in learning disabled readers: Is the phonological loop more important than the executive system? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 72: 1-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H.L. & Trahan, M. (1996). Learning disabled and average readers working memory and comprehension: Does metacognition play a role? British Journal of Educational Psychology 66: 333-355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H.L., Ashbaker, M.H. & Lee, C. (1996). Learning-disabled readers' working memory as a function of processing demands, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 61: 242-275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M.S. (1988). Articulatory loop explanations of memory span and pronunciation rate correspondences: A cautionary note, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26: 293-296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, G. & L.S. Siegel (1996). A critical evaluation of the IQ-achievement discrepancy based definition of dyslexia. In: K.P. van den Bos, L.S. Siegel, D.J. Bakker & D.L. Share (eds.), Current directions in dyslexia research (pp. 45-70). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. & Hirsch-Pasek, K. (1985). Are there qualitative differences between dyslexic and normal readers? Memory and Cognition 13: 357-364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, M.J. (1977). The intricacy of memory span, Memory and Cognition 5: 529-534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, C.E. (1979). Duration differences between rare and common words and their implications for the interpretation of word frequency effects, Memory and Cognition 7: 411-419.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Siné J.P. McDougall.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McDougall, S.J., Donohoe, R. Reading ability and memory span: Long-term memory contributions to span for good and poor readers. Reading and Writing 15, 359–387 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015224830778

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015224830778

Navigation