Skip to main content
Log in

Comparing the phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study tested the predictions of thephonological and double deficit hypotheses byexperimentally examining speech perception,phoneme awareness, lexical retrieval (serialand discrete), articulatory speed, and verbalSTM in school age child (N = 35) and adolescent(N = 36) dyslexics, and both chronological age(CA) and reading age (RA) controls. Theresults confirmed the findings of previousstudies of a deficit in phoneme awareness indevelopmental dyslexia. At both age levels,dyslexics performed significantly more poorlythan both their CA and RA controls. Althoughdeficits in the other processes investigated,particularly in rapid serial naming, were alsoapparent, they were not as clear-cut as thedeficit in phoneme awareness. In general,definite evidence of a deficit in rapid serialnaming was limited to the more severelyimpaired dyslexics. Furthermore, although rapidserial naming contributed independent variationto various literacy skills, its contributionwas modest relative to the contribution ofphoneme awareness, regardless of whether theliteracy skill relied more or less heavily onphonological or orthographic coding skills. Further analyses suggested that variation inrapid serial skill is particularly importantfor fluent reading of text, whereas phonemeawareness is particularly important for thedevelopment of the ability to read byphonologically recoding letters or groups ofletters in words into their phonological codes.This explains the relatively strongcontribution of phoneme awareness to readingand spelling ability in general. In sum, thephonological hypothesis offers a moreparsimonious account of the present resultsthan the double deficit hypothesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bowers, P.G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed's unique contributions to reading disability over time, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 189-216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G. & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 5: 69-85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S.A. (1997). Ability to encode phonological representations: An underlying difficulty of poor readers. In B.A. Blachman (ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia. Implications for early intervention (pp. 21-47). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S.A. (1991). The role of working memory on reading disability. In S.A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (eds.), Phonological processes in literacy. A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 129-151). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S., Shankweiler, D. & Mann, V. (1983). Speech perception and memory and coding in relation to reading ability, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 35: 345-367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits, Developmental Psychology 28: 874-887.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burhanpurkar, A. & Barron, R.W. (1999, April). Orthographic and phonological prerequisites for reading: Implications for dual-route models. Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, New Mexico.

  • Cardon, L.R., DeFries, J.C, Fulker, D.W., Kimberling, W.J., Pennington, B.F. & Smith, S.D. (1994). Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on chromosome 6, Science 265: 276-279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A. & Coltheart, M.C. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia, Cognition 47: 149-180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability, Journal of Learning Disability 25: 532-538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. & Rudel, R. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities, Neuropsychologia 14: 471-479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L.M. & Markwardt, F.C. (1970). Examiner's manual: Peabody Individual Achievement Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Borstrom, I. & Petersen, D. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items, Reading Research Quarterly 33: 36-60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In P. Gough, L. Ehri & R. Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107-144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J., Shaywitz, S., Shankweiler, D., Katz, L., Liberman, I., Stuebing, K., Francis, D., Fowler, A. & Shaywitz, B. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions, Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 6-23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A.E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In S.A. Brady & D.P. Shankweiler (eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 97-117). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, J. Marshall & M. Coltheart (eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301-330). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S.E. & Baddeley, A.D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names, British Journal of Educational Psychology 81: 439-454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, L.R. & Rozin, P. (1977). The structure and acquisition of reading: Relation between orthography and the structured language. In A.S. Reber & D.L. Scarborough (eds.), Toward a psychology of reading (pp. 1-53). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P.L. & Walsh, M.A (1991). Chinese, Phoenicians, and the orthographic cipher of English. In S. Brady & D.P. Shankweiler (eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 199-209). Illsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M.W. & Seidenberg, M.S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models, Psychological Review 106: 491-528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A.B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastak, S. & Wilkinson, G.S. (1984). WRAT-RWide Range Achievement Test. Wilmington, Delaware: Jastak Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, M. & Francis, W.N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., Frith, U. & Wimmer, H. (1996). Intrusion of orthographic knowledge on phoneme awareness: Strong in normal readers, weak in dyslexic readers, Applied Psycholinguistics 17: 1-14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G.R. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia, Annals of Dyslexia XLV: 3-27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S., Doi, L.M., McBride-Chang, C. & Petersen, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia, Cognition 58: 157-195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S. & Doi, L.M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders, Scientific Studies of Reading 3: 129-157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattingly, I.G. (1972). Reading, the linguistic process, and linguistic awareness. In J.F. Kavanaugh & I.G. Mattingly (eds.), Language by ear and by eye: The relationship between speech and reading (pp. 133-147). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. & Manis, F.R. (1996). Structural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double deficit hypothesis, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 323-339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M.S., Wood, F.B., Hart, L.A. & Felton, R.H. (1998). Selective predictive value of rapid automatized naming in poor readers, Journal of Learning Disabilities 31: 106-117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R., Stuebing, K., Fletcher, J., Shaywitz, S., Lyon, R., Shankweiler, D., Katz, L., Francis, D. & Shaywitz, B. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: A phonological core, Journal of Educational Psychology 90: 347-373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, L. & Pollatsek, A. (1994). Developmental dyslexia: Heterogeneity without discrete subgroups, Annals of Dyslexia 44: 120-146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B.F., Van Orden, G.C., Smith, S.D., Green, P.A. & Haith, M.M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics, Child Development 61: 1753-1778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B.F., McCabe, L.L., Smith, S.D., Lefly, D.L., Bookman, M.O., Kimberling, W.J. & Lubs, H.A. (1986). Spelling errors in adults with a form of familial dyslexia, Child Development 57: 1001-1013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B.F., Lefly, D.L., Van Orden, G.C., Bookman, M.O. & Smith, S.D. (1987). Is phonology bypassed in normal or dyslexic development? Annals of Dyslexia 37: 62-89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B.F. & Lefly, D.L. (in press). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia, Child Development.

  • Rack, J.P., Snowling, M.J. & Olson, R.K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia, Reading Research Quarterly 27: 28-53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H.S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children, Child Development 61: 1728-1743.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H.S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In P. Accardo, A. Capute & B. Shapiro (eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75-119). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H.S. & Domgaard, R.M. (1998, April). An exploration of the relationship between reading and rapid serial naming. Paper presented at the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, San Diego, CA.

  • Schroeder, M.R. (1968). Reference signal for signal quality studies, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 44: 1735-1736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S.D., Pennington, B.F., Kimberling, W.J. & Lubs, H.A. (1983). A genetic analysis of specific reading disability. In C. Ludlow (ed.), Genetic aspects of speech and language disorders (pp. 169-178). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S.D., Pennington, B.F., Kimberling, W.J. & Ing, P.S. (1990). Familial dyslexia: Use of genetic linkage data to define subtypes, Journal of the American Academy of Teen and Adolescent Psychiatry 29: 204-213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J.C. & Vanderwart, M. (1978). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity and visual complexity, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6: 174-215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity. In J.K. Torgesen & B.Y.L. Wong (eds.), Psychological and educational perspectives of learning disabilities (pp. 87-131). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1988). The dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonologicalcore variable-difference model, Journal of Learning Disabilities 21: 590-604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1994). Annotation: Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35: 579-595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. & Siegel, L.S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression based test of the phonological-core variable difference model, Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 24-53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E., Siegel, L.S. & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability, Journal of Educational Psychology 89: 114-127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, B. & Brady, S. (1995). Evidence for phonological processing deficits in less-skilled readers, Annals of Dyslexia 45: 51-78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K. & Torgeson, J.K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills, Psychological Bulletin 101: 192-212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K., Torgeson, J.K., Rashotte, C.A., Hecht, S.A., Barker, T.A., Burgess, S.R., Donahue, J. & Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study, Developmental Psychology 33: 468-479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for childrenrevised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiederholt, J.L. & Bryant, B.R. (1992). Gray oral reading tests, 2nd edn. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences, Reading Research Quarterly 26: 123-141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bally, H. & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers, Child Development 57: 988-1005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. & Bowers, P.G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias, Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 415-438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R.W. (1977). Woodcock-Johnson psycho-educational battery: Technical report. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Pennington, B.F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P.A. et al. Comparing the phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing 14, 707–755 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012239018038

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012239018038

Navigation