Skip to main content
Log in

Predicting reading level in grades 1 to 6 from listening level and decoding level: Testing theory relevant to the simple view of reading

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The relationships among pronunciation level (decoding), verbal level (listening), and accuracy level (reading) were investigated in grades 1 to 6, and for students who are in the advanced phase of decoding. The data collected were used to investigate the validity of the simple view of reading and the causal model of reading achievement which holds that pronunciation level (PL) and verbal level (VL) are the proximal causes of accuracy level (AL). A total of 135 students in grades 1-6 were given measures of nonword decoding, real word decoding, listening, and reading. All of the reliable variation in an indicant of the level of reading ability, AL, could be predicted from an indicant of the level of ability to decode real words, PL, and an indicant of listening level, VL. Furthermore, the strong relationship between pronunciation level, PL, and accuracy level, AL, did not evaporate for the students who had mastered basic decoding skills, as measured by nonword decoding tests. The correlations between pronunciation level, PL, and accuracy level, AL, were high even for students in grades 5 and 6, most of whom probably had progressed beyond the alphabetic phase (phonological recoding). Correlational support was found for the simple view of reading and the causal model which holds that AL is equal to the square root of the product of VL and PL. The above theory and supporting data were interpreted as suggesting that the level of reading accuracy, AL, of students can be improved the most throughout grades 1 to 6 by emphasizing instruction that will improve pronunciation level, or decoding, even for children who have progressed beyond the beginning to read phase which involves learning the alphabetic principal, or phonological recoding.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, T.A., Torgesen, J.K. & Wagner, R.K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks, Reading Research Quarterly 27: 334–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calfee, R.C. & Piontkowski, D.C. (1981). The reading diary: Acquisition of decoding, Reading Research Quarterly 16: 346–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J.B., Davies, P. & Richman, B. (1971). Word frequency book. New York: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1984). Rauding theory predictions of amount comprehended under different purposes and speed reading conditions, Reading Research Quarterly 19: 205–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1985). Measuring absolute amounts of reading comprehension using the rauding rescaling procedure, Journal of Reading Behavior 17: 29–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1990a). Reading rate: A review of research and theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1990b). Predicting accuracy of comprehension from the relative difficulty of the material, Learning and Individual Differences 2(4): 405–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1990c). Rescaling the Degrees of Reading Power test to provide valid scores for selecting materials at the instructional level, Journal of Reading Behavior 22: 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1991a). Manual for the Computer Assisted Reading Diagnosis. Kanas City, MO: Revrac Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1991b). Using letter-naming speed to diagnose reading disability, Remedial and Special Education 12: 33–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1992a). What do standardized tests of reading comprehension measure in terms of efficiency, accuracy and rate?, Reading Research Quarterly 27: 346–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1992b). The three factors in reading ability: Reanalysis of a study by Cunningham, Stanovich, and Wilson, Journal of Reading Behavior 24: 173–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1993). Merging the simple view of reading with rauding theory, Journal of Reading Behavior 25: 439–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1994a). Technical manual for the Computer Assisted Reading Diagnosis. Kansas City, MO: Revrac Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1994b). Manual for the Accuracy Level Test. Kanas City, MO: Revrac Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, R.P. (1997). Reading for one-second, one-minute, or one-year from the perspective of rauding theory, Scientific Studies of Reading 1: 3–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A.E. & Stanovich, K.E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience, Journal of Educational Psychology 82: 733–740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M.E. (1980). Development of components of reading skill, Journal of Educational Psychology 72: 656–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M. (1991). Individual differences in reading skills. In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal & P.D. Pearson (eds.), Handbook of reading research, Vol. II (pp. 512–538). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyer, L.G. & Katz, L. (1992). An examination of ‘the simple view of reading’, Yearbook of the National Reading Conference 41: 169–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. (1991). Development of the ability to read words. In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal & P.D. Pearson (eds.), Handbook of reading research, Vol. II (pp. 383–417). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M.A. & Carr, T.H. (1985). Cognitive abilities, conditions of learning, and the early development of reading skill, Reading Research Quarterly 20: 327–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, G.A. (1959). Statistical analysis in psychology and education. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K.E. Patterson, J.C. Marshall & M. Coltheart (eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P.B. (1972). One second of reading. In J.F. Kavanagh & I.G. Mattingly (eds.), Language by ear and by eye: The relationship between speech and reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P.B. & Tunmer, W.E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability, Remedial and Special Education 7: 6–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W.A. & Gough, P.B. (1990). The simple view of reading, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 127–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastak, J.F. & Jastak, S. (1978). Manual for the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised Edition. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of fifty-four children from first through fourth grade, Journal of Educational Psychology 80: 437–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P.L. & Gough, P.B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade, Journal of Educational Psychology 78: 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Wise, B., Connors, F., Rack, J. & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influence, Journal of Learning Disabilities 22: 339–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1987). Language, speech, and print: Some asymmetries in the acquisition of literacy. In Horowitz, R. & Samuels, S.J. (eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 355–369). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra, G.M. (1990). Convergence of listening and reading processing, Reading Research Quarterly 25: 115–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1991). Word recognition: Changing perspectives. In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal & P.D. Pearson (eds.), Handbook of reading research, Vol. II, New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E., Nathan, R.G. & Zolman, J.E. (1988). The developmental lag hypothesis in reading: Longitudinal and matched reading-level comparison, Child Development 59: 71–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S.S. (1951). Mathematics, measurement, and psychophysics. In S.S. Stevens (ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 1–49). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sticht, T.G., Beck, L.J., Hauke, R.N., Kleiman, G.M. & James, J.H. (1974). Auding and reading: A development model. Alexandria, VA: Human Resources Research Organization.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. & Lorge, I. (1944). Teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Teachers College Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F.R. (1991). Introduction to three studies on reading acquisition: Convergent findings on theoretical foundations of code-oriented versus whole-language approaches to reading instruction, Journal of Educational Psychology 83: 437–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M., Small, S.G. & Tanzman, M.S. (1991). The linguistic bases of reading ability: Converting written to oral language, Text 11: 99–133.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carver, R.P. Predicting reading level in grades 1 to 6 from listening level and decoding level: Testing theory relevant to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing 10, 121–154 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007923124312

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007923124312

Navigation