Skip to main content
Log in

Trait Anxiety and Attention: Cognitive Functioning as a Function of Attentional Demands

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the relationship between trait anxiety and cognitive functioning, specifically response inhibition and conflict resolution, by comparing attention switching and sustained attention conditions in a dichotic-listening to words task. Results showed that high- as compared to low-anxiety participants had a lower hit rate in both attention conditions, a lower intrusion rate in the sustained attention condition, and greater difficulty shifting attention in the mixed condition. Furthermore, laterality-related findings revealed that high-anxiety participants had a lower hit rate when attention was directed to the left-ear (right hemisphere) and less intrusions when attention was directed right-ear (left hemisphere) than did the low-anxiety participants. The findings are interpreted based on attentional control and load theories as well as on the attentional model of hemisphere asymmetry, supporting the proposition that high anxiety is associated with an imbalance between bottom-up and top-down processes, and that anxiety may affect cognitive control under high cognitive load conditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ansari, T. L., & Derakshan, N. (2011). The neural correlates of impaired inhibitory control in anxiety. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1146–1153.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asbjørnsen, A. E., & Bryden, M. P. (1998). Auditory attentional shifts in reading-disabled students: Quantification of attentional effectiveness by the Attentional Shift Index. Neuropsychologia, 36, 143–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24.

  • Barkley, R. A. (1999). Response inhibition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 5, 177–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basten, U., Stelzel, C., & Fiebach, C. J. (2011). Trait anxiety modulates the neural efficiency of inhibitory control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3132–3145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Basten, U., Stelzel, C., & Fiebach, C. J. (2012). Trait anxiety and the neural efficiency of manipulation in working memory. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 571–588.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berggren, N., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: Why you see them and why you don’t. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 440–446.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 307–316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 92–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruder, G. E., Schneier, F. R., Stewart, J. W., McGrath, P. J., & Quitkin, F. (2004). Left hemisphere dysfunction during verbal dichotic listening tests in patients who have social phobia with or without comorbid depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(1), 72–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P., & MacRae, L. (1988). Dichotic laterality effects obtained with emotional words. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 1, 171–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P., Munhall, K., & Allard, F. (1983). Attentional biases and the right-ear effect in dichotic listening. Brain and Language, 18, 236–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carretié, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Albert, J., López-Martín, S., De La Gándara, B. S., Igoa, J. M., & Sotillo, M. (2008). Modulation of ongoing cognitive processes by emotionally intense words. Psychophysiology, 45, 188–196.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C., & Häberling, I. S. (2017). The Many Sides of Hemispheric Asymmetry: A Selective Review and Outlook. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(9–10), 710–718.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3, 201–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Demaree, H. A., Everhart, D. E., Youngstrom, E. A., & Harrison, D. W. (2005). Brain lateralization of emotional processing: Historical roots and a future incorporating “dominance”. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4, 3–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., Ansari, T. L., Hansard, M., Shoker, L., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009a). Anxiety, inhibition, efficiency, and effectiveness: An investigation using the antisaccade task. Experimental Psychology, 56, 48–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., Smyth, S., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009b). Effects of state anxiety on performance using a task-switching paradigm: An investigation of attentional control theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1112–1117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: Trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 745–761.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225–236.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donner, N. C., & Lowry, C. A. (2013). Sex differences in anxiety and emotional behavior. Pflügers Archiv / European Journal of Physiology, 465, 601–626.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, M. S., Moore, P., Champion, J. C., & Edwards, E. J. (2015). Effects of trait anxiety and situational stress on attentional shifting are buffered by working memory capacity. Anxiety. Stress and Coping, 28, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engels, A. S., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., Banich, M. T., Webb, A. G., & Miller, G. A. (2007). Specificity of regional brain activity in anxiety types during emotion processing. Psychophysiology, 44(3), 352–363.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fales, C. L., Barch, D. M., Burgess, G. C., Schaefer, A., Mennin, D. S., Gray, J. R., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: Differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 239–253.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fougnie, D. (2008). The relationship between attention and working memory. In N. B, Johansen (Ed.) New Research on Short-Term Memory (pp. 1–45). Nova Science Publishers, Inc,

  • Fox, E. (1993). Attentional bias in anxiety: Selective or not? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 487–493.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1994). Attentional bias in anxiety: A defective inhibition hypothesis. Cognition & Emotion, 8, 165–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., & Georgiou, G. A. (2005). Anxiety modulates the degree of attentive resources required to process emotional faces. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 396–404.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gootjes, L., Bouma, A., Van Strien, J. W., Van Schijndel, R., Barkhof, F., & Scheltens, P. (2006). Corpus callosum size correlates with asymmetric performance on a dichotic listening task in healthy aging but not in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 44, 208–217.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, D.M., & White, E.J. (2016). Influence of Anxiety on Cognitive Control Processes. The Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology. Oxford University Press, USA.

  • Grimshaw, G. M., Kwassny, K. M., Covell, E., & Johnson, R. A. (2003). The dynamic nature of language lateralization: Effects of lexical and prosodic factors. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1008–1019.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gul, A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). The Relationship between Anxiety and Task Switching Ability. Global Journal of Human-Social Science Research, 14, 27–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, W., & Nitscke, J. B. (1998). The puzzle of regional brain activity in depression and anxiety: The importance of subtypes and comorbidity. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 421–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, W., Nitschke, J. B., Etienne, M. A., & Miller, G. A. (1997). Patterns of regional brain activity differentiate types of anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 376–385.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hugdahl, K. (1995). Dichotic listening: Probing temporal lobe functional integrity. In R. J. Davidson & K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain asymmetry (pp. 123–156). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hugdahl, K. (2003). Dichotic listening in the study of auditory laterality. In K. Hugdahl & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 441–475). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hugdahl, K. (2005). Symmetry and asymmetry in the human brain. European Review, 13, 119–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hugdahl, K., & Andersson, L. (1986). The “forced-attention paradigm” in dichotic listening to CV-syllables: A comparison between adults and children. Cortex, 22, 417–432.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hugdahl, K., Law, I., Kyllingsbaek, S., Bronnick, K., Gade, A., & Paulson, O. B. (2000). Effects of attention on dichotic listening: An 15O-PET study. Human Brain Mapping, 10, 87–97.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hugdahl, K., Westerhausen, R., Alho, K., Medvedev, S., Laine, M., & Hämäläinen, H. (2009). Attention and cognitive control: Unfolding the dichotic listening story. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 11–22.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hugdahl, K., Løberg, E. M., Falkenberg, L. E., Johnsen, E., Kompus, K., Kroken, R. A., Nygård, M., Westerhausen, R., Alptekin, K., & Özgören, M. (2012). Auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia as aberrant lateralized speech perception: Evidence from dichotic listening. Schizophrenia Research, 140, 59–64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jerger, J., & Martin, J. (2006). Dichotic listening tests in the assessment of auditory processing disorders. Audiological Medicine, 4, 25–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kallman, H. J., & Corballis, M. C. (1975). Ear asymmetry in reaction time to musical sounds. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 17, 368–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenemans, J. L., Bekker, E. M., Lijffijt, M., Overtoom, C. C., Jonkman, L. M., & Verbaten, M. N. (2005). Attention deficit and impulsivity: Selecting, shifting, and stopping. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 58, 59–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1970). The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychologica, 33, 193–201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., & Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of visual selective attention: Efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8, 395–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leshem, R. (2013). The effects of attention on ear advantages in dichotic listening to words and affects. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 932–940.

    Google Scholar 

  • Løberg, E. M., Jørgensen, H. A., & Hugdahl, K. (2004). Dichotic listening in schizophrenic patients: Effects of previous vs. ongoing auditory hallucinations. Psychiatry Research, 128, 167–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 571–598.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Øie, M., & Hugdahl, K. (2008). A 10–13 year follow-up of changes in perception and executive attention in patients with early-onset schizophrenia: A dichotic listening study. Schizophrenia Research, 106, 29–32.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). Assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Lupiáñez, J., Román, N., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Response inhibition and attentional control in anxiety. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 646–660.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Pascalis, V., Arwari, B., D’Antuono, L., & Cacace, I. (2009). Impulsivity and semantic/emotional processing: An examination of the N400 wave. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 85–92.

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poy, R., del Carmen Eixarch, M., & Ávila, C. (2004). On the relationship between attention and personality: Covert visual orienting of attention in anxiety and impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1471–1481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Righi, S., Mecacci, L., & Viggiano, M. P. (2009). Anxiety, cognitive self-evaluation and performance: ERP correlates. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1132–1138.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2005). Modeling task switching without switching tasks: A short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime computer software and manual. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools.

  • Scott, G. G., O’Donnell, P. J., Leuthold, H., & Sereno, S. C. (2009). Early emotion word processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 80, 95–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, I.E.C. (2010). Sex differences in handedness, brain asymmetry, and language lateralization. In: Hugdahl, K, Westerhausen, R. (eds). The two halves of the brain: Information processing in the cerebral hemispheres. The MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 287 – 312.

  • Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (form Y). Palo Alto; Consulting Psychologist Press.

  • Springer, S., & Deutsch, G. (1997). Left brain, right brain, 5th edn. Freeman, New York.

  • Stout, D. M., Shackman, A. J., & Larson, C. L. (2013). Failure to filter: Anxious individuals show inefficient gating of threat from working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 58.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. M. (1981). Lateral brain function, emotion, and conceptualization. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 19–46.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. M., Antes, J. R., Stenslie, C. E., & Barnhardt, T. M. (1978). Anxiety and lateral cerebral function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 380.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vytal, K., Cornwell, B., Arkin, N., & Grillon, C. (2012). Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: From impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load. Psychophysiology, 49, 842–852.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vytal, K. E., Cornwell, B. R., Letkiewicz, A. M., Arkin, N. E., & Grillon, C. (2013). The complex interaction between anxiety and cognition: Insight from spatial and verbal working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.

  • Wadnerkar, M. B., Whiteside, S. P., & Cowell, P. E. (2008). Dichotic listening asymmetry: Sex differences and menstrual cycle effects. Laterality, 13, 297–309.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wager, T. D., Jonides, J., & Reading, S. (2004). Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: A metaanalysis. NeuroImage, 22, 1679–1693.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westerhausen, R, & Hugdahl, K. (2010). Cognitive control of auditory laterality. In: K. Hugdahl & R. Westerhausen (eds). The two Halves of the brain (pp 469–498). The MIT Press, Cambridge.

  • Westerhausen, R., Moosmann, M., Alho, K., Medvedev, S., Hamalainen, H., & Hugdahl, K. (2009). Top-down and bottom-up interaction: Manipulating the dichotic listening ear advantage. Brain Research, 1250, 183–189.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J., Mathews, A., & Dalgleish, T. (2001). Anxiety and cognitive inhibition. Emotion, 1(2), 166–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by an EU Marie-Curie International Fellowship PIOF-GA-2009-236183 to Rotem Leshem.

This study was conducted while the author was a post-doc fellow in Eran Zaidel’s Cognitive Neuroscience lab, in the Psychology Department at UCLA. I wish to thank him deeply for his mentorship and support, especially in the conceptualization of this study.

Funding

This study was funded by EU Marie-Curie International Fellowship (PIOF-GA-2009-236183).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rotem Leshem.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in this study.

Conflict of Interest

The author, Dr. Rotem Leshem declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Leshem, R. Trait Anxiety and Attention: Cognitive Functioning as a Function of Attentional Demands. Curr Psychol 39, 1830–1842 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9884-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9884-9

Keywords

Navigation