Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1109, Issue 1, 13 September 2006, Pages 93-107
Brain Research

Research Report
Neuroanatomical localization of an internal clock: A functional link between mesolimbic, nigrostriatal, and mesocortical dopaminergic systems

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.031Get rights and content

Abstract

The effects of selective dopamine (DA) depleting lesions with 6-hydroxydopamine microinjection into the SN, CPu, and NAS, as well as radiofrequency lesions of the CPu on the performance characteristics of rats trained on a single-valued 20-s peak-interval (PI) timing procedure or a double-valued 10-s and 60-s PI procedure were evaluated. A double dissociation in the performance of duration discriminations was found. Rats with CPu lesions were unable to exhibit temporal control of their behavior suggesting complete insensitivity to signal duration but were able to show discrimination of the relative reward value of a signal by differentially modifying their response rates appropriately. In contrast, rats with NAS lesions were able to exhibit temporal control of their behavior by differentially modifying their response rates as a function of signal duration(s), suggesting no impairment of sensitivity to signal duration, but were unable to show discrimination of the relative reward value of a signal.

Introduction

The neural pathways essential for the behavioral effects mediated by the neurotransmitter dopamine (DA) can be divided into three major categories based on the locations of the DA cells bodies in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and substantia nigra (SN) and their sites of projection. These target sites are primarily composed of the nucleus accumbens (NAS), the prefrontal cortex (PFC), and the caudate–putamen (CPu)—which have been termed the mesolimbic, mesocortical, and nigrostriatal DA systems, respectively. Historically, the nigrostriatal DA system has been considered to play a major role in motor function (Marsden, 1982), whereas the mesocortical and mesolimbic DA systems have been associated with working memory and the quantification of reward, respectively (e.g., Abi-Dargham et al., 2002, Au-Young et al., 1999, Kelley and Berridge, 2002, Nicola et al., 2000, Phillips et al., 2004, Schultz et al., 1997, Smith-Roe and Kelley, 2000). The view that the nigrostriatal DA system is exclusively involved in motor function, however, has evolved over time and it is now generally accepted that it also contributes to attentional set switching, error prediction regarding the occurrence of rewards, and timing and time perception (e.g., Dreher and Grafman, 2002, Gibbon et al., 1997, Meck and Benson, 2002, Middleton and Strick, 2000, Pastor et al., 1992).

Of particular interest is the observation that the CPu and NAS receive dense dopaminergic innervation from mesencephalic A9 and A10 neurons (e.g., Moore et al., 1998, Ungerstedt, 1971), and pharmacologically distinct DA receptor subtypes (e.g., D1 and D2) have been identified that mediate different behaviors (e.g., Bunney, 1979, Cox and Waszczak, 1990, Dawson et al., 1985, Fowler et al., 1984, Kebabian and Calne, 1979, LaHoste and Marshall, 1990, Robertson and Robertson, 1987, Walters et al., 1987, White, 1987). In rats, discrete stimulation or blockade of telencephalic DA receptors by intracerebral administration of DA agonists or antagonists mimics the effects of these agents when given peripherally. For example, apomorphine induces stereotyped motor behavior whether it is injected systemically or placed directly into the CPu (e.g., Giménez-Llort et al., 2005, Ungerstedt et al., 1969) and rats will self-administer amphetamine either systemically or directly into the NAS (e.g., Hoebel et al., 1983, Ikemoto et al., 2005).

The large-scale inactivation of DA function with DA receptor antagonists and midbrain neurotoxin lesions results in a behavioral syndrome that is very similar to Parkinson's disease in man. Rats show akinesia, catalepsy, and are impaired in performance in a variety of behavioral situations that may involve the timing of stimulus events (Fibiger et al., 1974, Gibbon et al., 1997, Koob et al., 1984). More specific impairments of DA systems induced by microinjection of DA receptor antagonists or by neurotoxin terminal lesions have resulted in differential effects on behavior depending on the locus of the DA system and the type of behavior studied. For example, depletion of DA in the CPu, but not in the NAS, impairs reaction-time performance in a simple reaction-time task (e.g., Amalric and Koob, 1987, MacDonald and Meck, 2004), impairs a conditional discrimination of temporal frequency (Robbins et al., 1990), and impairs simple stimulus–response (S-R) associations in a variety of tasks, including the conditioned emotional response paradigm (White, 1989a). In contrast, neurotoxin-induced lesions of DA neurons in the NAS, but not the CPu, attenuates amphetamine self-administration and the enhanced responding to reward-related stimuli produced by intra-accumbens amphetamine injections (Lyness et al., 1979, Taylor and Robbins, 1986) as well as the motor stimulation induced by this drug (Kelly and Iversen, 1976). Furthermore, the differential behavioral effects of amphetamine microinjections into striatal sub-regions have been extensively mapped, suggesting that the NAS, but not the CPu, is the primary site underlying the registration of reward (Kelley and Delfs, 1991). It has also been demonstrated that NMDA glutamate receptors and dopamine D1 and D2 receptors interact in the regulation of signal transduction and induction of transcription factors in the basal ganglia for the expression of behavior (e.g., Radulovic et al., 2000).

One hypothesis that has been suggested to explain this overall pattern of results is that the ventral striatum (subsuming the NAS) is implicated in the processes of incentive motivation and the determination of the rewarding (approach-eliciting) properties of a stimulus, both of which contribute to associative measures of response strength (e.g., Robbins et al., 1990, White, 1989b); whereas the dorsal striatum (subsuming the CPu) is implicated in processes of sensorimotor integration, as exemplified by simple S-R associations that are necessary for planning, initiating, and coordinating a conditioned movement (real or imaginary) to a sensory cue (Amalric and Koob, 1987, Graybiel, 1990, Robbins and Everitt, 1992, Robbins et al., 1990, White, 1989a).

In order to investigate the contributions of these interacting DA systems to interval-timing behavior, a set of experimental procedures have been developed that can be used to separate the different psychological processes involved in timing and time perception (Church, 1984, Church, 1989, Meck and Church, 1982, Meck et al., 1987, Paule et al., 1999, Penney et al., 1996, Penney et al., 2000). For example, in the basic discrete-trial fixed-interval (FI) procedure, each trial begins with a signal onset. The animal is free to respond at any time during the trial, but only the first response after a trained temporal criterion (e.g., 20 s) is reinforced. The peak-interval (PI) procedure, a variation of the FI procedure, provides independent measures of several processes involved in duration discriminations (e.g., Church et al., 1991, Church et al., 1994, Miller et al., 2006, Rakitin et al., 1998, Roberts, 1981). Reinforced trials are identical to those of the FI procedure. For other probe trials, however, the signal continues much longer than the trained temporal criterion, and no reinforcement is made available. During these non-reinforced probe trials, the animal's response rate increases as a function of the signal duration until a point near the time that reinforcement is available in the FI procedure, when it decreases in a fairly symmetrical manner when plotted on a linear time scale. The mode of the Gaussian-shaped response rate function is called the peak time and it represents the time during the signal presentation that animals maximally expect reinforcement to be made available. The response rate at that time is called the peak rate and it represents the subject's level of motivation and/or the incentive value of a particular signal. Peak time and peak rate are considered to be independent measures to the extent that treatments may affect one of these measures without affecting the other (Roberts, 1981). A discrimination index (DI) can also be calculated by dividing the peak or maximal response rate by the mean response rate. This measure provides a measure of the animal's sensitivity to signal duration, values near 1.0 would indicate that the subject was highly insensitive to the temporal properties of the signal, perhaps indicating that the mechanism(s) responsible for timing had been damaged.

Although there exists considerable information on the function of the central dopaminergic projections in motor and motivational processes, only recently has research been directed to the exploration of the specific roles of DA and glutamate in timing and time perception (e.g., Abner et al., 2001, Buhusi, 2003, Buhusi and Meck, 2002, Cevik, 2003a, Cheng et al., in press, Cevik, 2003b, Drew et al., 2003, Gibbon et al., 1997, Maricq and Church, 1983, Matell et al., 2003, Matell et al., 2006, Matell et al., in press, Meck, 1983, Meck, 1986, Meck, 1988a, Meck, 1988b, Meck, 1996, Meck, 2003, Meck and Benson, 2002, Miller et al., 2006, Neil and Herndon, 1978, Ohyama et al., 2000, Rammsayer, 2006, Lustig and Meck, 2005, MacDonald and Meck, 2005). These questions, apart from their intrinsic importance, are currently of considerable interest, given the growing evidence of cognitive deficits in Huntington's disease and Parkinson's disease (e.g., Canavan et al., 1989, Harrington and Haaland, 1999, Lawrence et al., 1998), which are closely related to neuronal damage in the striatum and substantia nigra, respectively. Of particular interest are the observations that when Parkinson's disease patients are trained to perform in PI timing procedures developed for human participants (e.g., Rakitin et al., 1998), they show evidence of temporal memory dysfunctions and a slowing of time perception when tested off of their levodopa (l-DOPA) medication. In contrast, the same PD patients exhibit relatively normal temporal cognition when tested on their l-DOPA medication suggesting an important role of SNc DA neurons in timing and time perception in the seconds to minutes range (e.g., Malapani et al., 1998, Malapani et al., 2002).

In order to investigate the role of DA pathways in timing behavior, the present study examined the behavioral effects of DA depletion in rats produced by radiofrequency or 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA) neurotoxin lesions of the CPu, NAS, or SN on duration discriminations trained using the PI timing procedure. In addition, we examined whether l-DOPA administration would selectively restore interval-timing behavior to rats with CPu or SN lesions.

Section snippets

Preoperative peak-interval training (Sessions 1–30)

During PI baseline training, the peak time, peak rate, and DI measures were similar for all groups, with no significant differences observed in any of these measures. The average peak time (mean ± SEM) for rats in the CON group was 19.95 ± 0.67 s, in the CPU group it was 20 ± 0.61 s, and in the SN group it was 20.65 ± 0.27 s. The average maximum response rate (mean ± SEM) for rats in the CON group was 69 ± 1.84 responses/min, in the CPU group it was 65.2 ± 1.94 responses/min, and in the SN group it was 70.01 ±

Discussion

Functional dissociations between the mesolimbic, nigrostriatal, and mesocortical dopaminergic systems were examined using 6-OHDA lesions of the SN, CPu, or NAS, as well as radiofrequency lesions of the CPu. Rats were trained in PI timing procedures with either a single temporal criteria (20 s) or multiple temporal criteria (10 s and 60 s) and a double dissociation in the performance of these duration discriminations was found as a function of the brain area lesioned. Rats with CPu lesions were

Animals and housing

Experimentally naive Sprague–Dawley male rats (n = 60, Charles River VAF plus) approximately 400 g in weight and 6 months in age when the experiments began. Rats were allowed continuous access to water in their home cages and were fed a daily ration of about 15 g of rat chow shortly after the experimental session. A light–dark (LD) cycle of 12:12 was maintained in the vivarium with fluorescent lights on at 6:00 AM.

Apparatus

All experimental data were obtained in 8 operant chambers (Coulbourn Instruments,

Acknowledgments

Warren H. Meck, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University. The author would like to thank Matt Matell and Chris MacDonald for their extensive contributions to this project. This research was supported, in part, by fellowships from the James McKeen Cattell Fund and the CNRS, France.

References (154)

  • A.-M. Ferrandez et al.

    Basal ganglia and supplementary motor area subtend duration perception: an fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • M.M. Ferro et al.

    Comparison of bilaterally 6-OHDA- and MPTP-lesioned rats as models of the early phase of Parkinson's disease: histological, neurochemical, motor and memory alterations

    J. Neurosci. Methods

    (2005)
  • H.C. Fibiger et al.

    Deficits in instrumental responding after 6-hydroxydopamine lesion of the nigro-neostriatal dopaminergic projection

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1974)
  • L. Giménez-Llort et al.

    Two distinctive apomorphine-induced phenotypes in the Roman high- and low-avoidance rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2005)
  • A.M. Graybiel

    Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators in the basal ganglia

    TINS

    (1990)
  • S.C. Hinton et al.

    Frontal-striatal circuitry activated by human peak-interval timing in the supra-seconds range

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • P.H. Kelly et al.

    Selective 6-OHDA induced destruction of medolimbic dopamine neurons: abolition of psychostimulant-induced locomotor activity in rats

    Eur. J. Pharmacol.

    (1976)
  • G.F. Koob et al.

    Neuroleptic-like disruption of the conditioned avoidance response requires destruction of both the mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine systems

    Brain Res.

    (1984)
  • G.J. LaHoste et al.

    Nigral D1 and striatal D2 receptors mediate the behavioral effects of dopamine agonists

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (1990)
  • C. Lustig et al.

    Chronic treatment with haloperidol induces working memory deficits in feedback effects of interval timing

    Brain Cogn.

    (2005)
  • W.H. Lyness et al.

    Destruction of dopaminergic nerve terminals in nucleus accumbens: effects of d-amphetamine self-stimulation

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1979)
  • C.J. MacDonald et al.

    Systems-level integration of interval timing and reaction time

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2004)
  • M.S. Matell et al.

    Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence-detection of oscillatory processes

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • M.S. Matell et al.

    Dopamine D1 activation shortens the duration of phases in stereotyped grooming sequences

    Behav. Processes

    (2006)
  • W.H. Meck

    Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1986)
  • W.H. Meck

    Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • W.H. Meck

    Neuropsychology of timing and time perception

    Brain Cogn.

    (2005)
  • W.H. Meck et al.

    Dissecting the brain's internal clock: how frontal-striatal circuitry keeps time and shifts attention

    Brain Cogn.

    (2002)
  • W.H. Meck et al.

    Neuroimaging of interval timing

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • W.H. Meck et al.

    Un modèle neurobiologique de la perception et de l'estimation du temps

    Psychol. Fr.

    (2005)
  • A. Abi-Dargham et al.

    Prefrontal dopamine D1 receptors and working memory in schizophrenia

    J. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • R.T. Abner et al.

    Pharmacology of temporal cognition in two mouse strains

    Int. J. Comp. Psychol.

    (2001)
  • J.W. Aldridge et al.

    Coding of serial order by neostriatal neurons: a “natural action” approach to movement sequence

    J. Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • J.W. Aldridge et al.

    Neuronal coding of serial order: syntax of grooming in the neostriatum

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1993)
  • M. Amalric et al.

    Depletion of dopamine in the caudate nucleus but not in nucleus accumbens impairs reaction-time performance in rats

    J. Neurosci.

    (1987)
  • S.M. Au-Young et al.

    Medial prefrontal cortical output neurons to the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and their responses to burst-patterned stimulation of the VTA: neuroanatomical and in vivo electrophysiological analyses

    Synapse

    (1999)
  • K.C. Berridge

    Substantia nigra 6-OHDA lesions mimic striatopallidal disruption of syntactic grooming chains: a neural systems analysis of sequence control

    Psychobiology

    (1989)
  • K.C. Berridge et al.

    Disruption of natural grooming chains after striatopallidal lesions

    Psychobiology

    (1987)
  • K.C. Berridge et al.

    Cortex, striatum and cerebellum: control of serial order in a grooming sequence

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1992)
  • J.P. Bruno et al.

    Age-dependent neurobehavioral plasticity following forebrain dopamine depletions

    Dev. Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • C.V. Buhusi

    Dopaminergic mechanisms of interval timing and attention

  • C.V. Buhusi et al.

    Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • C.V. Buhusi et al.

    What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing

    Nat. Rev., Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • B.S. Bunney

    The electrophysiological pharmacology of midbrain dopaminergic systems

  • J.J. Canales et al.

    A measure of striatal function predicts motor stereotypy

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • M.B. Carew et al.

    Multiple functions of context during conditioning: a developmental analysis

    Dev. Psychobiol.

    (1991)
  • D. Centonze et al.

    Unilateral dopamine denervation blocks corticostriatal LTP

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1999)
  • M.O. Cevik

    Effects of methamphetamine on duration discrimination

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • M.O. Cevik

    Neurogenetics of interval timing

  • R.K. Cheng et al.

    α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors and temporal memory: synergistic effects of combining prenatal choline and nicotine on reinforcement-induced resetting of an interval clock

    Learn. Mem.

    (2006)
  • Cited by (262)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text