Skip to main content
Log in

Early construction of number as position with young children: a teaching experiment

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Our study is motivated by the importance of the number line in the visualisation of elementary properties of operations and numbers. For the number line to be an aid for visualisation, first it must be conceived as a coordinate system (origin, orientation and unit). This paper focuses on the construction of the concept of position on a line, and on the number as a means of remembering position, for pre-elementary school children aged 5–6 years. Using didactic design as a methodology, we conducted an experiment to observe how knowledge was constructed while carrying out a certain kind of task in a situation where position had to be designated. Our study shows that pupils, despite recognising number as a quantity, fail to apprehend number as something which represents position. They are, however, capable of understanding the characteristic elements of the number line in suitable situations. Our research suggests that it is possible to teach the use of ordinal numbers to young pupils.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14
Fig. 15
Fig. 16
Fig. 17
Fig. 18
Fig. 19
Fig. 20
Fig. 21
Fig. 22
Fig. 23
Fig. 24
Fig. 25
Fig. 26
Fig. 27
Fig. 28
Fig. 29
Fig. 30

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the rest of this paper we write ‘coordinate system’ but here we are dealing only with one dimension. We are aware that in English it is sometimes called a ‘number line’ but this term is not suitable for our paper, since we have to consider the question of the coordinate system (“repère” in French) and the use of numbers separately.

  2. http://www.education.gouv.fr/bo/2008/hs3/programme_maternelle.htm (Accessed 10 Oct 2013).

  3. For this figure, as for those that follow, we use the following key: A1 refers to class A, group 1 (customary mixed groups in each class, not corresponding to an academic level), the pupil’s first name, phase number; necklace 7 means that the coloured bead which the pupil had to reproduce was in 7th position from the knot. Since there is no colour in the printed version, the arrow means that one bead is different from the others.

  4. The only case which is not represented is the presence of the knot with the absence of the representation of the end of the necklace and the verticality.

References

  • Ainley, J. (2008). Task design based on Purpose and Utility. ICMI 11. http://tsg.icme11.org/document/get/291 (Accessed 10 Oct 2013).

  • Arcavi, A. (1999). The role of visual representations in the learning of mathematics. 23rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Haifa Israel.

  • Artigue, M. (1992). Didactique engineering. In R. Douady & A. Mercier (Eds.), Research in didactique of mathematics (pp. 41–66). Grenoble: La Pensée Sauvage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Artigue, M., & Perrin-Glorian, M.-J. (1991). Didactic engineering, research and development tool: Some theoretical problems linked to this duality. For the Learning of Mathematics, 11(1), 3–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, M., & Chevallard, Y. (1999). La sensibilité de l’activité mathématique aux ostensifs. Recherches en Didactique des Mathématiques, 19(1), 77–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brizuela, B. M., & Cayton, G. A. (2008). The role of punctuation marks while learning about written numbers. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 68, 209–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brousseau, G. (1997). Theory of didactical situations in mathematics. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brousseau, G. (2008). Notes on the observation of classroom practices. ICMI 11. http://tsg.icme11.org/document/get/315 (Accessed 10 Oct 2013).

  • Bruce, B., & Threlfall, J. (2004). One, two, three and counting. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 55, 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallard, Y. (2012). Teaching mathematics in tomorrow’s society: A case for an oncoming counterparadigm. 12th International Congress on Mathematical Education, Seoul, Korea. http://www.icme12.org/upload/submission/1985_F.pdf (Accessed 10 Oct 2013).

  • Comiti, C. (1980). Les premières acquisitions de la notion de nombre par l’enfant. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11, 301–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikos, C., Szitànyi, J., & Kelemen, R. (2012). The effects of using drawings in developing young children’s mathematical word problem solving: A design experiment with third-grade Hungarian students. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 81, 47–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douady, R. (1991). Tool, Object, Setting, Window Elements for analysing and constructing didactical situations in mathematics. In A. Bishop, S. Mellin-Olsen, & J. van Dormolen (Eds.), Knowledge: Its growth through teaching. Dordrecht: Klüwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duval, R. (1999). Representation, vision and visualization: Cognitive functions in mathematical thinking, basic issues for learning. 23rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Haifa, Israel.

  • Ernest, P. (1985). The number line as a teaching aid. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 16, 411–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C. (1983). Children’s counting and concepts of number. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Margolinas, C., Abboud-Blanchard, M., Bueno-Ravel, L., Douek, N., Fluckiger, A., Gibel, P., et al. (Eds.). (2011). En amont et en aval des ingénieries didactiques. Grenoble: La Pensée Sauvage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Margolinas, C., & Wozniak, F. (2012). Le nombre à l’école maternelle. Une approche didactique. Bruxelles: De Boeck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Szeminska, A. (1941). La genèse du nombre chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel Delachaux & Niestlé.

  • Rivera, F. (2010). Toward a visually-oriented school mathematics curriculum: Research, theory, practice, and issues. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sierpinska, A. (2003). Research in mathematics education: Through a keyhole. In E. Simmt & B. Davis (Eds.), Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of Canadian Mathematics Education Study Group. Wolfville, Nova Scotia: Acadia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinbring, H. (2006). What makes a sign a Mathematical Sign?—An epistemological perspective on mathematical interaction. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 61, 133–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsamir, P., Tirosh, D., Tabach, M., & Levenson, E. (2010). Multiple solution methods and multiple outcomes—is it a task for kindergarten children? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 73, 217–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urbanska, A. (1993). On the numerical competence of six-years-old children. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 24, 265–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (2008). Learning from ‘Didactikids’: An impetus for revisiting the empty number line. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 20(3), 6–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitney, H. (1985). Taking responsibility in school mathematics education. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 4(3), 219–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittman, E. (1995). Mathematics education as a ‘design science’. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 29(4), 355–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, F. (2013). Instrumental value and semiotic value of ostensives and task design. In C. Margolinas (Ed.), Task Design in Mathematics Education, Proceedings of ICME 22 (pp. 141–152). http://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00834054 (Accessed 10 Oct 2013).

  • Wright, R. J. (1994). A study of the numerical development of 5-year-olds and 6-year-olds. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 25–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claire Margolinas.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Margolinas, C., Wozniak, F. Early construction of number as position with young children: a teaching experiment. ZDM Mathematics Education 46, 29–44 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-013-0554-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-013-0554-y

Keywords

Navigation