Prefrontal and Accumbal Electric Activity during Auditory Stimulation in Virgin Female Rats: Changes Related to the Estrous Cycle

Abstract

The estrous cycle is a physiological process modulated by hormonal changes associated, in turn, with alterations of the attraction or sensitivity with which females perceive different sensory stimuli. Auditory stimuli play an important role in the social interaction of several mammals, including their sexual behavior and the mother-young relationship. Since the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and nucleus accumbens (Acc) had been associated with the processing and assignation of the incentive value of stimuli, this study was designed to analyze whether electroencephalographic (EEG) activity in the PFC and Acc was modified during the two antagonist phases of the estrous cycle (proestrus-estrus and diestrus) when female rats heard sound recordings with different types of cries emitted by other rats. EEGs were recorded in eight female virgin Wistar rats, 80-100 days old, bilaterally implanted in the PFC and Acc during an awake-quiet state, in three conditions: when hearing 1) cries of pups; 2) cries of adult male rats; and, 3) cries of adult female rats. The cries were recorded from pups (2-10 days old) and male and female adult rats after gentle prodding by a human hand. Each auditory stimulus (which included vocalizations up to 22 kHz) was randomly played back through a speaker placed near the female rats. Only upon perceiving the cries of male rats did the females in the proestrus-estrous phase show a lower relative power in the 14-30 Hz band in the Acc and an increased inter-prefrontal correlation in the 8-13 Hz band. It is probable that these EEG changes are associated with the lower activation or non-anxious state that the female rat presents specifically during the proestrus-estrous phase in response to the alarm cries of adult male rats.


Share and Cite:

M. Hernández-González, D. Chapa García Abascal, M. Guevara, R. Romero Orozco, M. Almanza Sepúlveda and L. Flores Mancilla, "Prefrontal and Accumbal Electric Activity during Auditory Stimulation in Virgin Female Rats: Changes Related to the Estrous Cycle," Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, Vol. 3 No. 6, 2013, pp. 454-462. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2013.36047.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] J. R. Pietras and G. D. Moulton, “Hormonal Influences on Odor Detection in Rats: Changes Associated with the Estrous Cycle, Pseudopregnancy, Ovariectomy, and Administration of Testosterone Propionate,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 12, 1974, pp. 475-491. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(74)90125-5
[2] R. Guevara-Guzmán, B. Barrera-Mera and M. L. Weiss, “Effect of the Estrous Cycle on Olfactory Bulb Response to Vaginocervical Stimulation in the Rat: Results from Electrophysiology and Fos Immunicytochemistry Experiments,” Brain research bulletin, Vol. 44, No. 2, 1997, pp. 141-149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(97)00103-2
[3] A. Fernandez-Guasti and O. Picazo, “Changes in Burying Behavior during the Estrous Cycle: Effect of Estrogen and Progesterone,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, Vol. 17, No. 6, 1992, pp. 681-689. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(92)90027-5
[4] D. E. Gonzalez and R. P. Deis, “Maternal Behavior in Cyclic and Androgenized Female Rats. Role of Ovarian Hormones,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 38, No. 8, 1986, pp. 789-793.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(86)90044-2
[5] M. Martínez-Gómez, Y. Cruz, M. Salas, R. Hudson and P. Pacheco, “Assessing Pain Threshold in the Rat: Changes with Estrus and Time of Day,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 55, No. 4, 1994, pp. 651-657. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)90040-X
[6] M. Corsi-Cabrera, J. Juarez, M. Ponce-de-Leon, J. Ramos and P. N. Velásquez, “EEG Activity during Estral Cycle in the Rat,” Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, Vol. 83, No. 4, 1992, pp. 265-269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(92)90120-7
[7] C. M. Contreras, M. Molina, M. Saavedra and L. Martínez-Mota, “Lateral Septal Neuronal Firing Rate Increase during Proestrus-Estrus in the Rat,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 68, No. 3, 2000, pp. 279-284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00169-9
[8] G. Ehret and C. Schmid, “Reproductive Cycle-Dependent Plasticity of Perception of Acoustic Meaning in Mice,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 96, No. 3, 2009, pp. 428-433.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.11.005
[9] J. A. Miranda and R. C. Liu, “Dissecting Natural Sensory Plasticity: Hormones and Experience in a Maternal Context,” Hearing Research, Vol. 252, No. 1-2, 2009, pp. 21-28.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2009.04.014
[10] M. Haggard and J. B. Gaston, “Changes in Auditory Perception in the Menstrual Cycle,” British Journal of Audiology, Vol. 12, No. 4, 1978, pp. 105-118. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/03005367809078862
[11] D. Al-Mana, B. Ceranic, O. Djahanbakhch and L. M. Luxon, “Alteration in Auditory Function during the Ovarian Cycle,” Hearing Research, Vol. 268, No. 1-2, 2010, pp. 114-122.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2010.05.007
[12] A. Serra, L. Maiolino, C. Agnello, A. Messina and S. Caruso, “Auditory Brain Stem Response throughout the Menstrual Cycle,” The Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology, Vol. 112, No. 6, 2003, pp. 549-553.
[13] V. Walpurger, R. Pietrowsky, C. Kirschbaum and O. T. Wolf, “Effects of the Menstrual Cycle on Auditory EventRelated Potentials,” Hormones and Behavior, Vol. 46, No. 5, 2004, pp. 600-606.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.07.002
[14] D. B. Geissler and G. Ehret, “Time-Critical Integration of Formants for Perception of Communication Calls in Mice,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Vol. 99, No. 13, 2002, pp. 9021-9025. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.122606499
[15] N. Suga, “Philosophy and Stimulus Design for Neuroethology of Complex-Sound Processing,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, Vol. 336, No. 1278, 1992, pp. 423-428. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1992.0078
[16] R. J. Blanchard, K. J. Flannelly and D. C. Blanchard, “Defensive Behavior of Laboratory and Wild Rattus norvegicus,” Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol. 100, No. 2, 1986, pp. 101-107.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.100.2.101
[17] S. M. Brudzynski, F. Bihari, D. Ociepa and X. W. Fu, “Analysis of 22 kHz Ultrasonic Vocalization in Laboratory Rats: long and Short Calls,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 54, No. 2, 1993, pp. 215-221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(93)90102-L
[18] S. M. Brudzynski, P. Kehoe and M. Callahan, “Sonographic Structure of Isolationinduced Ultrasonic Calls of Rat Pups,” Developmental Psychobiology, Vol. 34, No. 3, 1999, pp. 195-204.
[19] R. J. Blanchard, E. B. Yudko, D. C. Blanchard and H. K. Taukulis, “High-Frequency (35-70 kHz) Ultrasonic Vocalizations in Rats Confronted with Anesthetized Conspecifics: Effects of Gepirone, Ethanol, and Diazepam,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, Vol. 44, No. 2, 1993, pp. 313-319.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(93)90467-8
[20] B. Knutson, J. Burgdorf and J. Panksepp, “Anticipation of Play Elicits High Frequency Ultrasonic Vocalizations in Young Rats,” Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol. 112, No. 1, 1998, pp. 65-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.112.1.65
[21] T. K. McIntosh and R. J. Barfield, “The Temporal Patterning of 40-60 kHz Ultrasonic Vocalizations and Copulation in the Rat (Rattus norvegicus),” Behavioral and Neural Biology, Vol. 29, No. 3, 1980, pp. 349-358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(80)90259-9
[22] R. J. Barfield and L. A. Geyer, “Sexual Behavior: Ultrasonic Postejaculatory Song of the Male Rat,” Science, Vol. 176, No. 4041, 1972, pp. 1349-1350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.176.4041.1349
[23] R. J. Barfield and D. A. Thomas, “The Role of Ultrasonic Vocalizations in the Regulation of Reproduction in Rats,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 474, 1986, pp. 33-43.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1986.tb27996.x
[24] R. Lore, K. Flannelly and P. Farina, “Ultrasounds Produced by Rats Accompany Intraspecific Fighting,” Aggressive Behavior, Vol. 2, No. 3, 1976, pp. 175-181.
[25] M. Portavella, A. Depaulis and M. Vergnes, “22-28 kHz Ultrasonic Vocalizations Associated with Defensive Reactions in Male Rats Do Not Result from Fear or Aversion,” Psychopharmacology, Vol. 111, No. 2, 1993, pp. 190-194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02245522
[26] D. A. Thomas, L. K. Takahashi and R. J. Barfield, “Analysis of Ultrasonic Vocalizations Emitted by Intruders during Aggressive Encounters among Rats (Rattus norvegicus),” Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol. 97, No. 3, 1983, pp. 201-206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.97.3.201
[27] R. J. Blanchard, D. C. Blanchard, R. Agullana and S. M. Weiss, “Twenty-Two kHz Alarm Cries to Presentation of a Predator, by Laboratory Rats Living in Visible Burrow Systems,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 50, No. 5, 1991, pp. 967-972. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(91)90423-L
[28] S. M. Brudzynski and D. Ociepa, “Ultrasonic Vocalization of Laboratory Rats in Response to Handling and Touch,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 52, No. 4, 1992, pp. 655-660. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(92)90393-G
[29] A. Agmo, A. Villalpando, Z. Picker and H. Fernández, “Lesions of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Sexual Behavior in the Male Rat,” Brain Research, Vol. 696, No. 1-2, 1995, pp. 177-186.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(95)00852-H
[30] Y. C. Liu, B. D. Sachs and J. D. Salamone, “Sexual Behavior in Male Rats after Radiofrequency or Dopamine-Depleting Lesions in Nucleus Accumbens,” Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, Vol. 60, No. 2, 1998, pp. 585-592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00022-7
[31] M. Hernández-González and M. A. Guevara, “Participation of the Prefrontal Cortex in the Processing of Sexual and Maternal Incentives,” In: L. LoGrassso and G. Morretti, Eds., Prefrontal Cortex: Roles, Interventions and Traumas, Nova Science Publishers, New York, 2009, pp. 115-157.
[32] K. Takenouchi, H. Nishijo, T. Uwano, R. Tamura, M. Takigawa and T. Ono, “Emotional and Behavioral Correlates of the Anterior Cingulated Cortex during Associative Learning in Rats,” Neuroscience, Vol. 93, No. 4, 1999, pp. 1271-1287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00216-X
[33] E. Jodo, Y. Suzuki and Y. Kayama, “Selective Responsiveness of Medial Prefrontal Cortex Neurons to the Meaningful Stimulus with a Low Probability of Occurrence in Rats,” Brain Research, Vol. 856, No. 1-2, 2000, pp. 68-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(99)02386-0
[34] K. Braun and G. Poeggel, “Recognition of Mother’s Voice Evokes Metabolic Activation in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Lateral Thalamus of Octodon degus Pups,” Neuroscience, Vol. 103, No. 4, 2001, pp. 861-864. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00074-4
[35] J. P. Lorberbaum, J. D. Newman, A. R. Horwitz, J. R. Dubno, R. Bruce Lydiard, M. B. Hamner, D. E. Bohning and M. S. George, “A Potential Role for Thalamocingulate Circuitry in Human Maternal Behavior,” Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 51, No. 6, 2002, pp. 431-445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01284-7
[36] M. Hernández-González, C. A. Prieto-Beracoechea, M. Arteaga-Silva and M. A. Guevara, “Different Functionality of the Medial and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex during a Sexually Motivated Task in Rats,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 90, No. 2-3, 2007, pp. 450-458.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.10.006
[37] G. Damsma, J. G. Pfaus, D. G. Wenktern, A. G. Phillips, H. C. Fibiger, “Sexual Behavior Increases Dopamine Transmission in the Nucleus Accumbens and Striatum of Male Rats: Comparison with Novelty and Locomotion,” Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 106, No. 1, 1992, pp. 181-191.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.106.1.181
[38] J. G. Pfaus, G. Damsma, G. G. Nomikos, D. G. Wenkstern, C. D. Blaha, A. G. Phillips and H. C. Fibiger, “Sexual Behavior Enhances Central Dopamine Transmission in the Male Rat,” Brain Research, Vol. 530, No. 2, 1990, pp. 345-348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(90)91309-5
[39] M. Cador, J. Taylor and T. Robbins, “Potentiation of the Effects of Reward-Related Stimuli by Dopaminergic-Dependent Mechanisms in the Nucleus Accumbens,” Psychopharmacology, Vol. 104, No. 3, 1991, pp. 377-385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02246039
[40] J. Salamone, M. Cousins and B. Snyder, “Behavioral Functions of Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine: Empirical and Conceptual Problems with the Anhedonia Hypothesis,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, Vol. 21, No. 3, 1997, pp. 341-359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00017-6
[41] L. A. Mead and C. H. Vanderwolf, “Hippocampal Electrical Activity in the Female Rat: The Estrous Cycle, Copulation, Parturition, and Pup Retrieval,” Behavioral Brain Research, Vol. 50, No. 1-2, 1992, pp. 105-113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80292-X
[42] M. Hernández-González, M. A. Guevara, M. Cervantes, G. Moralí and M. Corsi-Cabrera, “Characteristic Frequency Bands of the Cortico-Frontal EEG during the Sexual Interaction of the Male Rat as a Result of Factorial Analysis,” Journal of Physiology-Paris, Vol. 92, No. 1, 1998, pp. 43-50.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0928-4257(98)80022-3
[43] P. H. Boeijinga and F. H. Lopez da Silva, “Modulations of EEG Activity in the Entorhinal Cortex and Forebrain Olfactory Areas during Odour Sampling,” Brain Research, Vol. 478, No. 2, 1989, pp. 257-268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(89)91506-0
[44] M. Hernández-González, C. A. Prieto-Beracoechea, M. Navarro-Meza and M. A. Guevara, “Prefrontal and Tegmental Electrical Activity during Olfactory Stimulation in Virgin and Lactating Rats,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 83, No. 5, 2005, pp. 749-758. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.09.013
[45] F. Lopes da Silva, “Neural Mechanisms Underlying Brain Waves: From Neural Membranes to Networks,” Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, Vol. 79, No. 2, 1991, pp. 81-93.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(91)90044-5
[46] G. Paxinos and C. H. Watson, “The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates,” 5th Edition, Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, 2005.
[47] M. A. Guevara, J. Ramos, M. Hernández-González, H. Madera and M. Corsi-Cabrera, “CAPTUSEN: A System for the Computerized Acquisition of EEG and Event-Related Potentials,” Mexican Journal of Psychology, Vol. 17, No. 1, 2000, pp. 77-88.
[48] T. Hummel, R. Gollisch, G. Wildt and G. Kobal, “Changes in Olfactory Perception during the Menstrual Cycle,” Experientia, Vol. 47, No. 7, 1991, pp. 712-715. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01958823
[49] V. N. Murthy and E. E. Fetz, “Coherent 25 to 35 Hz Oscillations in the Sensorimotor Cortex of Awake Behaving Monkeys,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Scien-ces of the United States of America, Vol. 89, No. 12, 1992, pp. 5670-5674. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.89.12.5670
[50] J. T. Coull, “Neural Correlates of Attention and Arousal from Electrophysiology, Functional Neuroimaging and Psychopharmacology,” Progress in Neurobiology, Vol. 55, No. 4, 1998, pp. 343-361.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00011-2
[51] D. Wenkstern, J. Pfaus and H. Fibiger, “Dopamine Transmission Increases in the Nucleus Accumbens of Male Rats during Their First Exposure to Sexually Receptive Female Rats,” Brain Research, Vol. 618, No. 1, 1993, pp. 41-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(93)90426-N
[52] S. H. M. Van Goozen, N. H. Frijda, V. M. Wiegant, E. Endert and N. E. Van de Poll, “The Premenstrual Phase and Reactions to Aversive Events: A Study of Hormonal Influences on Emotionality,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, Vol. 21, No. 5, 1996, pp. 479-497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(95)00022-4
[53] J. F. Rodriguez-Sierra, J. L. Howard, G. T. Pollard and S. E. Hendriks, “Effect of Ovarian Hormones on Conflict Behavior,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, Vol. 9, No. 3, 1984, pp. 293-300.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(84)90008-8
[54] J. C. Shaw, “Correlation and Coherence Analysis of the EEG: A Selective Tutorial Review,” International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, Vol. 1, No. 3, 1984, pp. 255-266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(84)90045-X
[55] R. Kramis, C. H. Vanderwolf and B. H. Bland, “Two Types of Hippocampal Rhythmical Slow Activity in Both the Rabbit and the Rat: Relations to Behavior and Effects of Atropine, Diethyl Ether, Urethane, and Pentobarbital,” Experimental Neurology, Vol. 49, No. 1, 1975, pp. 58-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(75)90195-8
[56] B. Calvino, J. M. Besson, A. Boehrer and A. Depaulis, “Ultrasonic Vocalization (22 -28 kHz) in a Model of Chronic Pain, the Arthritic Rat: Effects of Analgesic Drugs,” Neuroreport, Vol. 7, No. 2, 1996, pp. 581-584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199601310-00049
[57] R. W. Bell, “Ultrasounds in Small Rodents: Arousal-Produced and Arousal Producing,” Developmental Psychobiology, Vol. 7, No. 1, 1974, pp. 39-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.420070107
[58] S. M. Brudzynski and E. M. Chiu, “Behavioural Responses of Laboratory Rats to Playback of 22 khz Ultrasonic Calls,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 57, No. 6, 1995, pp. 1039-1044.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(95)00003-2
[59] M. L. Nunes Mamede Rosa, M. J. Nobre, A. Ribeiro Oliveira and M. L. Brandao, “Isolation-Induced Changes in Ultrasonic Vocalization, Fear-Potentiated Startle and Prepulse Inhibition in Rats,” Neuropsychobiology, Vol. 51, No. 4, 2005, pp. 248-255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000085820
[60] C. Sanchez, “Stress-Induced Vocalization in Adult Animals. A Valid Model of Anxiety?” European Journal of Pharmacology, Vol. 463, No. 1-3, 2003, pp. 133-143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0014-2999(03)01277-9
[61] N. Takahashi, M. Kashino and N. Hironaka, “Structure of Rat Ultrasonic Vocalizations and Its Relevance to Behavior,” PLoS ONE, Vol. 5, No. 11, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014115
[62] E. M. S. Snoeren and A. Agmo, “Female Ultrasonic Vocalizations Have No Incentive Value for Male Rats,” Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 127, No. 3, 2013, pp. 439-450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032027

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.