The neurobiological mechanisms contributing to major depressive disorder (MDD) have been increasingly investigated to advance our understanding of the etiology, diagnosis, and treatment of this illness (Drevets, Price, & Furey, 2008; Savitz & Drevets, 2009). In particular, structural MRI (sMRI) and functional MRI (fMRI) have been used to identify alterations in both the volume and shape of brain regions in MDD, as well as differences in functional brain activation during a variety of emotion-processing and emotion-regulation paradigms. For example, sMRI studies have frequently reported reduced hippocampal volume in MDD (Lorenzetti, Allen, Fornito, & Yucel, 2009; McKinnon, Yucel, Nazarov, & MacQueen, 2009; Sapolsky, 2000a; Savitz & Drevets, 2009). Such reductions are found in elderly (O'Brien, Lloyd, McKeith, Gholkar, & Ferrier, 2004; Steffens et al., 2000; Steffens, McQuoid, Payne, & Potter, 2011) and middle-aged MDD populations (Bremner et al., 2000; Frodl et al., 2002; Malykhin, Carter, Seres, & Coupland, 2010), even when controlling for total brain/intracranial volume. However, results in younger populations (between the ages of 8 and 21 years) have been less consistent than those for depressed adults; some studies have shown that hippocampal volume is reduced in depressed adolescents and children after controlling for total brain/intracranial volume (Caetano et al., 2007; MacMaster & Kusumakar, 2004; MacMaster et al., 2008; Rao et al., 2010), whereas other studies did not find such hippocampal differences (MacMillan et al., 2003; Rosso et al., 2005). Such mixed findings in depressed children or adolescents may suggest that hippocampal volume reductions reflect the impact of risk factors, such as early adversity/trauma or increased stress responsivity, that may take some time to accumulate; thus, differences may be more robust and detectable in adult depression.

Alternatively, the conflicting data for hippocampal reduction in pediatric MDD may be partially explained by evidence for variability in the severity of reduced hippocampal volume across studies and across individuals. For example, reduced absolute/normalized volume of the hippocampus is greater in MDD patients with longer episode durations (Bell-McGinty et al., 2002; Colla et al., 2007; MacQueen et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2009; Sheline, Sanghavi, Mintun, & Gado, 1999; Sheline, Wang, Gado, Csernansky, & Vannier, 1996), more/severe symptoms (Colla et al., 2007; MacQueen et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2009; Vakili et al., 2000), early-life adversity (Rao et al., 2010), poorer response to antidepressant treatment (Sheline, Gado, & Kraemer, 2003; Vakili et al., 2000), or more severe MDD-related cognitive dysfunction (Frodl et al., 2006). Such results raise the important question of whether individual differences in volume in the hippocampus are also associated with other neurobiological characteristics of MDD, such as alterations in functional brain responses to affect-eliciting stimuli.

A number of fMRI studies have shown alterations in functional brain responses during negative emotion-processing paradigms in MDD (or in individuals at high risk for MDD), including increased activation of limbic-paralimbic regions (e.g., amygdala, putamen, thalamus) (Fales et al., 2009, 2008; Fu et al., 2004; Monk et al., 2008; Roberson-Nay et al., 2006; Savitz & Drevets, 2009; Sheline et al., 2001, 2009; Siegle, Steinhauer, Thase, Stenger, & Carter, 2002; Surguladze et al., 2005) and altered activation of prefrontal regions (PFC; e.g., dorsolateral PFC) (Fales et al., 2009; Hamilton et al., 2012; Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer, & Thase, 2007) thought to be involved in emotion regulation (Maletic et al., 2007; Phillips, Drevets, Rauch, & Lane, 2003). Interestingly, MDD is also associated with increased activation to negative stimuli in the hippocampus itself (Sheline et al., 2009). Studies with healthy individuals have further suggested differential functions of hippocampal subregions: activation of the anterior hippocampus is usually observed in response to emotionally charged stimuli, whereas the posterior hippocampus seems to be activated to encoding of emotionally neutral stimuli (Dolcos, LaBar, & Cabeza, 2004; Small, Schobel, Buxton, Witter, & Barnes, 2011). However, to date, no research has directly examined the potential relationships between individual variability in hippocampal volume and variability in functional brain activation in either healthy or MDD populations.

There are at least two reasons to hypothesize such relationships. The first is that the integrity of the hippocampus may influence responses to emotional stimuli. For example, preclinical studies have shown that hippocampal lesions lead to affective disturbances, including impaired fear acquisition (Anagnostaras, Maren, & Fanselow, 1999; Ji & Maren, 2007; Otto & Poon, 2006; Yoon, Graham, & Kim, 2011), reduced affiliation, emotional overreactivity, and increased aggression (Machado & Bachevalier, 2006). Although hippocampal lesions are not equivalent to reduced hippocampal volume, these findings are consistent with the possibility that reduced hippocampal volume may contribute to altered emotional processing and, potentially, to the development of MDD. It has also been suggested that there are some brain regions where functional response to emotional stimuli may be modulated by the hippocampus. The anterior hippocampus projects to the amygdala and medial PFC, whereas the posterior hippocampus projects to the posterior cingulate cortex (Small et al., 2011). The literature on MDD postulates that the hippocampus also connects with the basal ganglia (Botteron, 2001; Drevets et al., 2008; Price & Drevets, 2010). Therefore, reduced hippocampal volume may be associated with impaired function of cortico-limbic emotion regions (e.g., amygdala, medial PFC, cingulate cortex, and basal ganglia) with neural projections from the hippocampus. This could manifest as altered emotional behavior and differences in brain activation to emotion-eliciting stimuli.

A second reason to contemplate a possible association between hippocampal volume and affect-elicited brain activation is that the degree of functional responsiveness to emotionally evocative stimuli (e.g., stress, trauma, life-threatening situations) could impact hippocampal volume. For example, increased stress/trauma exposure is associated with reduced hippocampal volume in both preclinical (Blugeot et al., 2011) and clinical (Karl et al., 2006; Woon, Sood, & Hedges, 2010) studies, probably due to excitotoxic effects of excessive glucocorticoids on hippocampal neurons (McEwen, 2000; McEwen, Eiland, Hunter, & Miller, 2011; Sapolsky, 2000a, b). In MDD, emotional responsivity has been studied both in terms of increased hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis stress reactivity (Burke, Davis, Otte, & Mohr, 2005; Lopez-Duran, Kovacs, & George, 2009; Luby, Heffelfinger, et al., 2003) and in terms of functional brain responses to emotional stimuli, and studies in adults (Fales et al., 2008; Fu et al., 2004; Sheline et al., 2001, 2009; Siegle et al., 2002; Surguladze et al., 2005), adolescents (Roberson-Nay et al., 2006), and children (Barch, Gaffrey, Botteron, Belden, & Luby, in press; Gaffrey, Luby et al., 2011) with MDD have shown increased amygdala responses to emotional stimuli or emotionally evocative stimuli. Although increased functional responses to emotional stimuli are not equivalent to experiencing trauma or life threatening situations, these findings do suggest a pattern of heightened emotional responsivity to negative stimuli in MDD. Such altered emotional responsivity in the amygdala may possibly impact hippocampal structure because the amygdala has neural projections to the hippocampus, especially its anterior region (Small et al., 2011) and because the hippocampus is clearly sensitive to stress-related excitotoxicity. Moreover, sMRI studies suggest that hippocampal volume reduction in MDD is associated with duration of illness (Bell-McGinty et al., 2002; Colla et al., 2007; MacQueen et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2009; Sheline et al., 1999, 1996), which has been hypothesized to reflect hippocampal volume reduction resulting from the accumulation of stressors (McKinnon et al., 2009; Sheline et al., 1999). Although children have had less time to accumulate “stressors,” as compared with adults, it is nonetheless possible that increased responsivity to emotionally evocative stimuli could be associated with reduced hippocampal volume even at a relatively younger age (perhaps due to greater neuroplasticity).

Therefore, the first goal of the present study was to examine the relationships between hippocampal volume and functional brain responses to affect-eliciting stimuli. The second goal was to further examine the neurobiological changes present in very early onset MDD. The majority of MRI research has focused on depressed adolescents and adults (i.e., 9 years of age or older). However, MDD has been also described in children as young as preschool age (i.e., ages between 3 and 6), referred to as preschool onset MDD (PO-MDD) (Luby, Si, Belden, Tandon, & Spitznagel, 2009). PO-MDD is a validated clinical syndrome, characterized by DSM–IV-defined depressive symptoms during preschool age (Luby, Belden, Sullivan, et al., 2009; Luby, Mrakotsky, et al., 2003; Luby et al., 2006). Despite potential differences in the duration of accumulation of stress or a history of depressive episodes between PO-MDD and adulthood MDD, there is emerging evidence that neurobiological alterations associated with PO-MDD are similar to those seen in depressed adolescents and adults. For example, in our prior work, the severity of depression in preschool-aged children with current PO-MDD, as well as school-aged children with a history of PO-MDD, was associated with increased activation of regions in the limbic system, such as the amygdala and hippocampus, in response to sad faces relative to neutral faces (Barch et al., in press; Gaffrey et al. 2011b; Luking et al., 2011). We hypothesized that reduced hippocampal volume would be correlated with altered functional cortico-limbic responses to negative stimuli among children. The present study focused on functional activation in specific cortico-limbic regions, selected on the basis of the prior fMRI studies on PO-MDD and MDD discussed above: the amygdala, hippocampus, basal ganglia, medial and dorsolateral PFC, and anterior cingulate cortex. We hypothesized that smaller hippocampal volumes would be negatively correlated with functional activation to negative faces in emotional processing/limbic regions such as the amygdala, hippocampus, basal ganglia, and potentially the anterior cingulate, but they would be positively correlated to functional activation in emotion-regulation regions in the PFC. In addition, we hypothesized that these relationships would be present in both healthy children and children with PO-MDD, although they might be stronger in PO-MDD children if there is more variability in their hippocampal volumes or functional activation responses as a function of their MDD status.

Method

Subjects

Between the years of 2003 and 2005, preschoolers between the ages of 3.0 and 5.11 were recruited from pediatricians’ offices, daycares, and preschools in the St. Louis metropolitan area, using a screening checklist to oversample preschoolers with symptoms of MDD into the Validation of Preschool Depression Study (PDS), a longitudinal study of PO-MDD. Written consent was obtained from parents, and all study procedures were approved in advance by the Institutional Review Board. A sample of 306 children, without exclusion criteria (head trauma, neurological disease, or severe developmental delays or prematurity), were assessed at baseline, and 257 of them completed two follow-up annual waves, using an age-appropriate diagnostic interview, the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA; Egger, 2009; Egger, Ascher, & Angold, 1999/2003; Egger et al., 2006). On the basis of the PAPA diagnoses, children were classified into those with a history of PO-MDD, with other psychiatric disorders without PO-MDD, or with healthy status.

At the time of this report, usable data were available from 85 children who participated in structural and functional imaging 4–8 years after the baseline assessment, when they were between the ages of 7 and 12. There were additional 25 children who underwent scanning but who did not provide usable data. Five of these children could not complete the scanning protocol, and 20 had poor quality data (average signal-to-noise ratio [SNR] less than 150 on both face task runs). We focused on SNR for exclusion rather than movement alone, since this tends to be a more conservative metric that captures both movement effects and scan quality. This is a loss rate of approximately 20 %, which is typical for developmental neuroimaging studies. Twenty one of the 85 children with usable data were excluded because they either were currently taking psychotropic medicines or had taken them in the past. Therefore, a total of N = 64 nonmedicated children were included in the current analysis: n = 24 healthy children (i.e., no psychiatric diagnosis across all waves), n = 27 children with a history of PO-MDD (i.e., MDD at least once before age of 6 across waves), and n = 13 children with a history of other diagnoses without PO-MDD (i.e., disruptive or anxiety disorders at least once but no MDD diagnosis across waves). Our final sample was representative of children who were free from potential effects of psychotropic medication.

After processing structural imaging data, the volumetric data of the right hippocampus from 2 healthy subjects and the left hippocampal data from 10 subjects (3 healthy, 5 PO-MDD, and 2 other diagnosis) did not pass quality control for segmentation of the hippocampus (see below for details). Thus, the present study contained 62 subjects (22 healthy, 27 MDD, 13 other) in all analyses of right hippocampal volume and 54 subjects (21 healthy, 22 MDD, 11 other) in all analyses of left hippocampal volume.

Mock scanner training for children

After obtaining consent, children participated in a training sequence designed to increase their comfort and understanding of the scanning procedure and to minimize in-scan movement. Training included watching a short video following another child through their scanning experience, followed by the use of a mock scanner (i.e., MRI simulator) to introduce the scanning environment. Children were allowed to complete this experience at their own pace, and each training session was altered to meet the needs of the individual subject. Generally, this included lying down on the scanner table, having headphones placed on their head, and entering the bore of the simulator. Once inside the bore, children still practiced lying while listening to recorded MRI sounds and receiving visual feedback of their head movement in real time (MoTrak software). After approximately 10 min of practice, children were removed from the simulator, and any remaining questions regarding the practice or actual scan were addressed.

Diagnostic assessment

Trained staff from the Early Emotional Development Program (EEDP) at Washington University School of Medicine conducted up to three in-person annual interview sessions using the PAPA with child subjects and their caregivers. Approximately 20 % of the interviews were audiotaped and reviewed for reliability and quality control as previously reported (Luby, Belden, Pautsch, Si, & Spitznagel, 2009). The PAPA consists of a series of developmentally appropriate questions answered by the primary caregiver, which cover the DSM–IV criteria for Axis I disorders that may arise in preschoolers, including MDD, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), general anxiety disorder (GAD), separation anxiety disorder (SAD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and conduct disorder (CD). The 2-week duration of symptoms requirement for MDD during preschool was set aside on the basis of data suggesting that it may not be an appropriate threshold for this developmental period (Gaffrey, Belden, & Luby, 2011; Luby et al., 2002, 2009c). Otherwise, preschoolers who fell into the MDD group met all DSM–IV MDD symptom criteria.

Children were classified as having a history of PO-MDD if they displayed clinical levels of MDD on the PAPA at any time point prior to the age of 6.0 (Luby et al., 2002). The PAPA also allows for quantification of symptom severity: for each in-person diagnostic time point, depression sum scores (the total number of MDD symptoms endorsed in the PAPA) were calculated and used to assess core MDD symptom severity (Luby, Mrakotsky, Heffelfinger, Brown, & Spitznagel, 2004). We also assessed symptoms of non-MDD internalizing disorders (PTSD, GAD, and SAD) and externalizing disorders (ODD, ADHD, and CD) at each time point. We used these diagnostic characteristics to examine whether results were specific to PO-MDD or were related more generally to childhood internalizing or externalizing psychopathology.

Functional task and stimuli

The fMRI task was a facial emotion-processing task, using the MacArthur Network Face Stimuli Set, a validated stimulus set that contains 43 different actors from different ethnic/cultural backgrounds (Tottenham et al., 2009). Children were shown faces that varied in affective content (sad, angry, fearful, happy, and neutral) from 10 sets of adults and were asked to decide whether the face was male or female. We chose to use a task that did not require explicit attention to the emotional content, because of evidence that heightened amygdala responses associated with MDD may be more apparent with tasks that do not explicitly require a focus on the emotional content of the stimuli (Fales et al., 2008; Monk et al., 2008). In addition, we created intermediate affective expressions by morphing the neutral expression for each individual with their emotional expression so that the resulting face was halfway between neutral and the target emotion (MorphAge software). We included these stimuli because behavioral and brain activation biases in depression may be more apparent at less intense emotional expressions where there may be a greater likelihood of detecting bias. Thus, each “actor” in the stimulus set provided a total of nine facial expressions (neutral; 50 % sad, angry, fearful, and happy; 100 % sad, angry, fearful, and happy). Each stimulus was presented for 2,500 ms, followed by an ITI ranging between 500 and 6,500 ms. The task was programmed in PsyScope, and behavioral responses in the scanner were acquired via a fiber optic button box interfaced with the PsyScope button box. Each run consisted of 45 stimuli, five from each of the nine conditions. Selection of the specific face to be presented on each trial was determined by PsyScope using a random without replacement algorithm, within the constraint of the number of faces of each type to be presented in a run. The images were projected onto a computer screen behind the subject’s head within the imaging chamber. The screen was viewed by a mirror positioned approximately 8 cm above the subject’s face.

Data acquisition

Structural and functional scanning was performed on a 3.0 Tesla TIM TRIO Siemens whole-body system within the same session. Each session took approximately 1 h 15 min. Two three-dimensional T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) scans (~6 min each; TR = 2,400 ms, TE = 3.16 ms, flip angle = 8°, slab = 160 mm, 160 slices, isotropic 1 × 1 × 1 mm voxels on a matrix of 256 × 224 × 160, 1 signal average) were acquired in the sagittal plane. Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) images during face processing were acquired with a T2*-weighted asymmetric spin-echo echo-planar sequence (~5 min; TR = 2,500 ms, TE = 27 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 256 mm, voxel size = 4 × 4 × 4 mm) in the axial plane parallel to the anterior–posterior commissure, with a 12-channel head coil. During each functional run, 99 sets of 36 contiguous axial images with isotropic voxels (4 mm3) were acquired parallel to the anterior–posterior commissure plane. These functional face processing scans took approximately 12 min. In addition, the children completed a film-processing task (~5 min), two resting state runs (~12 min), and two diffusion tensor imaging runs (~15 min).

Structural data processing

The 16-bit MPRAGE imaging data were converted to Analyze format (Robb, 2001). The two acquisitions were then coregistered and summed to increase the SNR (Holmes et al., 1998). The registered images were resampled to 0.5 mm3 voxels and then converted to 8-bit while maintaining the dynamic range of the image intensities.

The target hippocampus was rotated to its head/tail orientation and was divided into five coronal parts. Then, four landmarks (superior, lateral, inferior, and medial) were placed on these coronal views of the hippocampus (Csernansky et al., 1998) by an experienced rater (C.M.B.) who was blind about diagnostic classification. The same procedure was applied to place landmarks on the template hippocampus, which an experienced rater (C.M.B.) manually delineated on an anatomically representative individual drawn from the same age population on the basis of our previously described hippocampal boundary definitions (Haller et al., 1996, 1997). The template hippocampus was then converted to a 3-D tessellated surface to generate the template surface.

The landmarked subject hippocampus was oriented to the template hippocampus by applying a rigid landmark transformation algorithm (no stretch or warping). A nonlinear large deformation landmark (LDL) transformation was then applied to refine the initial transformation. These two transformations aligned the subject hippocampus with the template hippocampus purely on the basis of expert landmarks. A nonlinear diffeomorphic transformation was then generated using the voxel intensities of the subject and template image through the large deformation diffeomorphic metric mapping (LDDMM; Beg, Miller, Trouve, & Younes, 2005; Miller, Trouve, & Younes, 2002). The inverse of the high-dimensional transformation from LDDMM was applied to the template surface. Finally, the LDL transformation was inverted to generate a surface representation of the subject hippocampus. Hippocampal volumes were calculated within the resulting surfaces.

Although our transformation technique performs with high accuracy rates, there are generally a small proportion of subject scans whose final transformation results do not meet quality check (QC) standards assessed during visual review of all hippocampi. Visual manifestations of QC problems include surface boundaries that extend into the cerebral spinal fluid and outside of the hippocampal gray matter. Two right hippocampi and ten left hippocampi did not meet our QC standards and were excluded in subsequent analyses.

Functional data preprocessing

The fMRI data were preprocessed using the following steps: (1) compensation for slice-dependent time shifts; (2) removal of the first four images of each run to allow BOLD signal to reach steady state; (3) elimination of odd/even slice intensity differences due to interpolated acquisition; (4) realignment of data acquired in each subject within and across runs to compensate for rigid body motion (Ojemann et al., 1997); (5) intensity normalization to a whole brain mode value of 1,000; (6) registration of the 3-D structural volume (T1) to the atlas representative template in the Talairach coordinate system (Talairach & Tournoux, 1988) using a 12-parameter affine transform and resampling to 1-mm cubic representation (Buckner et al., 2004; Ojemann et al., 1997); (7) coregistration of the 3-D fMRI volume to the T2, and the T2 to the subjects’ structural image; (8) transformation of the fMRI to atlas space using a single affine 12-parameter transform that included resampling to a 3-mm cubic representation; and (9) spatial smoothing using a 6-mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian filter. Prior research has validated the use of this approach with children in our age range (Burgund et al., 2002; Kang, Burgund, Lugar, Petersen, & Schlaggar, 2003). This validation has included evidence that, when a child-specific atlas is used (as we did), the variation in the degree of volumetric displacement in children 7 years of age and older following registration to a standard space is no more than that for adults and that children (7 years and older) and adults show similar hemodynamic response functions to basic visual and motor processing.

Analysis of fMRI data was performed using in-house software (FIDL analysis package, http://www.nil.wustl.edu/labs/fidl/index.html) utilized in numerous previously published studies (Becerril, Repovs, & Barch, 2011; Braver, Paxton, Locke, & Barch, 2009; Gaffrey et al. 2011b; Jimura, Locke, & Braver, 2010; Ollinger, Corbetta, & Shulman, 2001; Ollinger, Shulman, & Corbetta, 2001; Savine & Braver, 2010). Estimates of BOLD response to each face type within each subject were obtained using fixed effects general linear models incorporating regressors for linear trend and baseline shifts. A hemodynamic response shape was assumed (Boynton function) and used to derive magnitude estimates relative to fixation baseline. These single-subject estimates were then entered into group-level analyses that treated subjects as random effects.

Analytical approach

Demographic and clinical differences (age, gender, handedness, PO-MDD diagnosis, average core MDD severity over the three annual assessments, and comorbid non-MDD internalizing/externalizing diagnosis) between the PO-MDD and healthy groups were tested using independent-samples t-tests or cross tabulation chi-square tests. Moreover, additional independent-samples t-tests were used to compare right and left hippocampal volumes across groups.

The present study used a priori region of interest (ROI) analysis for the correlations between hippocampal volume and functional brain responses to negative stimuli. The selected ROIs were those previously thought to be relevant for understanding emotion processing in MDD: the amygdala, hippocampus, basal ganglia, medial and dorsolateral PFC, and anterior cingulate cortex. Previous fMRI studies on PO-MDD or MDD have identified functional alterations in the amygdala (Barch et al., in press; Fales et al., 2008, 2009; Fu et al., 2004; Gaffrey et al. 2011b; Roberson-Nay et al., 2006; Sheline et al., 2001, 2009; Siegle et al., 2007; Surguladze et al., 2005), dorsolateral PFC (Fales et al., 2009), hippocampus (Barch et al., in press; Sheline et al., 2009), medial PFC (Sheline et al., 2009), anterior cingulate cortex (Fu et al., 2004; Sheline et al., 2009), and basal ganglia (Fu et al., 2004), while it has also been postulated that the hippocampus structurally interacts with the amygdala, medial PFC, cingulate cortex, and basal ganglia (Botteron, 2001; Drevets et al., 2008; Price & Drevets, 2010; Small et al., 2011) within emotional processing network. Our a priori selection of ROIs was based on these extant literatures.

Our anatomical templates of the amygdala, hippocampus, and basal ganglia were derived from prior work (Mamah et al., 2007, 2010; Wang et al., 2008). The PFC ROIs were defined on an atlas-representative image using the boundaries described by Rajkowska and Goldman-Rakic (1995a, b). The cingulate ROIs were used from centroids of activation identified in prior studies (Fales et al., 2008, 2009; Sheline et al., 2009), around which we drew 25-mm diameter spherical ROIs edited to respect gray matter boundaries on an atlas-representative image.

We examined voxel-based analysis of partial correlations (controlling for age and sex) between left/right hippocampal volume and two indices of functional brain responses to emotional faces: (1) activation to 50 % and 100 % sad faces, relative to neutral faces, and (2) activation to the average of all negative face types (50 % and 100 % sad, angry, and fearful faces), relative to neutral faces. These tests were performed both for the entire nonmedicated sample and just within each of the PO-MDD or healthy groups, with correction for multiple comparisons using simulations to generate a p-value and cluster size criterion that provide a false positive rate of p < .05 for the whole ROI mask (Forman et al., 1995; McAvoy, Ollinger, & Buckner, 2001). This threshold/cluster-size requirement provides protection against type I error and was chosen on the basis of Monte Carlo simulations via AlphaSim (Ward, 2000). The present study set the threshold as z = 2.6 at p < .005 and 16 voxels. If any significant correlations were found, we tested whether they remained significant after controlling for a cumulative history of PO-MDD status, non-MDD internalizing disorders, and/or externalizing disorders. This was measured by assessing whether or not subjects had ever met criteria for PO-MDD, a non-MDD internalizing disorder, and/or an externalizing disorder between the baseline assessment and the follow-up assessments prior to the present MRI study. In addition, we investigated whether the correlations found in the entire sample remained significant (or similar in magnitude) within the healthy control or PO-MDD group alone.

To examine any group difference in the correlations, we used hierarchical regression analyses to predict the magnitude of BOLD in the regions identified in the initial correlational analyses, with age, sex, PO-MDD status, and hippocampal volume entered at step 1 and the cross-product of PO-MDD status and hippocampal volume at step 2.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics

There were no differences in age, t(49) = 0.18, p > .05, gender, χ 2(1) = 0.16, p > .05, or handedness, χ 2(1) = 3.59, p > .05, between the PO-MDD and healthy groups (see Table 1). As was expected, the PO-MDD group scored higher on average core MDD severity than the healthy group, adjusted t(44) = 10.10, p < .01 (see Table 1). In addition, 18 subjects in the PO-MDD group (=67 %) showed a history of non-MDD internalizing (i.e., PTSD, GAD, and SAD) and/or externalizing psychiatric symptoms (i.e., ODD, ADHD, and CD).

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Group differences in hippocampal volume

Figure 1 illustrates that the PO-MDD group showed significantly smaller left hippocampal volume than did the healthy group, t(41) = 2.17, p < .05. There was also a trend for the PO-MDD group to show smaller right hippocampal volume than did the healthy group, t(47) = 1.88, p = .07.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Comparisons of left and right hippocampal volume between the PO-MDD group and the healthy group. Asterisk indicates a significant difference at p < .05

Correlations in the total sample

50 % and 100 % sad faces

Left hippocampal volume was negatively correlated with the magnitude of functional activation to sad faces in the left amygdala, right hippocampus, left lateral globus pallidus, right anterior nucleus of the thalamus, right caudate body, and right dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) (see Table 2 and Fig. 2). These correlations were significant even after controlling for a history of PO-MDD, non-MDD internalizing, and externalizing disorders (p < .05). When the correlations within the ROIs identified in the whole sample were computed within the PO-MDD and healthy control groups separately, the correlations between left hippocampal volume and functional activation in the left amygdala and the right caudate body were significant in both groups (see Table 2). The correlation with functional activation in the left lateral globus pallidus was significant only in the PO-MDD group, whereas the correlations with functional activation in the right hippocampus, right anterior thalamus, and right DLPFC were significant only in healthy controls (see Table 2). However, hierarchical regression analyses indicated that none of correlation magnitudes differed between groups. There were no significant correlations between right hippocampal volume and functional brain responses to sad faces.

Table 2 Functional activity correlations with hippocampal volumes identified in total sample (N = 64)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Brain regions demonstrating correlations between left hippocampal volume and functional brain response to sad faces (upper) or all negative faces (lower) in the total sample. Cool colors represent negative correlations. DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; ANT = anterior nucleus of thalamus; CB = caudate body; Hippo = hippocampus; LGP = lateral globus pallidus; Amy = amygdala

50 % and 100 % all negative faces

Both left hippocampal (see Fig. 2) and right hippocampal (see Fig. 3) volumes were negatively correlated with left amygdala activation to all negative faces, and these correlations remained significant even after a history of PO-MDD, non-MDD internalizing, and externalizing disorders was covaried (p < .05) (see Table 2). Figure 4 shows scatterplots of correlations between hippocampal volume on each side and left amygdala functional activation. When the correlations within the ROIs detected in the total sample were examined within the PO-MDD group and the healthy control group separately, the correlation with left hippocampal volume was significant in both groups (see Table 2). In contrast, the correlation with right hippocampal volume was significant only in the PO-MDD group, although hierarchical regression analyses indicated that the magnitude of this correlation did not differ between the groups (see Table 2).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Brain regions demonstrating correlations between right hippocampal volume and functional brain response to all negative faces in the total sample. Cool colors represent negative correlations. Amy = amygdala

Fig. 4
figure 4

Scatterplots of correlations between amygdala activation and hippocampal volume. Left scatterplot illustrating the correlation between left hippocampal volume and amygdala responses to all negative faces versus neutral faces in the total sample. Right scatterplot illustrating the correlation between right hippocampal volume and amygdala responses to all negative faces versus neutral faces in the total sample

Correlations in the PO-MDD group

Among children with a history of PO-MDD, only right hippocampal volume was negatively correlated with right putamen activation to sad faces and left amygdala activation to all negative faces (see Table 3 and Fig. 5). These correlations remained significant with partialling out a history of non-MDD internalizing and externalizing disorders (p < .05). The healthy controls did not show significant correlations with hippocampal volumes in these same regions (see Table 3). A hierarchical regression analysis revealed that the interaction between group and hippocampal volume was significant in predicting left amygdala activation, indicating that the PO-MDD group showed significantly stronger correlation between right hippocampal volume and left amygdala activation than did the healthy controls (see Table 3). However, the hierarchical regression analysis indicated that there was not a significant group difference in the magnitude of the relationship between right-hippocampal volume and right putamen activity (see Table 3).

Table 3 Functional activity correlations with hippocampal volumes identified in only PO-MDD (n = 27)
Fig. 5
figure 5

Brain regions demonstrating correlations between right hippocampal volume and functional brain response to sad faces (upper) or all negative faces (lower) in the PO-MDD sample. Cool colors represent negative correlations. Pu = putamen; Amy = amygdala

Correlations in healthy controls

In contrast to the PO-MDD group, healthy controls alone demonstrated only one significant correlation, which was between left hippocampal volume and functional brain activity to sad faces in the right anterior nucleus of the thalamus (see Table 4). This correlation was not significant in the PO-MDD group, but a hierarchical regression indicated that the magnitude of this correlation did not differ significantly between the groups (see Table 4).

Table 4 Functional activity correlations with hippocampal volume identified in only healthy controls (n = 24)

Group differences in functional activation

As was presented above, the PO-MDD group showed smaller left hippocampal volume (with a trend for reduced right hippocampal volume as well), and we found negative correlations between hippocampal volumes and functional brain activation to negative faces in both the total sample and the PO-MDD group. Thus, we also investigated whether the PO-MDD group showed significantly different activation than did controls in the regions showing correlations with hippocampal volume in the total sample and/or the PO-MDD analysis only. We computed an ANOVA comparing activation to sad faces versus neutral faces, with group as a between-subjects factor (PO-MDD and healthy) and region (the seven regions from Tables 2 and 3) as a within-subjects factor. This ANOVA indicated a trend level main effect of group, F(1, 49) = 3.48, p = .07, but no significant group × region interaction, F(6, 294) = 1.02, p = .80. The PO-MDD children showed a trend for greater activation than did the healthy children across all regions. We computed a similar ANOVA for all negative faces, as compared with neutral, with group as a between-subjects factor and region as a within-subjects factor. In this case, region had three levels, as shown in Tables 2 and 3. This ANOVA did not reveal any significant main effects or interactions.

Discussion

The primary goal of the present study was to examine the relationships between hippocampal volume and functional brain activation to sad or negative facial expressions, as compared with neutral expressions, in a sample of children with and without a history of PO-MDD. The present study demonstrated that children with a history of PO-MDD exhibited significantly smaller left hippocampal volume than did healthy controls, with a trend for a similar finding in the right hippocampus. These results are consistent with findings in depressed individuals of older ages (Bell-McGinty et al., 2002; Bremner et al., 2000; Campbell, Marriott, Nahmias, & MacQueen, 2004; Frodl et al., 2002; Lorenzetti et al., 2009; MacMaster & Kusumakar, 2004; MacMaster et al., 2008; Malykhin et al., 2010; McKinnon et al., 2009; O'Brien et al., 2004; Rao et al., 2010; Savitz & Drevets, 2009; Steffens et al., 2000, 2011; Videbech & Ravnkilde, 2004). Furthermore, we found that individual differences in hippocampal volume were associated with individual differences in functional brain responses to sad or negative faces. Lastly, we found a trend for some of the regions showing correlations between hippocampal volume and functional activation to show enhanced activation in the PO-MDD children, as compared with controls. Taken together, these results provide support for the hypothesis that there is a meaningful relationship between altered hippocampal volume and functional brain activation to affectively valenced stimuli, both in the general population and among children with PO-MDD.

Hippocampal volume and functional brain responses to negative faces in the whole sample

The results of analyses in the total sample indicated that greater left hippocampal volume was associated with less activation to sad faces (as compared with neutral) in the left amygdala, right (posterior) hippocampus, left globus pallidus, right anterior nucleus of the thalamus, right caudate, and right DLPFC. In contrast, there were no such relationships with right hippocampal volume in the total sample. However, greater right and left hippocampal volumes were both associated with less left amygdala activation to all negative faces versus neutral faces. Furthermore, almost all of the correlations with the left hippocampus remained significant when examined separately in the PO-MDD and healthy groups. The fact that these relationships were similar in both the PO-MDD and healthy groups suggests that, at least, some of the mechanisms driving the relationships between hippocampal volume and functional brain activation to emotional valenced faces are not specific or unique to MDD.

It is particularly interesting that left hippocampal volume was associated with functional reactivity in the left amygdala, right posterior hippocampus, and right DLPFC in response to sad facial expressions. These regions have been consistently implicated as important for emotional reactivity and regulation (Mayberg, 2003). Specifically, the amygdala responds to a wide variety of negative stimuli (Fusar-Poli et al., 2009; Whalen et al., 2001), consistent with the hypothesis that the amygdala functions to help perceive affective salience or signals in the environment (Tottenham & Sheridan, 2009). Episodic encoding of emotional stimuli may be performed by the hippocampus, as evidenced by studies showing increased activation in the hippocampus during emotional/facial encoding (Chua, Schacter, Rand-Giovannetti, & Sperling, 2007; Fischer, Nyberg, & Backman, 2010; Ku, Tolias, Logothetis, & Goense, 2011). However, encoding of emotional stimuli increases functional activation in the anterior, rather than posterior, subregion of the hippocampus (Dolcos et al., 2004; Small et al., 2011), whereas the present study found that altered hippocampal volume was associated with functional activity in a more posterior aspect of the hippocampus in response to sad faces. Prior data suggest that the posterior hippocampus serves to help support processing of and memory for emotionally neutral stimuli (Dolcos et al., 2004; Small et al., 2011). Additional research is needed to confirm these distinctions in the processing functions of anterior versus posterior hippocampal regions and to replicate our findings of correlations between hippocampal volume and activation to emotional faces in posterior, rather than anterior, region.

Some aspects of emotional processing in the amygdala and hippocampus are regulated by the DLPFC (Maletic et al., 2007; Phillips et al., 2003). For example, healthy individuals showed increased activation in the right DLPFC when they were instructed to ignore fearful stimuli (Fales et al., 2008). Nevertheless, given that depression is thought to be associated with reduced hippocampal volume (Caetano et al., 2007; MacMaster & Kusumakar, 2004; MacMaster et al., 2008; Rao et al., 2010) and less activation to negative stimuli in the DLPFC (Hamilton et al., 2012), it was unexpected that the present study found the negative relationship between hippocampal volume and DLPFC activation. As such, this finding needs to be replicated and expanded in future work.

Taken together, the results of the present study are consistent with the hypothesis that left hippocampal structural integrity (as indexed by volume) may modulate emotional responsivity in regions such as the left amygdala and the right hippocampus, as well as potentially influence emotional regulation functions subserved by the DLPFC. An alternative mechanism could be that hypersensitivity to negative stimuli in regions such as the amygdala or the DLPFC might influence the structure of the hippocampus, through excitotoxic effects of glutamate accumulation (Sapolsky, 2000b) or repeated activation of the HPA axis (Juster, McEwen, & Lupien, 2010; McEwen, Eiland, Hunter, & Miller, 2012) that might contribute to loss of or inhibited neurogenesis or synaptogenesis of neurons, leading to hippocampal atrophy.

PO-MDD-specific relationships between hippocampal volume and functional brain responses to negative faces

The results of analyses in the PO-MDD group alone yielded significant correlations between right hippocampal volume (but not left) and functional activation to sad faces in the right putamen and to all negative faces in the left amygdala. In particular, the correlation between right hippocampal volume and left amygdala activation was (1) not identified in the healthy controls, (2) remained significant even when controlling for a history of non-MDD psychiatric disorders, and (3) was significantly stronger in the PO-MDD than in the healthy group. Such results suggest that the negative correlation between right hippocampal volume and left amygdala activation may be specific to PO-MDD, rather than generalized to internalizing or externalizing psychopathology.

Reduced hippocampal volume (Caetano et al., 2007; MacMaster & Kusumakar, 2004; MacMaster et al., 2008; Rao et al., 2010), as well as increased amygdala responses to negative stimuli (Barch et al., in press; Gaffrey et al. 2011b; Roberson-Nay et al., 2006), has been found in childhood and/or adolescent MDD. Our findings are consistent with and help elucidate the relationship between these two biomarkers for MDD. There is a discrepancy across fMRI findings such that depressed adolescents have shown increased functional activation to emotionally evocative stimuli (Roberson-Nay et al., 2006) but decreased activation to fearful faces (Thomas et al., 2001) in the left amygdala. Although our findings do not clearly help us to understand such discrepancies in the literature, it would be interesting to determine whether variability in amygdala responsivity across prior studies was related to hippocampal volume differences across studies.

The correlation between right hippocampal volume and functional activation to sad faces in the right putamen was also found in the PO-MDD group, but not the whole sample or the healthy controls, even when controlling for a history of non-MDD psychiatric disorders. However, these findings provided only weak evidence for a specific PO-MDD finding because the strength of this correlation in the PO-MDD group did not significantly differ from that in the controls. Nevertheless, one interesting study showed that 8-year-old children with potential high risk for MDD (i.e., T allele carriers of CERB1 gene) showed increased activation in the right putamen, as compared with controls (i.e., C allele carriers), when they viewed sad-film clips (Fortier et al., 2011). Hence, hyperactivation to sad stimuli in the right putamen may be associated with risk for MDD, and our study additionally suggests that this activation in the right putamen may be related to right hippocampal volume.

Limitations

The analyses in the total sample used a modest sample size (N = 64) that is comparable to or larger than sample sizes used in many other child-focused studies. However, the sample sizes for the PO-MDD and control groups alone were smaller, reducing the power for analyses in these subgroups. Related to this point, the significant correlations with left hippocampal volume found in the whole sample (i.e., correlations between left hippocampal volume and functional activation to sad/all-negative faces in the left amygdala, left lateral globus pallidus, and right caudate body) also remained significant when tested within the PO-MDD group at the ROI level. However, these correlations were not identified in the voxel-wise analyses with the PO-MDD group alone. This discrepancy likely reflects the smaller sample size for the PO-MDD group (n = 27), as compared with the whole sample, which reduced statistical power to detect the correlations in the voxel-by-voxel analysis in the PO-MDD group alone.

A second limitation is that our results cannot inform the direction of effects (e.g., determine whether reductions in hippocampal volume contribute to increased functional brain activation or vice versa). As was discussed above, some studies in adults have indicated that reduced hippocampal volume was correlated with longer MDD episode durations (Bell-McGinty et al., 2002; Colla et al., 2007; MacQueen et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2009; Sheline et al., 1996, 1999). These findings contribute to the argument that hippocampal volume reduction may result from the experience of MDD, including potentially hyperactive emotional responses. It will be necessary to conduct longitudinal investigations to clarify the nature of the causal relationships between hippocampal atrophy and altered functional activation in the brain.

Conclusions

In summary, the present findings demonstrated that children with PO-MDD showed smaller left hippocampal volume than did healthy children, that left hippocampal volume was associated with functional brain activation to negative faces in both PO-MDD and healthy children, that right hippocampal volume was associated with functional brain activation to negative faces in PO-MDD children alone, and that the regions showing such correlations with hippocampal volume tended to show enhanced activation in the PO-MDD children, as compared with the healthy children. Taken together, these findings provide direct evidence that there may be common mechanisms contributing to hippocampal volume reductions and hyperactivity of cortico-limbic regions in response to negative stimuli among children with PO-MDD. These data cannot inform us as to the direction of these relationships, and further work will be needed to determine whether reduced hippocampal volume contributes to hyperresponsivity of cortico-limbic regions to emotional stimuli, whether hyperresponsivity to emotional stimuli contributes to hippocampal volume reduction, or whether another variable critical to the pathophysiology of MDD contributes to both reduced hippocampal volume and increased cortico-limbic activity to negative stimuli.