When an object layout is understood allocentrically—without reference to the observer’s vantage point—the relative locations of the objects are given by exocentric distances between the objects and exocentric directions of the objects relative to one another. Distance information is at least as important as direction information in understanding a layout (Waller, Loomis, Golledge, & Beall, 2000). The distance between two points is thought to be perceived in terms of the visual angle formed when the points are intersected by vectors originating at the observer (Foley, Ribeiro, & Da Silva, 2004). A polar coordinate system is implicit in the visual angle conception.

Although exocentric distances are perceived in terms of the visual angle, they are often organized in memory in terms of an allocentric reference frame. Such a frame comprises a pair of orthogonal axes, one of which gives a reference direction (Mou, McNamara, Valiquiette, & Rump, 2004). The frame provides a basis for encoding the locations of objects in a layout and for updating those locations over movement (Mou, Liu, & McNamara, 2009). To understand the distance between two objects in terms of an allocentric reference frame, the observer must often extract orthogonal components, corresponding to the axes of the frame, from that distance (Mou et al., 2009). Figure 1 illustrates this idea. Note that, whereas a polar coordinate system is implicit in the assessment of the distance between two objects, a rectangular coordinate system is implicit in the extraction of orthogonal components from that distance.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Hypothetical layout. Solid circles A–F are objects. Open circle X is a hypothetical point. Let us assume that the layout of objects A–F is understood in terms of a reference frame consisting of orthogonal axes, one of which intersects A, C, D, and F (Mou, Liu, & McNamara, 2009; Mou, Zhao, & McNamara, 2007). Understanding the distance BC in terms of this reference frame involves extracting the distances BX and CX, respectively, from that distance

How are the orthogonal components corresponding to the axes of a reference frame extracted from the distance between two objects? Such problems have attracted interest because observers often do not solve them cleanly (Maddox & Dodd, 2003). An observer is not, that is, guided solely by the psychological dimension of interest in solving such a problem. This sort of imprecision has been described as dimensional interaction (Melara, 1989, 1992). Dimensional interaction may be fostered when orthogonal components are extracted from distances because the process requires reconciliation of the polar and rectangular coordinate systems.

Traditionally, questions of dimensional interaction have been studied with speeded classification tasks (Kemler Nelson, 1993; Lockhead, 1972, 1979). Recently, such a task has been used to explore how horizontal and vertical components are extracted from distances in a frontal plane (Dopkins, 2005). On each trial of the complex distance task, small circles appear at two locations on a computer screen. Across trials, circles appear at a task array of locations, with adjacent locations being separated by the same physical distance on the horizontal and vertical dimensions of the array. The participant is required to indicate whether the test locations are a specified distance apart on one of the dimensions of the array. A response signal controls the interval between the presentation of the circles and the participant’s response. Thus, the error rate data are of greatest interest.

Dopkins (2005) tested participants in the complex distance task with a 7 column × 3 row task array. Participants were to indicate on each trial whether the test locations were two or fewer horizontal positions apart (see Fig. 2). The error rate in Dopkins’s task increased with increases in the horizontal distance between the test locations for pairs of locations that were two or fewer horizontal positions apart (2> pairs) and decreased with increases in horizontal distance for pairs of locations three or more horizontal positions apart (3< pairs). Of greater interest, the error rate increased with increases in vertical distance for 2> pairs and decreased with increases in vertical distance for 3< pairs.Footnote 1 Thus, although the task required an assessment of the focal horizontal distance between the test locations, performance also reflected the nonfocal vertical distance between the locations. Of particular interest were the relative sizes of the horizontal and vertical distance effects. The 2> pairs were most important to this comparison because the horizontal and vertical distance between the test locations varied through the same three levels (0, 1, 2 positions), making possible a matched test of the relative sizes of the two distance effects. When the response interval was 400 ms, the error rate for 2> pairs depended to an equivalent degree on horizontal and vertical distances. When the response interval was 1,000 ms, the error rate for 2> pairs depended to a greater degree on horizontal than on vertical distance.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Task array used in the complex distance task of Dopkins (2005). Positions are indicated on the horizontal and vertical dimensions (participants did not see these numbers). Participants were to indicate on each trial whether the test locations were two or fewer horizontal positions apart

The present study proposes an interpretation for Dopkins’s (2005) nonfocal distance effect and offers support for this interpretation. To motivate the interpretation, notice that the task of Dopkins can be performed in two ways. On the one hand, responses can be based on the horizontal distance between the test locations, as the instructions indicate. Alternatively, responses can be based on the overall distance between the test locations. Performance in the latter case depends on how the horizontal and vertical dimensions are weighted in the calculation of overall distance. With the two dimensions weighted equally, the overall distance is at most 2.8 (positions) for 2> pairs and at least 3 for 3< pairs. Thus, the task can be accurately performed, but the required discrimination is more difficult than if responses are based on horizontal distance (in which case, the overall distance is at most 2 for 2> pairs and at least 3 for 3< pairs).Footnote 2 To the degree that the vertical dimension is weighted less heavily than the horizontal dimension, the maximum overall distance for 2> pairs decreases toward 2, and the discrimination becomes easier.

In explaining the nonfocal distance effect, we start from the following premises: (1) The distance between the test locations in the complex distance task is assessed on the basis of the visual angle between them, in terms of an implicit system of polar coordinates, centered at the observer (Foley et al., 2004); (2) the horizontal and vertical positions of the test locations are assessed on the basis of the frontal/horizontal/median frame, in terms of an implicit system of rectangular coordinates, also centered at the observer (Matin & Li, 1995). Given these assumptions, we suggest that because polar coordinates do not acknowledge the horizontal and vertical dimensions, the focal horizontal or vertical distance between the test locations cannot be directly assessed under the conditions of the complex distance task. Only the overall distance between the test locations can be assessed. As a consequence, the task is performed on the basis of overall distance. However, because the horizontal and vertical positions of the test locations can be assessed, in terms of rectangular coordinates, the focal and nonfocal positions of the locations may be differentially weighted, before the overall distance between the locations is assessed, to reduce the contribution of nonfocal distance to overall distance (Maddox, 1992; Nosofsky, 1986, 1987). Such position weighting occurs because it makes the required discrimination easier. For Dopkins (2005), such weighting occurred at the long, but not the short, response interval.

Because we seek to model these ideas, we introduce general recognition theory (GRT), which provides a framework for understanding dimensional interaction in recognition/categorization judgments. A key virtue of GRT is that it separates the perceptual and decisional aspects of such judgments. In GRT, a stimulus consists of multiple components, each of which corresponds to a level on a physical dimension. The perceptual effect of a stimulus on a given occasion is represented as a point in a psychological space, the dimensions of which correspond to the physical dimensions in terms of which the stimulus is defined. The perceptual effect of the stimulus across occasions is represented as a multivariate probability distribution, the variables of which correspond to the dimensions of the space. A judgment regarding the effect of a stimulus on a given occasion is based on a decision bound that divides the space into sectors associated with different judgment outcomes (Ashby & Perrin, 1988).

For modeling purposes, we state our interaction in distance extraction (IE) hypothesis as follows: The perceptual effect of each location in the complex distance task is represented across trials as a multivariate normal distribution in a Euclidean psychological space.Footnote 3 Under the hypothesis, the test locations for the task are represented in terms of polar coordinates for the purposes of distance assessment and in terms of rectangular coordinates for the purposes of position weighting. For simplicity in our modeling, we represent the locations in terms of rectangular coordinates for both processes (note that this simplification does not affect the predictions of the hypothesis). Thus, the dimensions of the space record perceived focal and nonfocal position. The response on a given complex distance trial is generated by (1) sampling from the distributions for the test locations, (2) computing the overall (Euclidean, unsigned) distance between the sampled points, and (3) comparing that distance with a criterion value. Before the overall distance between the points is computed, the coordinates of the points on the dimension of perceived nonfocal position may be adjusted to reduce the difference between the points on that dimension.Footnote 4

Other accounts may be offered, however, for the nonfocal distance effect. One possibility is that the effect reflects dimensional interaction in the process whereby the test locations are perceived, rather than in the process whereby the horizontal and vertical distances between them are extracted. This account holds, in contrast to the IE hypothesis, that the horizontal or vertical distance between the test locations can, in principle, be independently assessed. The account holds, however, that the horizontal or vertical distance between the locations cannot, in practice, be independently assessed, because the perceptual effect of a given horizontal or vertical position is not the same at all vertical or horizontal positions. In more detail, we formulate the account in terms of mean shift integrality (MSI) (Ashby & Maddox, 1994)—the most common conceptualization of dimensional interaction in the perceptual component of the GRTFootnote 5 (Kadlec & Hicks, 1998; Kingston & MacMillan, 1995; Kingston, MacMillan, Dickey, & Thorburn, 1997; MacMillan & Ornstein, 1998). Figure 3 demonstrates degrees of MSI for a 7 column × 3 row task array such as Dopkins (2005) used. Figure 3a shows a representation of the array in the absence of MSI. Each point in the figure corresponds to a location in the array. Figure 3b shows the array under the form of MSI that has typically been studied in past work. A location with a given horizontal position has a greater value on the dimension of perceived horizontal position the greater its vertical position. However, an increase on the dimension of horizontal position produces a greater increase on the dimension of perceived horizontal position than does an increase on the dimension of vertical position. Figure 3c shows the array under what we will term absolute MSI. The perceptual effects of increases on the dimensions of horizontal and vertical position are indistinguishable. The mental representation of the task array collapses the horizontal and vertical dimensions of the array onto the minor diagonal of the psychological space. Most of the points in the resulting representation correspond to multiple locations. To explain the results of Dopkins, we would assume that MSI was absolute at the short, but not the long, response interval.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mental representation underlying performance in the complex distance task according to the IP hypothesis. a The task array when MSI is absent. Each point corresponds to one of the locations in the array. b The task array under MSI as it has typically been studied in past work. A location with a given horizontal position has a greater value on the dimension of perceived horizontal position the greater its vertical position. However, an increase on the dimension of horizontal position produces a greater increase on the dimension of perceived horizontal position than does an increase on the dimension of vertical position. c The array under absolute MSI. The perceptual effects of increases on the dimension of horizontal and vertical positions are indistinguishable. The mental representation of the task array collapses the horizontal and vertical dimensions of the array onto the minor diagonal of the psychological space. Most of the points in the resulting representation correspond to multiple locations. The dotted lines show the trajectories according to which the two-dimensional representation collapses into a one-dimensional representation under MSI

More precisely, the interaction in perception (IP) hypothesis holds that the perceptual effect of each location in the complex distance task is represented across trials as a multivariate normal distribution in a Euclidean psychological space the rectangular dimensions of which correspond to perceived focal and nonfocal position. The response on a given trial is generated by (1) sampling from the distributions for the test locations, (2) computing the focal (unsigned) distance between the sampled points, and (3) comparing that distance with a criterion value. The space in which the locations are represented is characterized by MSI that varies in degree.

A third possibility is that the nonfocal distance effect reflects dimensional interaction in the decision component of the complex distance task. This account holds, in contrast to the IP hypothesis, that the horizontal and vertical positions of a location in the task can be independently perceived. The account holds, in contrast to the IE hypothesis, that the horizontal and vertical distances between a pair of test locations can be independently assessed. The account holds, however, that decisions in the task are based on the nonfocal, as well as the focal, distance between the test locations. The importance of considering dimensional interaction in the decision component of a task has been emphasized in recent work (Mack, Richler, Gauthier, & Palmeri, 2011; Silbert & Thomas, 2013; Wenger & Ingvalson, 2003). To explain the results of Dopkins (2005) in terms of such an interaction, we would assume that decisions depended equivalently on horizontal and vertical distances at the short interval and on horizontal distance to a greater degree than vertical distance at the long interval.

More precisely, the fundamental stimulus entities are the combinations of (unsigned) focal and nonfocal distance between the pairs of test locations. Over trials, the perceptual effect of each distance combination is represented as a multivariate normal distribution in a Euclidean psychological space the rectangular dimensions of which correspond to perceived focal and nonfocal distance. The response on a given trial is determined by (1) sampling from the distribution for the relevant distance combination and (2) comparing the sampled point with a decision bound. Figure 4 illustrates this interaction in decision (ID) hypothesis in terms of Dopkins’s (2005) task.Footnote 6

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mental representation underlying performance in the complex distance task according to the ID hypothesis. Each point corresponds to the center of the distribution for one of the distance combinations in the task. The dashed line shows a decision bound under which the response depends on perceived horizontal and vertical distance equivalently

Experiment 1

The present study sought support for the IE hypothesis in contrast to the IP and ID hypotheses. Experiment 1 sought support for the IE hypothesis from the pattern of sequential dependencies in the complex distance task. The important data came from crucial trial pairs, on each of which the participant responded, in succession, to a preceding trial and a target trial. Performance on the target trial was of primary interest. The rationale was as follows: Under each hypothesis, complex distance responses are determined by a process in which critical values, derived from the test locations, are compared with a criterion. Under a given hypothesis, performance on a given target trial should be facilitated if the values that are critical under that hypothesis are similar on the preceding trial and the target trial. Because the critical values differ under the different hypotheses, the hypotheses make different predictions as to the crucial trial pairs for which facilitation should be observed on the target trial.

In more detail, the preceding trial and target trial for the crucial trial pairs were always of the 2> type. The crucial trial pairs were of 14 types. The distance combination on the target trial was {2,0} for 7 of the trial-pair types and {0,2} for 7 of the trial-pair types. To generate the 14 trial-pair types, each of the two types of target trial was paired with 7 types of preceding trial, corresponding to the seven possible distance combinations that produced 2> trials, aside from that of the target trial. Predictions were made under the three hypotheses as to which of the crucial trial-pair types would produce the most facilitation and, thus, the lowest error rates on the target trial. To simplify the predictions, the 400-ms response interval was used, so that perceived horizontal and vertical distance would be interchangeable (as in Dopkins, 2005). The predictions of the hypotheses are summarized in Table 1 with respect to trial-pair types for which the distance combination on the target trial was {2,0}.Footnote 7 In the first line under each hypothesis, the table shows the predicted ordering of trial-pair types, where the types are identified in terms of the distance combination on the preceding trial. In the second and third lines for each hypothesis, the table shows, for each subset of trial-pair types, the idealized critical values on which responses to the preceding trial and the target trial would be based. These values are idealized in that they assume complete veridicality and no variability in the mental representation of the task array. In the fourth line for each hypothesis, the table shows, for each subset of trial-pair types, the differences between those critical values.Footnote 8 (The rest of the table will be discussed later.)

Table 1 Experiment 1: Predictions and results for sequential analysis

The predictions of the hypotheses had the following rationale. For IE hypotheses: the critical value would be the perceived overall distance between the test locations. For the IP hypothesis, the critical value would be the perceived horizontal distance between the test locations. However, horizontal distance would not, in practice, be capable of assessment independently of vertical distance, because the perceptual effect of a given horizontal position would not be the same at all vertical positions. When MSI was absolute, as it would have to be to account for the perceived interchangeability of horizontal and vertical distance, the mental representation of the task array would collapse the horizontal and vertical dimensions. Pairs of test locations that were separated by the same sum of horizontal and vertical distance would be associated with the same perceived horizontal distance. For the ID hypothesis, the critical values would be the perceived horizontal and vertical distances between the test locations.

Method

Participants

The 75 participants were drawn from an undergraduate psychology course. They participated in fulfillment of a course requirement.

Stimuli

Each test circle was 3 mm in diameter. Circles in horizontally and vertically adjacent locations were separated by 8 mm (from edge to edge). The participant sat approximately 60 cm from the computer screen. Thus each test circle subtended a visual angle of approximately 0.29°, and circles in adjacent locations were separated by a visual angle of approximately 0.76°.

Design

A total of 600 trials were presented in the experiment. Eight versions of each of the 14 crucial trial-pair types described earlier were presented. Thus, a total of 112 (2 (target trial type) × 7 (preceding trial type) × 8 (repetition)) crucial trial pairs were presented (for a total of 224 trials). In addition, 112 pairs of successive 3< trials (thus, a total of 224 trials) were presented (recall that the preceding trial and target trial were always of the 2> type on the crucial trial pairs). The distance combination on each of the trials in each of these pairs was sampled randomly from the set of 12 possible 3< distance combinations. In addition, 152 filler trials were presented. The distance combination on each of these filler trials was sampled randomly from the set of 20 (8 2> and 12 3<) possible distance combinations. On average, then, 285 2> trials were presented (224 + 8/20 × 152). On average, 315 3< trials were presented (224 + 12/20 × 152). The particular test locations for a given trial were sampled randomly from those that were consistent with the distance combination in effect on the trial.

Procedure

At the beginning of each trial, “Ready” appeared on the computer screen. When the participant pressed the space bar of the computer, “Ready” disappeared, and a pair of circles appeared. Four asterisks appeared at the bottom of the screen 400 ms after the circles appeared. The participant was instructed (1) to indicate whether or not the test locations were two or fewer horizontal positions apart, (2) to use the “B” and “N” keys to indicate positive and negative responses, respectively, and (3) to respond concurrently with the appearance of the asterisks. If the interval between the appearance of the circles and the participant’s response was less than 400 ms, the message “TOO FAST” appeared at the bottom of the screen after the participant’s response and remained there until the participant pressed the space bar. If the interval was greater than 650 ms, the message “TOO SLOW” appeared in the same manner. When the participant made an error, a message appeared to that effect, after the message, if any, regarding response speed. The message remained on the screen until the participant pressed the space bar. At the beginning of every sixth trial, a message appeared asking the participant to press the space bar to see the task array. When the participant pressed the space bar, circles simultaneously appeared in the 21 test locations. The array of circles remained on the screen until the participant pressed the space bar.

Results

Table 2 presents the error rate and response time data for the experiment, collapsed across participants, as a function of the horizontal and vertical distances between the test locations. To assess the relationship between error rate and response time, a correlation was computed, for each participant, between error rate and mean response time across the 20 distance combinations. Across participants, the average correlation was .389. Thus, there was no speed–accuracy trade-off. Focusing primarily on the error rate data is warranted. An equal variance Gaussian signal detection analysis of the error rate data, treating 2> trials as signal and 3< trials as noise, found d' and c to be 1.83 and .095, respectively. Thus, performance was moderately accurate and demonstrated a slight bias toward “3<” responses.

Table 2 Experiment 1: Performance as function of distance between test locations on horizontal and vertical dimensions

Examination of Table 2 suggests that the pattern of Dopkins (2005) was replicated. A generalized linear model analysisFootnote 9 found that the error rate for 2> trials increased with increases in horizontal, Z = 12.36, SE = .04, p < .0001, and vertical, Z = 12.67, SE = .04, p < .0001, distance. The effects of horizontal and vertical distance for 2> trials were of roughly equivalent size. At the same time the error rate for 3< trials decreased with increases in horizontal, Z = 7.70, SE = .05, p < .0001, and vertical, Z = 6.54, SE = .02, p < .0001, distance.

Our goal in the experiment was to distinguish the contending hypotheses in terms of their proposed processes and the critical values that these processes compared, as revealed in sequential dependencies on a crucial subset of trials. As a precondition to doing this, we modeled performance across all trials with GRT to establish the representations underlying the proposed processes. We made an attempt, for each hypothesis, to find the parameters that allowed the hypothesis to best fit the average numbers of correct and incorrect responses, across participants, for each distance combination (as are reflected in Table 2), assuming the same series of trials as participants experienced. We fit the three hypotheses with a simplex procedure, using the likelihood objective (Busemeyer & Diederich, 2010). Details regarding the specific models that were fit are given in the Appendix. The fit to the three hypotheses was good: IE hypothesis, G 2 = 27.23 ; IP hypothesis, G 2 = 32.70; ID hypothesis, G 2 = 32.60. Tables 35 give the predicted error rates for each hypothesis under the best-fitting parameters. Tables 9 and 10 describe the parameters and give the best-fitting values.

Table 3 Experiment 1: Error rate predicted under the IE hypothesis
Table 4 Experiment 1: Error rate predicted under the IP hypothesis
Table 5 Experiment 1: Error rate predicted under the ID hypothesis

It is worth noting that the predictions of the IP hypothesis departed systematically from the observed results. Note that the IP hypothesis predicted equivalent error rates for all trials on which the horizontal and vertical distances between the test locations summed to the same value (these are trials corresponding to sets of cells parallel to the minor diagonal of Table 4). This prediction followed under the hypothesis because, when MSI is absolute, as it must have been to account for the interchangeability of perceived horizontal and vertical distances in Experiment 1, the mental representation of the task array collapses the horizontal and vertical dimensions. Pairs of test locations that are separated by the same sum of horizontal and vertical distance are associated with the same perceived horizontal and vertical distances. Given that performance in the task depends on perceived horizontal distance, performance should be equivalent for all such pairs. In fact, however, error rates were not equivalent for such trials. For example, the error rate (.31) for trials on which the distance combination was {3,1} differed from the error rate (.17) for trials on which the distance combination was {4,0}, Z = 7.14, SE = .09, p < .0001. Similarly, linear trends were present across the error rates for trials on which the distance combinations were {3,2}, {4,1}, and {5,0}, Z = 5.23, SE = .04, p < .0001, and trials on which the distance combinations were {4,2}, {5,1}, and {6,0}, (Z = 3.48, SE = .04, p < .0005.

Having found the representational parameters that allowed each hypothesis to best fit the aggregate data for the experiment, we used these parameters, along with simple, plausible, processing assumptions, to simulate predictions regarding the sequential manipulation. Specifically, we used each hypothesis to simulate error rates on the target trial for the different types of crucial trial pair, under the assumption that performance on a given target trial would be facilitated if the critical values for the preceding trial and target trial were similar. In detail, we simulated error rates, assuming the same series of trials as participants experienced, under the assumption that the standard deviations of the critical distribution(s) (corresponding to the test locations under the IE and IP hypotheses and to the distance combination for the test locations under the ID hypothesis) for a given target trial was/were reduced by a proportion r if the critical values on the preceding trial and target trial differed by less than a criterion value d. We varied r and d informally for each hypothesis until the ordering of error rates across subsets of trial-pair types matched as closely as possible the ordering given by the idealized analysis. In Table 1, the fifth line of the section for each hypothesis shows the mean error rate that was simulated across the pair types in each subset of pair types and the complementary subset (that is, the subsets of pair types for which the distance combination on the target trial was {0,2}). For the most part, the ordering of the simulated error rates matched the ordering given by idealized predictions.

In Table 1, the sixth line of the section for each hypothesis shows the mean error rate that was observed across each subset of the crucial trial-pair types and the complementary subset. We will refer to the trial-pair types in terms of the distance combination on the preceding trial. The observed error rates were ordered in fairly good agreement with the predictions of the IE hypothesis, with many of the observed differences being statistically significant. In keeping with the predictions of the hypothesis, the error rate (.164) for pair type {0,2} was significantly lower than the error rate (.191) for trial-pair types {0,1} and {1,0}, Z = 2.29, SE = .01, p = .02, the error rate (.203) for trial-pair type {2,2}, Z = 2.69, SE = .01, p = .007, and the error rate (.188) for trial-pair type {1,1}, Z = 1.76, SE = .01, p = .08 (significant with one-tailed test).

The observed error rates agreed fairly well with the predictions of the IP hypothesis, with one of the observed differences being statistically significant (the error rate (.178) for trial-pair types {0,2}, and {1,1} was significantly lower than the error rate (.203) for trial-pair type {2,2}, Z = 2.02, SE = .07, p = .04). However, whereas the IP hypothesis predicted no difference between trial-pair types {0,2} and {1,1}, as has been noted, the error rate for trial-pair type {0,2} was significantly lower than the error rate for trial-pair type {1,1}, in accordance with the predictions of the IE hypothesis.

Finally, the observed error rates did not agree with the predictions of the ID hypothesis. For example, whereas the hypothesis predicted that the error rate for trial-pair type {0,2} would be higher than that for any other trial-pair type, in fact the error rate (.164) for this trial-pair type was lower than that for any other trial-pair type. And whereas the hypothesis predicted that the error rate for trial-pair type {2,2} would be among the lowest for any trial-pair type, in fact the error rate (.203) for this trial-pair type was higher than that for any other trial-pair type. The ID hypothesis might be able to accommodate the low error rate for pair type {0,2} because the assumed decision bound is attuned to perceived horizontal and vertical distance and, thus, to the perceived overall distance; facilitation might therefore be expected with repeated testing of the same overall distance. However, the hypothesis would predict that, if facilitation occurs with repeated testing of the same overall distance, facilitation should also occur with repeated testing of the same horizontal and vertical distance. Whereas overall distance is not explicitly represented in the model, horizontal and vertical distance are explicitly represented (see Fig. 4).

Discussion

The results that Dopkins (2005) observed at the short interval were replicated. Perceived horizontal and vertical distances were essentially interchangeable for 2> trials. More important, the error rates for the target trials of the crucial trial pairs were as predicted by the IE hypothesis.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 sought further support for the IE hypothesis in terms of evidence for the position weighting that the hypothesis posits. Dopkins (2005) observed larger effects of focal than of nonfocal distance in the complex distance task using a long response interval. Under the IE hypothesis, such differential distance effects imply position weighting. Pilot findings suggested that such differential distance effects also occur in the complex distance task when a relatively high level of distance exists between the locations in the task array. The IE hypothesis can accommodate the position weighting implied by these pilot findings if we assume that the positions of locations are more accessible and amenable to weighting as the locations are further apart. Experiment 2 sought to confirm these pilot findings and to gather evidence of the position weighting that they imply.

Participants performed a complex distance task with a task array in which the locations were substantially further apart than in Experiment 1 or Dopkins (2005). Whereas some participants compared the test locations on the horizontal dimension, using a 7 column × 3 row task array as in Dopkins, other participants compared the locations on the vertical dimension, using a 7 row × 3 column task array. After making the absolute distance judgment required by the complex distance task, participants, on some trials, made a relative distance judgment, deciding whether the focal or the nonfocal distance between the test locations was greater. It was expected, in light of the relatively high level of distance between the locations in the task array and the aforementioned pilot findings, that the focal distance effect would be greater than the nonfocal distance effect in the absolute distance judgment. The question of interest was whether this differential distance effect reflected position weighting, as was predicted by the IE hypothesis. To answer this question, we asked whether focal distances tended to be perceived as greater than nonfocal distances in the relative distance judgment. We assumed that the relative distance judgment would be based on an assessment of the orientation of the implicit segment joining the test locations. Whereas the IE hypothesis predicted an advantage for focal distance in the relative distance judgment, as a reflection of position weighting, the IP and ID hypotheses did not. The rationale was as follows.

IE hypothesis

To explain the focal distance advantage in the absolute distance judgment, the hypothesis would hold that the focal dimension was weighted more heavily than the nonfocal dimension. It was assumed that the dimensional weighting in place for the absolute distance judgment would be maintained for the relative distance judgment. Thus, focal distance would have an advantage. IP hypothesis: To explain the focal distance advantage in the absolute distance judgment, the hypothesis would hold that MSI was partial. Under partial MSI, the two dimensions would be affected symmetrically. To see this, note, in Fig. 3b, that the points for locations with complementary combinations of vertical and horizontal position are equally far from the minor diagonal. This point is made more formally in Table 6, which gives average distance, on the dimensions of perceived focal and nonfocal position, in an idealized IP representation, under partial MSI (σ = .5; for an explanation of σ, see the Appendix), between the means of the distributions for all pairs of test locations associated with each combination of focal and nonfocal distance on 2> trials. Similarly, symmetrical patterns were predicted under different partial degrees of MSI. Because partial MSI underlies the perception of the test locations and because the locations must be perceived for the relative, as well as the absolute, distance judgment, partial MSI would be reflected in the relative, as well as the absolute, distance judgment. Thus, focal distance would have no advantage. ID hypothesis: To explain the focal distance advantage in the absolute distance judgment, the hypothesis would hold that the decision bound took focal distance into account to a greater degree than nonfocal distance. But the decision bound for the absolute distance judgment would play no role in the relative distance judgment. Thus, focal distance would have no advantage.

Table 6 Experiment 2: IP hypothesis—Distances between means of distributions for test locations from 2> trials on dimensions of perceived focal and nonfocal position as a function of actual focal and nonfocal distances between locations

Method

Participants

The 96 participants were drawn from the same population as the participants for Experiment 1.

Stimuli

Each test circle was 7 mm in diameter. Pairs of circles in adjacent locations were separated by 22 mm (from edge to edge) on the horizontal and vertical dimensions. The participant sat approximately 60 cm from the computer screen. Thus, each test circle subtended a visual angle of approximately 0.67°, and pairs of circles in adjacent locations were separated by a visual angle of approximately 2.1°.

Design

Eight groups of participants were tested. Participants in Groups 1–4 made absolute judgments of horizontal distance (2 or fewer vs. 3 or more horizontal positions) with respect to locations in a 7 column × 3 row array. Participants in Groups 5–8 made absolute judgments of vertical distance (2 or fewer vs. 3 or more vertical positions) with respect to locations in a 7 row × 3 column array. In the relative distance judgment, participants in Groups 1 and 5 responded to the prompt “Horizontal Greater,” participants in Groups 2 and 6 responded to the prompt “Vertical Greater,” participants in Groups 3 and 7 responded to the prompt “Horizontal Smaller,” and participants in Groups 4 and 8 responded to the prompt “Vertical Smaller.”

A total of 420 trials were presented in the experiment. The crucial trials were those on which the distance combination was either {2,1} or {1,2}. These trials were important for testing the experimental hypothesis because the levels of focal and nonfocal distance complemented one another. Each type of crucial trial was presented 48 times. On 40 of each type of crucial trial, the absolute distance judgment was followed by a relative distance judgment. Each of the other 18 types of trial (20 types of trial could be distinguished on the basis of the focal/nonfocal distance between the test locations) was presented 18 times. On 10 of each of these other types of trial for which focal distance was not equivalent to nonfocal distance, the absolute distance judgment was followed by a relative distance judgment. The particular test locations on a given trial were sampled randomly from those that were consistent with the values of focal and nonfocal distance in place for the trial. In sum, 204 2> trials were presented—48 + 48 + 108 (6 [the number of remaining 2> distance combinations aside from {2,1} and {1,2}] × 18). In addition, 216 3< trials were presented (12 [the number of 3< distance combinations] × 18). The trials were presented in 21 blocks of 20 each.

Procedure

The procedure was the same as for Experiment 1, except in the following respects: (1) The task array was presented at the beginning of each block of 20 trials, and (2) on trials requiring a relative distance judgment, the judgment was prompted by the message “Horizontal Greater,” “Vertical Greater,” “Horizontal Smaller,” or “Vertical Smaller,” which appeared on the screen after the response speed and accuracy feedback (if any) for the absolute distance judgment. The participant responded “yes” or “no” to the prompt. A response signal was not used for the relative distance judgment. Feedback was provided on the accuracy of the relative distance judgment.

Results

Table 7 presents the error rate and response time data for the absolute distance judgments, collapsed across participants, as a function of the focal and nonfocal distances between the test locations. To assess the relationship between error rate and response time, correlations were computed between error rate and response time as in Experiment 1. Across participants, the average correlation between error rate and response time was .309. Thus, there was no speed–accuracy trade-off. Focusing primarily on the error rate data is warranted. An equal variance Gaussian signal detection analysis of the error rate data, treating 2> trials as signal and 3< trials as noise, found d' and c to be 1.51 and .15, respectively. Thus, performance was again moderately accurate and again demonstrated a slight bias toward “3<” responses.

Table 7 Experiment 2: Performance on absolute distance judgments as function of distance between test locations on focal and nonfocal dimensions

Examination of Table 7 suggests a replication of the pattern that Dopkins (2005) observed at the 1,000-ms interval. A generalized linear model analysis found significant effects of focal distance (2> trials, Z = 10.17, SE = .04, p < .0001; 3< trials, Z = 8.37, SE = .03, p < .0001). The analysis found significant effects of nonfocal distance (2> trials, Z = 7.22, SE = .03, p < .0001; 3< trials, Z = 3.67, SE = .03, p < .0002). As was expected on the basis of the pilot findings, the effect of focal distance was larger than the effect of nonfocal distance in the data for 2> trials.

The data for the relative distance judgments were of primary interest. Here, the data for trials on which focal distance was less than 3 were most useful, because focal and nonfocal distance varied through the same three levels (0, 1, 2) on these trials; thus, the levels of focal and nonfocal distance were matched. Table 8 presents the data for these trials as a function of the focal and nonfocal distance between the test locations. Note that two complementary sets of trials are presented in the table. For each combination of focal and nonfocal distance in the set of trials that is presented below the main diagonal, there is a complementary combination of nonfocal and focal distances in the set of trials that is presented above the main diagonal. The overall distance in the two sets is matched. In the focal set, which is presented below the diagonal, focal distance is greater than nonfocal distance. In the nonfocal set, which is presented above the diagonal, nonfocal distance is greater than focal distance. As can be seen in the table, the error rate was greater for the nonfocal set than for the focal set (.41 vs. .35), Z = 2.34, SE = .06, p < .01. Response time, however, did not differ in the focal and nonfocal sets, (1,200 vs. 1,139), F(1, 95) = 2.39, MSe = 225,704. The difference between the two sets was reflected in the results for the crucial trials. Error rate, Z = 2.0, SE = .02, p < .05, and response time, F(1, 95) = 5.72, MSe = 109,249, were greater for trials on which focal distance was 1 and nonfocal distance was 2 than for trials on which focal distance was 2 and nonfocal distance was 1.

Table 8 Experiment 2: Performance on relative distance judgments as function of distance between test locations on focal and nonfocal dimensions

Discussion

These results support the IE hypothesis, which predicted an advantage for focal distance over nonfocal distance in the relative distance judgment. The results offer no support for the IP and ID hypotheses, which predicted no such advantage. To allay any concerns that the results somehow reflect the unequal extension of the task array along the focal and nonfocal dimensions, we note that we have replicated the results using a 6 × 6 task array.

General discussion

We wish to understand how components corresponding to the orthogonal axes of an allocentric reference frame are extracted from distances between the objects in a layout. We have addressed this question with a task in which participants assess the horizontal and vertical distances between pairs of locations in a frontal plane. We have found that, when processing is speeded, performance in the task depends on the nonfocal, as well as focal, distance between the test locations. To account for these results, we suggest that, under speeded conditions, neither the horizontal nor the vertical distance between a pair of locations is directly available; only the overall distance between the locations is available. The horizontal or vertical distance between the locations can be assessed, however, to the extent that the horizontal and vertical positions of the locations are weighted prior to the assessment of overall distance so that horizontal or vertical distance makes a disproportionate contribution to overall distance.

We have contrasted our account of the nonfocal distance effect (the IE hypothesis) with accounts that attribute the effect to dimensional interaction in the perception of the test locations (the IP hypothesis) and in the decision component of the complex distance judgment (the ID hypothesis). The contrast between the IE and the IP hypotheses can be best seen in the way the two accounts construe the absence of dimensional interaction. Under the IE hypothesis, dimensional interaction is absent when the nonfocal dimension is given no weight at all—when the focal dimension is chosen over the nonfocal dimension. Under the IP hypothesis, dimensional interaction is absent when MSI is absent. No choice of dimension is required. The IE hypothesis differs from the ID hypothesis primarily in that it attributes the relative influence of focal and nonfocal distance to perceptual, as opposed to decisional, processes. In Experiment 1, we showed that the pattern of sequential dependency in complex distance data could be accommodated better under the IE hypothesis than under the IP and ID hypotheses. In Experiment 2, we observed relative judgment effects that were consistent with the IE hypothesis, and not the IP and ID hypotheses.

In addition, we note that the IE hypothesis is simply more plausible than the ID hypothesis. To explain the nonfocal distance effect, the IE account proposes that only the overall distance between the test locations is available to perceptual processes. This is plausible, given that selective access of focal distance requires reconciliation of polar and rectangular coordinate systems. To explain this effect, the ID account proposes that decision factors consistently set the decision bound in the complex distance task so as to be sensitive to nonfocal, as well as focal, distance. It is difficult, however, to understand what these factors might be, given that the task is more easily performed on the basis of focal distance alone, with no consideration of nonfocal distance.

Generality

Further work is needed to establish the boundary conditions of the nonfocal distance effect and our interpretation of it. One question is whether the IE account generalizes to situations in which processing is not speeded. In such situations, observers may use other means to assess horizontal and vertical distance. For example, to find the horizontal or vertical distance between two points, observers may mentally project one of the points onto a line running parallel to the horizontal or vertical axis of the prevailing reference frame and through the other point.Footnote 10 Regardless of the relevance of the IE account to nonspeeded situations, speed is necessary in enough situations that the present results are probably of general relevance.

Implications for the perception of distance and position

We suggest that the horizontal or vertical distance between two points cannot be directly assessed under speeded conditions because distance is assessed on the basis of visual angle, in terms of implicit polar coordinates, which do not acknowledge the horizontal and vertical dimensions. We suggest that, under speeded conditions, horizontal and vertical distances can only be assessed through the weighting of position information. By implication, horizontal and vertical positions are available under such conditions, although horizontal and vertical distances are not.

Implications for attention

Precedent exists for the idea that the dimensions of psychological space can be weighted to reflect the requirements of the current task. This sort of dimension weighting has been proposed to explain performance in object judgment, visual search, and category-learning tasks (Kumada, 2001; Muller & O’Grady, 2000; Nosofsky, 1986, 1987). By suggesting that the dimensions of physical space can be also weighted to reflect the requirements of the current task, the present results contribute to an emerging view of physical space as subject to attentional manipulation (Liverence & Scholl, 2011).

In conclusion, we have presented evidence that, under speeded conditions, the horizontal or vertical distance between two locations in a frontal plane can be assessed only to the extent that the horizontal or vertical positions of the locations are weighted more heavily than the vertical or horizontal positions, so that horizontal or vertical distance makes a disproportionate contribution when the overall distance between the locations is assessed.