The attentional blink (AB; Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992) refers to a decline in accurate reporting of a second target (T2) when it is presented within about half a second of a first target (T1) in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) of distractors. The AB has been the topic of extensive research in the past couple of decades, and its popularity is ever-growing. Its appeal is in part linked to the belief that it reflects a severe and fundamental limitation in human cognitive abilities.

The AB is a remarkably robust phenomenon, but its underlying causes are a source of debate. One class of theories points to capacity limitations as the root cause of the AB; other theories implicate the distractors that intervene between T1 and T2. In the present study, we sought to distinguish between these two views by systematically investigating the role of intervening distractors in the AB.

Early theories of the AB, such as the two-stage model (Chun & Potter, 1995) and the central interference theory (Jolicoeur, 1998), proposed that when attentional mechanisms are engaged in consolidating T1, they are not available for consolidating T2 into visual short-term memory (VSTM). As a consequence, consolidation of T2 is subject to a delay, during which the T2 representation decays and is vulnerable to overwriting by trailing items. These capacity-based theories are bolstered by the finding that the magnitude of the AB is related directly to the difficulty of T1 processing. For example, inserting a blank frame after T1 (i.e., removing the T1 mask) leads both to improved T1 identification and to a reduced AB (Chun & Potter, 1995; Raymond et al., 1992). The reduction in AB is explained as follows. Removal of the trailing mask allows faster processing of T1, with a corresponding reduction in the delay of T2 processing. The shorter delay permits T2 to be consolidated sooner, thus reducing the probability of decay and overwriting by trailing items.

Capacity-based theories have been challenged by recent findings; when three successive targets (T1, T2, T3) are inserted in an RSVP stream of distractors, no AB is in evidence for either T2 or T3. In contrast, when T2 is replaced by a distractor, T3 exhibits a substantial AB (Di Lollo, Kawahara, Ghorashi, & Enns, 2005; Olivers & Meeter, 2008; Olivers, van der Stigchel, & Hulleman, 2007). What capacity-based theories cannot explain is why an AB is not found when the system's resources are taxed with three targets but crops up when processing demands are reduced by omitting one of the targets.

The absence of an AB with multiple successive targets has prompted a new set of accounts, known collectively as input-control theories. These accounts maintain that the AB is triggered not by T1, but by the distractor directly following T1. Two original input-control models have been proposed: the temporary loss of control (TLC) model (Di Lollo et al., 2005) and the boost and bounce (BB) model (Olivers & Meeter, 2008; Olivers et al., 2007).

According to the TLC model (Di Lollo et al., 2005), target selection is mediated by an input filter set to pass targets and reject distractors. The filter is actively maintained by signals from a central executive, which is also required for consolidating targets into VSTM. A key assumption is that the central executive cannot keep on issuing maintenance signals while consolidating an item into VSTM. As a consequence, while T1 is being consolidated, the filter becomes vulnerable to disruption. If the item following T1 is another target, the filter's configuration remains unchanged. If, however, the item following T1 is a distractor, the filter's configuration is disrupted. The AB occurs when T2 is presented after the filter has been disrupted but before there has been sufficient time to reconfigure it once T1 has been processed.

The BB model of Olivers et al. (2007; Olivers & Meeter, 2008), on the other hand, proposes that the post-T1 distractor is enhanced by the excitatory attentional response evoked by T1. This, in turn, triggers a strong inhibitory response, which affects subsequent targets. In brief, both the TLC and the BB models claim that the AB occurs because the post-T1 distractor triggers critical changes in input control, causing ensuing targets to be missed until normal control is restored.

As is clear from the foregoing, assessing the role of intervening distractors in the AB has important theoretical implications. The goal of the present study was to determine whether distractors cause the AB directly, as is proposed in distractor-based models, or can modulate the AB only indirectly by modulating T1 processing, as is proposed in capacity-based models

To achieve this goal, our principal manipulation was whether or not a distractor was presented in one of the two RSVP frames following T1 (i.e., at lag 1 or at lag 2). A distractor could be presented at lag 1 (100 ms after T1 onset; the T1 + 1 condition), at lag 2 (200 ms after T1 onset; the T1 + 2 condition), or at neither lag (no-distractor condition). The intertarget lag was either short (lag 3; in which case, T2 was presented during the period of the AB) or long (lag 9; in which case, T2 was presented beyond the period of the AB). The display sequence for each of the three conditions is illustrated in Fig. 1 for the short intertarget lag.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of the short intertarget lag display sequence in Experiment 1. T1, first target; T2, second target

Theoretical considerations

Predictions from the distractor-based and the capacity-based models are outlined below. In the present work, the magnitude of the AB was estimated by subtracting the percentage of correct T2|T1 responses at lag 3 (at which T2 was presented during the period of the AB) from the corresponding score at lag 9 (at which T2 was presented beyond the period of the AB).

No-Distractor condition

Distractor-based theories predict that no AB should occur in this condition simply because there are no intervening distractors to disrupt input control. Predictions from capacity-based models depend on whether the processing of T1 has been completed during the 300-ms period between the onset of T1 and the onset of T2. In conventional studies, the distractor presented directly after T1 acts as a mask that slows the processing of T1. This delays the consolidation of T2, thereby extending the period of the AB. The absence of a mask in the no-distractor condition should permit faster processing of T1 with correspondingly smaller AB than in the T1 + 1 condition.

T1 + 1 condition

Both theories predict that an AB should occur in the T1 + 1 condition and that it should be larger than in the no-distractor condition. According to distractor-based theories, the AB should occur because input control is disrupted by the distractor presented directly after T1. According to capacity-based theories, the AB should occur because the processing of T1 is delayed by the mask in the T1 + 1 condition, but not in the no-distractor condition.

T1 + 2 condition

Distractor-based theories predict that the distractor presented immediately before T2 will cause a disruption in input control and, hence, an AB at least as large as that in the T1 + 1 condition. This is because, in the T1 + 1 condition, there is a 100-ms blank during which input control could conceivably begin to be restored. In contrast, no such period is available in the T1 + 2 condition, in which T2 is presented directly after the disruptive distractor. According to capacity-based theories, the distractor in the T1 + 2 condition is presented too late to be an effective mask for T1 (Enns, 2004). Given negligible masking of T1, the T1 + 2 condition becomes functionally equivalent to the no-distractor condition, thus yielding ABs of comparable magnitudes.

To anticipate, in Experiment 1, where the first task was to identify whether T1 was A, B, C, or D and the second task was to detect whether T2 was or was not Y, we found strong evidence in favor of capacity-based models. First, an AB was observed even when no distractors were presented between T1 and T2. Second, an intervening distractor modulated the AB only if it affected T1 performance. But distractor-based models could also account for these results on the grounds of a task switch between T1 and T2 that might have disrupted input control. In Experiment 2, we removed the task switch and still observed an AB in the no-distractor condition, confirming that the presence of intervening distractors is not necessary to produce the AB. However, Experiment 2 also produced evidence consistent with distractor-based accounts. To wit, we found that the T1 + 2 distractor modulated the AB, even though it did not affect T1 performance. These results were replicated in Experiment 3 with a conventional AB paradigm in which two letter targets were inserted into a stream of digit distractors. This pattern of results is hard to reconcile with either model, but both models can be made to account for the data when appropriately modified.

Experiment 1

The present work was part of a series of experiments involving event-related potentials (ERPs) in which the frequency of the T2 letter was manipulated in order to compute the frequency-related P3 difference wave. In Experiment 1, both the T1 and the T2 stimuli were chosen so as to be compatible with the ERP experiments. The targets were uppercase letters inserted in an RSVP stream of digit distractors. The T1 letter was A, C, D, or E. The T2 letter could be Y (on 25% of the trials) or H, J, K, L, M, N, P, R, S, T, U, or V (on 75% of the trials). Participants identified T1 and indicated whether or not T2 was a Y.

Method

Participants

Forty-four undergraduate students at Simon Fraser University participated for course credits or financial compensation. All reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive as to the purpose of the experiment. Three participants were excluded for not complying with instructions, leaving 41 participants in the final sample.

Apparatus and stimuli

The experiment was programmed in E-Prime 2.0 and was run on a Pentium PC computer, with a 17-in. VGA monitor. The RSVP stream, presented in the center of the screen, consisted of digit distractors (2–9) and two uppercase letter targets, presented in 20-point bold Courier New font. All the stimuli were white and were presented on a darker gray background. T1 was chosen randomly from the letters A, C, D, and E. T2 was chosen from the letters H, J, K, L, M, N, P, R, S, T, U, V, and Y. The letter Y was presented on 25% of the trials (infrequent T2 response category), and one of the other letters was presented randomly on the other 75% of the trials (frequent T2 response category). Infrequent and frequent T2 response category trials were presented randomly in each block.

Each item in the RSVP stream remained on the screen for 50 ms and was separated from the next item by an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 50 ms, yielding a presentation rate of 10 items/s. The number of distractors preceding T1 was determined randomly on each trial and varied between 18 and 24. On any given trial, the distractors were selected randomly, with replacement, from the set of digits 2–9, with the constraint that the selected digit was not one of the two preceding items. T2 was then presented either at lag 3 (short lag) or at lag 9 (long lag). In the no-distractor condition, the two digits following T1 (i.e., in the lag 1 and lag 2 positions) were replaced by 50-ms blank frames. In the T1 + 1 condition, the digit in the lag 2 position was replaced by a 50-ms blank frame, and in the T1 + 2 condition, the digit in the lag 1 position was replaced by a 50-ms blank frame. On short-lag trials, T2 was presented at lag 3 and was followed by a one-digit distractor that acted as a mask. On long-lag trials, digits continued to be presented from lag 3 to lag 8, and then T2 was presented, followed by a digit mask. Figure 1 illustrates the RSVP stream for the short lag in all three conditions.

Design and procedure

A within-subjects design was employed with three factors: T1 distractor position (no distractor, T1 + 1, or T1 + 2), T1–T2 lag (lag 3 or lag 9), and T2 response category frequency (T2 frequent or T2 infrequent). Each participant took part in one session, consisting of two blocks of 240 trials preceded by one practice block of 24 trials. All conditions were intermixed randomly in each block of trials.

At the beginning of the session, participants were required to read the instructions displayed on the screen and were invited to ask questions to clarify procedural details. Participants were instructed to ignore the distractors and to report, without speeded pressure, the identity of T1 by pressing the Z, X, C, or V key on the keyboard with the left hand for A, C, D, and E responses, respectively, and to report whether T2 was a Y or not by pressing the N or M key with the right hand for Y and not-Y responses, respectively.

At the beginning of each trial, a small fixation cross was presented in the center of the screen, indicating the location at which the RSVP stream was about to appear. Participants initiated each trial by pressing the space bar. The RSVP stream started 200–500 ms after trial initiation. Eight hundred ms after the stream ended, a first question—"First letter: A, C, D, or E?"—was displayed until response. A second question—"Second letter: Y or not Y?"—was displayed, immediately after the first response, until the second response was executed, at which point the fixation cross reappeared, indicating readiness for the next trial. The instructions emphasized the importance of responding “Y” only when a Y was seen.

Results

T1 performance was about the same in the two T2-frequency conditions (no main effect or interactions with this factor; see below). For this reason, the T1 scores were combined over frequent and infrequent T2 trials. Analyses of T2 performance were performed only for T2-infrequent trials on which T1 was reported correctly. T2-frequent trials were excluded from the analyses because participants were instructed to make a “Y” response only if a Y was seen. Therefore, if participants did not consciously “see” the second letter, they would answer “not-Y”. This renders the frequent-T2 trials uninformative with respect to the AB, because a "not-Y" response could indicate either that a letter other than Y had been displayed or that the letter Y had been displayed but had been missed because an AB had occurred. Accuracy of T2 identification on T2-frequent trials was very high (94% or higher in all conditions), indicating that participants were following instructions.

T1 Performance

The percentages of correct identifications of T1, averaged over participants, are shown in Table 1. The data were analyzed in a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with two within-subjects factors: lag (two levels: lag 3 and lag 9) and T1 distractor (three levels: no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2). The analysis revealed only a main effect of T1 distractor, F(2, 80) = 50.39, MSE = 14.35, p < .001. No other effects were significant (all Fs < 1). Pairwise comparisons confirmed that T1 accuracy was lower in the T1 + 1 condition (88.9%) than in the no-distractor (94.5%; t(40) = 8.21, p < .001) or in the T1 + 2 condition (93.3%; t(40) = 6.64, p < .001). The T1 + 2 and the no-distractor conditions did not differ significantly at lag 3, t(40) = 1.68, p > .05, but performance in the no-distractor condition was more accurate by 1.2% at lag 9, t(40) = 2.19, p = .04. When frequent trials were excluded from the analysis, the pattern of results was unchanged, except that the T1 + 2 and the no-distractor conditions did not differ significantly either at lag 3, t(40) = .45, p > .05, or at lag 9, t(40) = 1.83, p > .05.

Table 1 Mean percentage of correct T1 responses for each lag in the no distractor (ND), T1 + 1, and T1 + 2 conditions in each of the three experiments

T2 performance

For reasons noted above, only the data for the T2-infrequent condition were used in the analysis of T2 performance. The result of principal interest was the magnitude of the AB obtained in the three conditions (no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2; see the "Theoretical Considerations" section in the introduction). Accordingly, we computed the magnitude of the AB by subtracting each participant's percentage of correct T2|T1 responses at lag 3 from the corresponding score at lag 9, separately for each of the three conditions. Those scores, averaged over participants, are illustrated in Fig. 2. The percentages of correct identifications of T2|T1, averaged over participants, are shown in Table 2. Perhaps the most notable aspect of the results in Fig. 2 is that an AB was obtained even when no distractors intervened between the two targets (no distractor condition), indicating that the presence of intervening distractors is not necessary to produce an AB.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Magnitude of the attentional blink for each condition in Experiment 1. Two separate unspeeded responses were required on each trial. The first response was a four-alternative discrimination to the first letter (T1), chosen randomly from A, C, D, or E. The second response was a two-alternative discrimination as to whether the second letter (T2) was a Y (25% of the trials) or not (75% of the trials)

Table 2 Mean percentage of correct T2|T1 responses for each lag in the No Distractor (ND), T1 + 1, and T1 + 2 conditions in each experiment

The pattern of results in Fig. 2 mimics the pattern observed for T1 accuracy (Table 1) and suggests that an intervening distractor can modulate the AB only indirectly, via its effect on T1 processing. This is in line with capacity-based models, which propose that the magnitude of the AB should be related inversely to the accuracy of T1 identification. These observations were supported by the outcome of a within-subjects one-way ANOVA with position of the T1 distractor at three levels: no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2. The analysis revealed a significant effect of T1 distractor, F(2, 80) = 4.42, MSE = 171.80, p = .015. A one-sample t test confirmed that an AB was obtained in the no-distractor condition, t(40) = 2.94, p = .005. Planned pairwise comparisons confirmed that the AB in the T1 + 1 condition was significantly larger than that in either the no-distractor condition, t(40) = 2.59, p = .013, or the T1 + 2 condition, t(40) = 2.34, p = .024, which did not differ significantly from one another, t(40) = 0.74, p > .05.

Discussion

In the present experiment, we used two intertarget lags (lags 3 and 9) and presented an intervening distractor at lag 1 (T1 + 1 condition) at lag 2 (T1 + 2 condition), or at neither lag (no-distractor condition). Our objective was to determine whether intervening distractors are necessary to produce an AB and, if so, whether distractors modulate the AB directly, as is proposed in distractor-based theories, or indirectly by affecting T1 processing, as is proposed in capacity-based theories.Footnote 1

As was expected, accuracy for T1 was lowest in the T1 + 1 condition, in which the distractor/mask was presented directly after T1. Importantly, an AB was observed in all the distractor conditions, including the no-distractor condition, in which, at lag 3, T1 and T2 were separated by an uninterrupted blank interval (see Nieuwenstein, Potter, & Theeuwes, 2009, for a similar finding). The fact that an AB was observed in the no-distractor condition clearly demonstrates that intervening distractors are not critical for the AB, a finding that is contrary to the fundamental assumption made in distractor-based models (Di Lollo et al., 2005; Olivers & Meeters, 2008; Taatgen, Juvina, Schipper, & Martens, 2009).

Furthermore, the finding that the AB was of approximately equal magnitude in the no-distractor and T1 + 2 conditions strongly suggests that intervening distractors not only are unnecessary for the AB, but also can affect the AB only indirectly by increasing T1 difficulty. Had distractors interfered directly with T2 identification, we should have seen a larger AB when an intervening distractor was presented (the T1 + 2 condition) than when no intervening distractor was presented. This should be the case even in the absence of any effect of the distractor on T1 accuracy.

Although the results of Experiment 1 seem to provide compelling evidence against the proposition that intervening distractors produce or modulate the AB directly, it is possible that the outcome might have been influenced by a task switch that occurred between T1 and T2. The presence of a task switch is known to modulate the AB (e.g., Juola, Botella, & Palacios, 2004; Kawahara, Zuvic, Enns, & Di Lollo, 2003; Potter, Chun, Banks, & Muckenhoupt, 1998; Vachon, Tremblay, & Jones, 2007; Visser, Bischof, & Di Lollo, 1999). To be sure, in the present experiment, both T1 and T2 were letters, but the specific tasks associated with the two targets were different. The T1 task was to identify the target in a four-alternative forced choice among the letters A, C, D, and E. The T2 task, on the other hand, was to detect whether T2—chosen from the letters H, J, K, L, M, N, P, R, S, T, U, V, and Y—was or was not a Y. It is therefore possible that an online reconfiguration of the attentional filter (from an “A, C, D, E” filter to a “Y” filter) and, possibly, a reconfiguration of the stimulus–response mapping might have occurred in Experiment 1. It is worth noting, at least in passing, that a similar task switch may have been present in Raymond et al. (1992) study, because the attentional filter for T1 (“white” filter) was different from the filter for T2 (“X” filter).

It is of interest to note that an AB triggered by task switching can be encompassed within the TLC model in which the AB is the result of a disruption of the attentional filter, set to select targets with maximal efficiency. If T1 and T2 require different filters, the filter set to select T1 at the beginning of the trial would have to be reconfigured upon detection of T1, in order to maximize selection efficiency for T2. Given that reconfiguration cannot take place during the AB, there may not be sufficient time to reconfigure the filter when T2 is presented at lag 3. This could lead to an AB-like effect even in the no-distractor condition.

Experiment 2

A task switch between T1 and T2 was eliminated in Experiment 2 by making the T2 task the same as the T1 task. In both cases, participants were required to indicate whether the target was A, C, D, or E. The four letters were presented randomly as T1 and T2. However, to avoid repetition blindness effects (Kanwisher, 1987), all trials on which T1 and T2 shared the same identity were removed from the analyses. To the extent that task switching produced an AB in the no-distractor condition in Experiment 1, elimination of the task switch should result in a corresponding elimination of the AB in Experiment 2.

Method

Participants

Forty-eight undergraduate students at Simon Fraser University participated in this experiment for course credit or financial compensation. All reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive as to the purpose of the experiment.

Apparatus and stimuli

The apparatus and stimuli were the same as those in Experiment 1, with the exception that T2 was chosen from the same set of four letters (A, C, D, and E) as T1.

Design and procedure

A within-subjects design was employed with two factors: T1 distractor (no distractor, T1  + 1, or T1 + 2), and T1–T2 lag (lag 3 or lag 9). Each participant took part in one experimental session, consisting of two blocks of 192 trials preceded by one practice block of 24 trials. All conditions were randomly intermixed in each block of trials. All 16 combinations of T1 and T2 letters were presented equally often in each block. Therefore, T1 and T2 had a different identity on 75% of the trials and were the same on the other 25% of the trials.

The procedure was the same as that in Experiment 1, except that participants were required to report the identity of T2 by pressing the N, M, [,], or [.] key with the right hand for A, C, D, and E in response to the screen prompt "Second letter: A, C, D, or E?"

Results

To avoid confounding with repetition blindness effects, trials on which T1 and T2 had the same identity were excluded from the analyses. Order of report was ignored when scoring the responses.

T1 performance

The percentages of correct identifications of T1, averaged over participants, are shown in Table 1. As was the case in Experiment 1, T1 accuracy was lowest in the T1 + 1 condition, in which T1 was masked by the immediately following distractor, and was equivalent in the other two conditions. The data were analyzed in a repeated measures ANOVA with two within-subjects factors: lag (two levels: lag 3 and lag 9) and T1 distractor (three levels: no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2). The analysis yielded a significant effect of T1 distractor, F(2, 94) = 33.99, MSE = 19.04, p < .001, but neither the effect of lag (F < 1) nor the lag × T1 distractor interaction, F(2, 94) = 3.03, MSE = 11.81, p > .05, reached significance. As in Experiment 1, planned pairwise comparisons confirmed that T1 accuracy was lower in the T1 + 1 condition (84.8%) than in either the no-distractor condition (89.5%), t(47) = 6.67, p < .001, or the T1 + 2 condition (89.1%), t(47) = 6.16, p < .001, which did not differ significantly from one another, t(47) = 0.83, p > .05.

T2 performance

The magnitude of the AB was calculated as in Experiment 1. The AB scores, averaged over participants, are illustrated in Fig. 3. The percentages of correct identifications of T2|T1, averaged over participants, are shown in Table 2, separately for each condition and lag.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Magnitude of the attentional blink for each condition in Experiment 2

As was the case in Experiment 1, an AB was obtained in all three conditions. Unlike in Experiment 1, however, the AB in the T1 + 2 condition was larger than that in the no-distractor condition, even though T1 accuracy was not affected by the T1 + 2 distractor. This means that intervening distractors can modulate the AB directly in the absence of a task switch. Notably, intervening distractors can modulate the magnitude of the AB even though they are not necessary for producing the AB itself, as evidenced by the AB obtained in the no-distractor condition (Fig. 3).

These observations were supported by the outcome of a within-subjects one-way ANOVA with position of the T1 distractor at three levels: no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2. The analysis revealed a significant effect of T1 distractor, F(2, 94) = 8.94, MSE = 84.05, p < .001. A one-sample t test confirmed that an AB was obtained in the no-distractor condition, t(47) = 4.30, p < .001. Planned pairwise comparisons confirmed that the AB in the T1 + 1 condition was significantly larger than that in the no-distractor condition, t(47) = 3.74, p < .001, but did not differ from that in the T1 + 2 condition, t(47) = 1.12, p > .05. The AB was significantly larger in the T1 + 2 condition than in the no-distractor condition, t(47) = 2.97, p = .005.

The principal objective of Experiment 2 was to find out whether the ABs obtained in Experiment 1 were due, at least in part, to a task switch between T1 and T2. A direct comparison of the two experiments revealed no evidence of task switching on the magnitude of the AB. The results of the two experiments were compared directly in an ANOVA with one between-subjects factor at two levels (experiment: 1 and 2) and one within-subjects factor at three levels (condition: no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2). The analysis revealed a significant effect of condition, F(2, 174) = 11.17, MSE = 124.40, p < .001. Neither the effect of experiment nor the interaction effect reached significance (both Fs < 1). Planned pairwise comparisons confirmed that a larger AB was obtained in the T1 + 2 condition (11.7%) than in the no-distractor condition (7.6%), t(88) = 2.43, p = .017.

Discussion

Two aspects of the results of Experiment 2 are especially relevant. First, a significant AB was obtained in the no-distractor condition. Notably, the absence of an interaction effect in the combined analysis of Experiments 1 and 2 indicates that the magnitude of the AB obtained in the no-distractor condition in Experiment 2 was comparable to that obtained in the corresponding condition in Experiment 1. This finding strongly suggests that task switching was not selectively responsible for the ABs obtained in Experiment 1. By the same token, the presence of an AB in the no-distractor condition in both experiments confirms that distractors are not necessary to produce an AB. This outcome is clearly consistent with predictions from capacity-based theories, but not from distractor-based theories.

Second, the results of the combined analysis show that a larger AB was obtained in the T1 + 2 condition than in the no-distractor condition. The absence of a corresponding difference in T1 performance (see above) is consistent with the hypothesis that the intervening distractor in the T1 + 2 condition mediated the AB directly, rather than indirectly, through masking of T1. As is shown in Table 1, T1 performance never exceeded 90%, suggesting that the equivalent performance in the T1 + 2 and the no-distractor conditions was not the result of a ceiling effect. This pattern of results is consistent with distractor-based theories, but not with capacity-based theories, which predict that intervening distractors modulate the AB only indirectly by masking T1.

Experiment 3

The larger AB in the T1 + 2 condition, relative to the no-distractor condition, in Experiment 2 was interpreted as favoring distractor-based models. That interpretation, however, depended critically on the null finding that accuracy of T1 identification was approximately the same in those two conditions. Although the sample size in Experiment 2 was large (N = 48) and the null effect was likely not due to a ceiling effect, we thought it prudent to replicate those findings. This was done in Experiment 3, using a conventional AB paradigm in which the two targets were chosen from a larger pool of letters (all the letters in the English alphabet, excepting B, O, Q, Y, and Z).

A second objective of Experiment 3 was to examine the hypothesis that the impairment in T2 identification in the T1 + 2 condition, relative to the no-distractor condition, in Experiment 2 might have arisen from forward masking. It is possible that identification of T2 in the T1 + 2 condition was impaired not only because the target was presented during the period of the AB, but also because it was forward-masked by the distractor presented immediately ahead of it. No such masking could have occurred in the no-distractor condition, because T2 was preceded by a blank screen. We checked on this forward-masking option by omitting the distractor immediately preceding T2 at lag 9 on half of the trials. Showing that this manipulation has no effect on T2 identification would provide evidence that forward masking was not an important determinant of the AB in Experiment 2.

Method

Participants

Thirty-four undergraduate students at Simon Fraser University participated in this experiment for course credit or financial compensation. All reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive as to the purpose of the experiment.

Apparatus and stimuli

The apparatus and stimuli were the same as those in Experiments 1 and 2, with two exceptions. First, T1 and T2 were uppercase letters chosen randomly from the English alphabet, excepting B, O, Q, Y, and Z. Second, the distractor immediately preceding T2 was replaced randomly by a blank on half of the trials at lag 9.

Design and procedure

The design and procedure were the same as those in Experiment 2, except that participants reported the identity of each target by pressing the corresponding key on the keyboard when prompted.

Results and discussion

As in Experiment 2, trials on which T1 and T2 had the same identity were excluded from the analyses. Order of report was ignored when scoring the responses.

The forward-masking hypothesis outlined above was not supported by the experimental outcome. Accuracy of T2 identification at lag 9 was approximately the same when T2 was preceded by a distractor/forward-mask (91.1%) as when it was preceded by a blank screen (91.4%), t(33) = 0.42; p > .05, This outcome strongly suggests that the larger AB in the T1 + 2 condition than in the no-distractor condition in Experiment 2 was unlikely to have been caused by forward masking of T2 by the immediately preceding distractor in the T1 + 2 condition.

T1 performance

The percentages of correct identifications of T1, averaged over participants, are shown in Table 1. As was the case in Experiments 1 and 2, T1 accuracy was lowest in the T1 + 1 condition, in which T1 was masked by the immediately following distractor, and was equivalent in the other two conditions. The data were analyzed in a repeated measures ANOVA with two within-subjects factors: lag (two levels: lag 3 and lag 9) and T1 distractor (three levels: no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2). The analysis yielded a significant effect of T1 distractor, F(2, 66) = 36.71, MSE = 20.97, p < .001, but neither the effect of lag, F(1, 33) = 3.08, MSE = 24.50, p > .05, nor the lag × T1 distractor interaction, F < 1, reached significance. As in Experiments 1 and 2, planned pairwise comparisons confirmed that T1 accuracy was lower in the T1 + 1 condition (87.0%) than in the no-distractor condition (93.2%), t(34) = 6.50 p < .001, or in the T1 + 2 condition (92.3%), t(34) = 4.82, p < .001, which did not differ significantly from one another, t(34) = 1.86, p > .05.

One of the principal findings in the present study was that, contrary to expectations from capacity-based theories, a larger AB was obtained in the T1 + 2 condition than in the no-distractor condition, without a corresponding difference in T1 performance. To confirm the robustness of the latter outcome, we combined the T1 data for Experiments 2 and 3 in a single ANOVA with one within-subjects factor (condition: T1 + 2 and no distractor) and one between-subjects factor (experiment: 2 and 3). Even though the sample size was increased to N = 82, there was still no sign of a significant difference in T1 performance between the no-distractor condition (91.6%) and the T1 + 2 condition (91.1%), F < 1.

T2 performance

The magnitude of the AB was calculated as in Experiments 1 and 2. The AB scores, averaged over participants, are illustrated in Fig. 4. The percentages of correct identifications of T2|T1, averaged over participants, are shown in Table 2, separately for each condition and lag.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Magnitude of the attentional blink for each condition in Experiment 3

The overall pattern of results was similar to that in Experiment 2 (Fig. 3). Notably, a substantial AB was obtained in the no-distractor condition, consistent with capacity-based, but not with distractor-based, theories. On the other hand, a larger AB was again obtained in the T1 + 2 condition than in the no-distractor condition in the absence of a corresponding difference in T1 performance, consistent with distractor-based, but not with capacity-based, theories.

These observations were supported by the outcome of a within-subjects one-way ANOVA with position of the T1 distractor at three levels: no distractor, T1 + 1, and T1 + 2. The analysis revealed a significant effect of T1 distractor, F(2, 64) = 19.09, MSE = 98.86, p < .001. A one-sample t test confirmed that an AB was obtained in the no-distractor condition, t(32) = 6.17, p < .001. Planned pairwise comparisons confirmed that the AB in the T1 + 1 condition was significantly larger than those in both the no-distractor condition, t(33) = 5.70, p < .001, and the T1 + 2 condition, t(33) = 2.84, p = .008. As in Experiment 2, there was also a significantly larger AB in the T1 + 2 condition than in the no-distractor condition, t(32) = 3.55, p = .001.

In summary, the results of Experiment 3 confirmed that forward masking was not an important factor in the AB obtained in Experiment 2. They also added to the robustness of the theoretically important null finding that accuracy of T1 identification was approximately equal in the T1 + 2 and the no-distractor conditions.

General discussion

The goal of the present study was to determine whether distractors presented during the period of the AB modulate the magnitude of the AB directly, as is postulated in distractor-based theories (e.g., Di Lollo et al., 2005; Olivers & Meeters, 2008; Taatgen et al., 2009), or affect T2 performance only indirectly via their effect on T1 difficulty, as is postulated in capacity-based theories (Chun & Potter, 1995; Jolicoeur, 1998, Nieuwenstein & Potter, 2006; Nieuwenstein et al., 2009; see also Wyble, Bowman, & Nieuwenstein, 2009). To this end, we manipulated the presence/absence and temporal position of an intervening distractor that was presented at lag 1, at lag 2, or at neither of these two lags. T2 was presented either at lag 3 or at lag 9. The magnitude of the AB was calculated by subtracting the T2 score at lag 3 from the corresponding score at lag 9.

Experiment 1 showed that, consistent with the findings of Nieuwenstein et al. (2009), an AB was obtained in the no-distractor condition, in which the two distractors between T1 and T2 were replaced by blanks. This finding is consistent with capacity-based, but not with distractor-based, theories. On the other hand, the finding that a larger AB occurred in the T1 + 2 condition than in the no-distractor condition in the absence of a corresponding difference in T1 performance is consistent with distractor-based, but not with capacity-based, theories. Experiment 2 showed that the ABs obtained in Experiment 1 were not due to task switching. Experiment 3 confirmed the theoretically important finding that accuracy of T1 identification was approximately the same in the T1 + 2 and no-distractor conditions and ruled out forward masking of T2 as a determinant of the AB in the T1 + 2 condition.

An important result in all three Experiments was that an AB occurred even when no distractors were presented between T1 and T2. Nieuwenstein et al. (2009) reported a similar result, but the AB was observed only in a condition in which the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between T2 and its mask was very short (58 ms). Moreover, the T2 mask was not chosen from the set of digit distractors but was the same unfamiliar pattern mask from trial to trial. Here, we show that the results of Nieuwenstein et al. can be replicated using a typical SOA between T2 and its mask (100 ms) and when the T2 mask is chosen from the set of distractor digits from the RSVP stream in which the targets were presented.

Although entirely in line with predictions from capacity-based theories, the finding that an AB occurs with no distractors intervening between T1 and T2 cannot be encompassed within distractor-based theories as presently stated. Predictions from the BB model are disconfirmed because, in that model, the AB is critically dependent on the bounce response triggered by a post-T1 distractor. Omitting the post-T1 distractors should eliminate the AB, which was clearly not the case in the no-distractor condition. Predictions from the TLC model are similarly disconfirmed, because the disruption of the input filter—held to be the root cause of the AB—depends critically on the presence of post-T1 distractors.

Given a small modification, however, the TLC model can account for the AB obtained when no distractors intervene between T1 and T2. Namely, it is possible that, in the absence of intervening distractors, the input filter may decay during the period in which the maintenance signals are discontinued while the central processor is busy with T1. On this option, an AB may be observed in the absence of intervening distractors, provided that the intertarget lag is sufficiently long to allow the filter to decay, but not long enough to allow the central processor to resume issuing control signals.

Just as distractor-based models are challenged by the presence of an AB in the absence of intervening distractors, capacity-based models are challenged by the direct modulation of the AB by distractors intervening between T1 and T2. Clearly, these theories need to be revised, or, at least, some of their assumptions need to be made explicit. One plausible option is that an ongoing attentional episode may be disrupted by a post-T1 distractor. Recall that the reengagement model (Nieuwenstein, 2006) and its computational counterpart, the eSTST model (Wyble et al., 2009), assume that attention remains engaged so long as there is target input. Both of these models could account for the direct modulation of the AB if it is assumed that distracting input presented between T1 and T2 (e.g., the T1 + 2 distractor) can wipe out any residual trace of T1 at the input layer, thereby causing an abrupt termination of attentional engagement.Footnote 2

In conclusion, the results of the present work reveal clear limitations in both capacity-based and distractor-based theories of the AB. The finding that intervening distractors can produce an AB directly without differentially affecting T1 performance creates a problem for capacity-based theories. The problem can be overcome, however, by the additional assumption that intervening distractors can produce an AB by disrupting the ongoing attentional episode initiated by T1. Similarly, the presence of an AB in the absence of intervening distractors creates a problem for distractor-based models. The problem seems intractable for the BB model but can be overcome in the TLC model on the assumption that, in the absence of endogenous control, the attentional filter decays unless maintained exogenously by the onset of subsequent targets or other items that contain task-relevant features.