Skip to main content
Log in

Unexpected Costs of High Working Memory Capacity Following Directed Forgetting and Contextual Change Manipulations

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Greater working memory capacity is usually associated with greater ability to maintain information in the face of interruptions. In two experiments, we found that some types of interruptions actually lead to greater forgetting among high-span people than among low-span people. Specifically, an instruction designed to change mental context resulted in significant forgetting for high-span people but minimal forgetting among the low-span people. Intentional forgetting instructions also resulted in greater forgetting among higher working memory capacity participants than among lower working memory capacity participants. A candidate explanation called the intensified context shift hypothesis is proposed which suggests that high-span people are more context dependent than low-span people.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bjork, R. A., LaBerge, D., &LeGrand, R. (1968). The modification of short-term memory through instructions to forget.Psychonomic Science,10, 55–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewin, C. R., &Beaton, A. (2002). Thought suppression, intelligence, and WMC.Behaviour Research & Therapy,40, 923–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunting, M. F., Conway, A. R. A., &Heitz, R. P. (2004). Individual differences in the fan effect and working memory capacity.Journal of Memory & Language,51, 604–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, J., &Engle, R. W. (1993). Working memory capacity as longterm memory activation: An individual differences approach.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,19, 1101–1114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., &Engle, R. W. (1994). Working memory and retrieval: A resource-dependent inhibition model.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,123, 354–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A., &Fthenaki, A. (2003). Disruption of inhibitory control of memory following lesions to the frontal and temporal lobes.Cortex,39, 667–686

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A., Harries, K., Noyes, J., Racsmány, M., &Frankish, C. R. (2000). The disruption and dissolution of directed forgetting: Inhibitory control of memory.Journal of Memory & Language,43, 409–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., &Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,19, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delaney, P. F., &Knowles, M. E. (2005). Encoding strategy changes and spacing effects in the free recall of unmixed lists.Journal of Memory & Language,52, 120–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., &Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,128, 309–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, W. N., &KuČera, H. (1982).Frequency analysis of English usage. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaccard, J., &Turrisi, R. (2003).Interaction effects in multiple regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., &Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working memory capacity.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,130, 169–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., &Engle, R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,26, 336–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knox, A. V. (2002).Functional magnetic resonance imaging of inhibitory processes during directed forgetting. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, UCLA.

  • Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., &Milne, A. B., &Ford, R. L. (1997). On regulation of recollection: The intentional forgetting of stereotypical memories.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,72, 709–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mensink, G. M., &Raaijmakers, J. G. (1988). A model for interference and forgetting.Psychological Review,95, 434–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., Lange, E., &Engle, R. W. (2004). Working memory capacity and resistance to interference.Journal of Memory & Language,51, 80–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, V. M., &Engle, R. W. (1997). The role of working memory capacity in retrieval.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,126, 211–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, W., &Engle, R. W. (1998). Working memory capacity and suppression.Journal of Memory & Language,39, 418–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahakyan, L., &Delaney, P. F. (2003). Can encoding differences explain the benefits of directed forgetting in the list-method paradigm?Journal of Memory & Language,48, 195–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahakyan, L., &Delaney, P. F. (2005). Directed forgetting in incidental learning and recognition testing: Support for a two-factor account.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition,31, 789–801.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahakyan, L., Delaney, P. F., &Kelley, C. M. (2004). Self-evaluation as a moderating factor in strategy change in directed forgetting benefits.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,11, 131–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahakyan, L., &Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition,28, 1064–1072.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., &Vela, E. (2001). Environmental context dependent memory: A review and meta analysis.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,8, 203–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. L., &Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working-memory capacity task dependent?Journal of Memory & Language,28, 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., &Engle, R. W. (2006). A temporal-contextual retrieval account of complex span: An analysis of errors.Journal of Memory & Language,54, 346–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D., Schneider, D., Carter, S., &White, T. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,53, 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter F. Delaney.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Delaney, P.F., Sahakyan, L. Unexpected Costs of High Working Memory Capacity Following Directed Forgetting and Contextual Change Manipulations. Memory & Cognition 35, 1074–1082 (2007). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193479

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193479

Keywords

Navigation