Have We Been Barking up the Wrong Ancestral Tree? Australopithecines Are Probably Not Our Ancestors

Letter Open Access

Have We Been Barking up the Wrong Ancestral Tree? Australopithecines Are Probably Not Our Ancestors

Author Information
1
Faculty of Medicine & Health Sciences, Ghent University, B9000 Ghent, Belgium
2
Kato Lechonia, 37300 Volos, Greece
3
National Museum of Australia, Canberra, ACT 2601, Australia
4
Studiecentrum Antropologie, B2580 Putte, Belgium
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Views:10364
Downloads:310
Citations: 3
Nature Anthropology 2024, 2 (1), 10007;  https://doi.org/10.35534/natanthropol.2023.10007

Received: 12 September 2023 Accepted: 08 December 2023 Published: 15 December 2023

Creative Commons

© 2024 by the authors; licensee SCIEPublish, SCISCAN co. Ltd. This article is an open access article distributed under the CC BY license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

ABSTRACT: The dominant paradigm regarding human evolution since the split with Pan considers australopithecines as hominins, i.e., the closest relatives and/or direct ancestors of Homo. Historically, this paradigm started from the assumption that the Homo/Pan/Gorilla last common ancestor was a knuckle-walking ape that evolved into the fully upright (orthograde), obligate bipedal genus Homo, whereas Pan and Gorilla remained knuckle-walkers. Obligate terrestrial upright bipedalism, unique for our species, is an odd locomotor behaviour for a primate. Therefore, it had become generally accepted that a cooler and drier African climate had caused deforestation, which had forced our ancestors to develop upright bipedalism as an adaptation to living on open grassland savannah. This view, already held by Lamarck and Darwin, appeared most parsimonious in the almost complete absence of fossils. The discovery in the 20th century of australopithecine fossils, bipedal apes with small brains, in open country in southern and eastern Africa corroborated the savannah paradigm. Therefore, australopithecines are considered hominins. However, it is now recognized that most australopithecines instead lived in a mosaic of forests, grasslands and wetlands, and better knowledge of their fossils clearly indicates that they possessed several climbing adaptations. Moreover, none of the extinct ape species older than Australopithecus and Paranthropus for which postcranial remains have been described (e.g., Morotopithecus, Sahelanthropus, Orrorin, Ardipithecus) were knuckle-walking. On the other hand, upright posture/gait is already present to different degrees even in Miocene apes. Moreover, the notion that hominoid orthogrady is a primitive characteristic is corroborated by the growing consensus that knuckle-walking is not a primitive trait but has evolved in parallel, independently in both Pan and Gorilla. Consequently, it is possible that australopithecines are not transitional between a semi-erect ancestor and upright bipedal humans, but to the contrary, are intermediate between a more upright ancestor and extant semi-erect African apes. In summary, hypotheses that attempt to explain how a semi-erect Homo/Pan last common ancestor transitioned into the bipedal australopithecines as an adaptation to life on the savannah appear to be ill-conceived and moreover seem to have been superfluous from the very start. We review the numerous similarities between australopithecines and extant African apes, suggesting that they are possibly not hominins and therefore not our direct ancestors. We suggest that we may have been barking up the wrong ancestral tree, for almost a century.
Keywords: Human evolution; Last common ancestor; Australopithecine; Orthogrady; Bipedalism; Knuckle walking; Savannah paradigm

1. Introduction

The dominant paradigm of hominin evolution, i.e., the evolution of the genus Homo after the split with Pan—estimated to have occurred between 7 and 5 million years ago (Ma)—takes for granted that obligate orthograde (vertical, upright) bipedalism is the hallmark of humanity, and this forms the basis upon which most other typically human characteristics can be explained. It is precisely because of their habitual orthograde bipedalism that australopithecines are considered to be hominins, i.e., most closely related and/or ancestral to living humans e.g., [1,2]. This second major paradigm (australopithecines are our ancestors) is based on the assumption that australopithecines—orthograde/bipedal hominids with an ape-sized brain—are intermediate in the evolution from a semi-erect quadrupedal chimpanzee-like Homo/Pan last common ancestor towards an obligate orthograde bipedal human with a large brain. During the past decades, however, several of the assumptions on which these paradigms are based have been shown to be no longer valid or at least subject to reconsideration. First, the notion that the last common ancestor of Homo and Pan was a semi-erect, quadrupedal, chimpanzee-like ape is no longer supported by many paleoanthropologists. During the past decades, there is growing consensus that the Homo/Pan ancestor already possessed a largely orthograde body plan and may have been capable of facultative or habitual bipedalism. Apparently, all or most fossil hominoids (apes) were orthograde to some degree [3,4,5,6], at least since Morotopithecus (more than 20 Ma) [7]. The Homo/Pan ancestor, according to recent studies, was most likely an orthograde climber, combining a vertical body posture while climbing bipedally among the branches [5] or with bipedalism while standing on the ground [8]. These insights accord with the arguments for an aquarboreal origin for the orthograde body-plan of apes put forward previously by some of the present authors [9,10,11,12], in which the last common ancestor of Homo/Pan combined vertical climbing with bipedal wading in flooded forests. This insight is further corroborated by the fact that all extant apes—greater and lesser—are capable of bipedal locomotion to some extent [13]. The assumption that any hominoid species capable of bipedalism must be a human ancestor, therefore, appears outdated. Second, there is growing consensus that knuckle-walking in Pan and Gorilla—somewhat counterintuitively—is not a primitive condition of the hominid lineage, but one that evolved in parallel in Gorilla and Pan respectively, after the split with the hominine and the hominin last common ancestor [14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21]. This suggestion was put forward already a century ago [22]. It appears that there was never a transition from a semi-erect (diagonograde) Homo/Pan ancestor to orthograde hominins, but instead there were separate transitions from an already orthograde hominine ancestor to the extant semi-erect chimpanzees and gorillas. The fact that knuckle-walking is a derived feature in Gorilla and Pan is a further indication of orthogrady in at least the last common ancestor of humans and African apes. More generally, the extant African apes are now considered to be strongly derived e.g., [8], instead of being a model for the Homo/Pan ancestor, as has been assumed by most experts since Lamarck. Third, since the 1990s, the hypothesis that australopithecines developed orthograde bipedalism as an adaptation to increasing areas of open grassland (the savannah hypothesis) is no longer tenable, given the evidence that there was no increase in savannah approximately 4 Ma, at which time evidence of bipedalism in australopithecines is already well-established. Moreover, if we accept that the Homo/Pan last common ancestor was not a knuckle-walker, but already an orthograde ape, it has never been necessary to explain how hominin orthogrady evolved from knuckle-walking after the split with Pan. Consequently, the savannah hypothesis was a narrative [23] conceived to explain a transition that never happened. It was, in other words, not only ill-conceived but superfluous from the very start. Taken together, the growing consensus that the Homo/Pan ancestor was, like probably most or all extinct hominoids, already largely orthograde renders obsolete the numerous speculations on how australopithecines evolved orthograde bipedalism. Rather, what needs to be explained is how both human obligate orthograde bipedalism, and knuckle-walking in extant great apes, evolved from the ancestral (aqu)arboreal orthograde morphology and possible facultative/habitual bipedalism of Miocene and Pliocene apes. Taking into account the above considerations, it is rather intriguing that the paradigm that australopithecines are hominins has hardly been questioned. The possibility has to be considered that the bipedal australopithecines were not evolving from a quadrupedal semi-erect chimp-like Homo/Pan last common ancestor towards human obligate bipedality, but, to the contrary, that they were intermediate between already orthograde ape ancestors and extant knuckle-walking chimps and/or gorillas. Consequently, it is possible that they are not our ancestors and that we have been barking up the wrong ancestral tree for almost a century. Here, we reconsider several of the previously formulated arguments that australopithecines are not hominins [10,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33] and propose that it is just as likely and even more probable that they are more closely related to Pan and/or Gorilla [10,24,26,28,29,30,31] or represent an extinct branch of hominids [25,32]. For clarity, bipedalism and orthogrady should not be confused, the latter indicating an upright, vertical body morphology. Dinosaurs, ostriches, and kangaroos are bipedal runners, but they are not orthograde, unlike Homo. On the other hand, an orthograde body does not necessarily indicate a habitual or obligate bipedalism, and may instead relate to vertical tree-climbing and/or branch-hanging (hylobatid brachiation), for example. Moreover, it is important to point out that bipedalism is frequently understood to be synonymous with a ‘bipedal gait’ or ‘bipedal locomotion’ (i.e., moving on two hind limbs). However, it can also mean bipedal posture, as is mostly the case for chimpanzees [34,35], and which may have been the case for most australopithecines [34,35]. Here, we use ‘bipedalism’ for both posture and gait, which is in agreement with the definitions provided by Stamos and Alemseged [36]: Facultative or postural bipeds are species which primarily locomote non-bipedally on the ground but are able to support their weight on two legs while using the forelimbs for other purposes, such as harvesting fruit or moving in the trees (e.g., extant African apes). Habitual bipeds are species which are well-adapted to upright standing/walking on the ground, but may use other means to locomote some of the time, and/or which may still spend time in the trees (e.g., Ardipithecus, Australopithecus). Obligate bipeds are species which only practice upright locomotion on two limbs on the ground (e.g., Homo sapiens, kangaroos, birds). In this review, we largely adopt recent terminology regarding primate taxonomy [6], with important remarks, as summarized in Table 1.
Table 1. Taxonomy and nomenclature of the Hominoidea. Species listed are not exhaustive. Largely based on [6].

2. The Historical Roots and Evolution of the Dominant Paradigms in Paleoanthropology

2.1. The Savannah Paradigm It is of utmost importance to briefly recapitulate the origin and evolution of the currently dominant paradigms of hominin evolution, to understand how australopithecines came to be considered unquestionably as our closest relatives and direct ancestors. We consider the inclusion of a historical review as essential to understanding not only the origin of the savannah paradigm, which claims that our ancestors became bipedal as an adaptation to increasingly open savannah, but also how this hypothesis was at the basis of the paradigm that the bipedal australopithecines are our ancestors. Moreover, both paradigms reinforce each other. Ever since we started thinking about the origin of our species from an evolutionary perspective [37], the same lines of reasoning have been adopted. The savannah paradigm is based on a series of assumptions that once appeared straightforward (parsimonious), and which were already used by Lamarck, Darwin and Wallace. These lines of reasoning are briefly summarized below: 1. Among extant primates, only our species shows obligate orthograde bipedal locomotion. Therefore, bipedality, considered as a unique trait for our genus, must have evolved after the split between our closest relatives and the genera Pan and Gorilla. 2. Both Pan and Gorilla knuckle-walk. Therefore, it is most parsimonious to assume that the last common ancestor of Homo, Pan and Gorilla, and consequently the last common ancestor of Homo and Pan, was a semi-erect, quadrupedal, knuckle-walking ape with an ape-sized brain. 3. A knuckle-walking ancestor makes sense, since knuckle-walking by apes can be understood as a locomotor behavior that is intermediate between full arboreal quadrupedality, as in monkeys, and fully obligate bipedality, as in humans. This is further corroborated by the observation that Pan and Gorilla already live on the forest floor to a certain extent. This conviction is reflected by the numerous versions of the popular and ubiquitous image showing the “progression” from a quadrupedal monkey into fully upright Man via a semi-erect knuckle-walking chimpanzee. 4. For a vertebrate animal, it is most unusual to walk upright on hind legs. Since our species is not arboreal, but terrestrial, this bipedal gait must be an adaptation to living on the ground. 5. Overall, primates are folivorous and fructivorous. It seems rather odd, therefore, that a primate should be ground-dwelling, where leaves and fruit are rare. Consequently, living on the ground was most probably forced upon our ancestors by environmental changes, whereby forests were replaced by open habitats in the regions where our ancestors lived, causing the split of the hominin and panin lineages. 6. All other specifically human characteristics can be best explained as consequences of our upright posture and gait, which freed our forelimbs (hands), which led to increased manual dexterity, which enabled tool (and weapon) production and use, which improved hunting by running over the savannah, which increased the consumption of energy-rich meat, which led to brain size increase, which enabled articulate language. In summary, the dominant paradigm states that our ancestors were forced to adapt to life on open plains, as caused by climate change, that they did so by walking/running on hind legs and that our obligate bipedalism explains how we became human e.g., [2,6]. This story of human evolution, as first envisioned in the absence of relevant fossil evidence or knowledge regarding paleo-climatology, is considered by some to be adequate. For example, Ernst Mayr [38] found Lamarck’s hypothesis “startlingly modern”. More recently, it was stated that “Lamarck’s view is modern in that it is fairly close to what we believe today to be true.” [39]. The discovery of australopithecine fossils in southern and eastern Africa, from 1925 onwards, fit perfectly within this scenario. The southern and eastern African landscapes were assumed to have been open savannah at the time of fossilization, and moreover these were locations where no extant ape species live. This lent further weight to the argument that orthograde bipedalism evolved as a means to survive in open habitats, and to the conviction that bipedalism was the hallmark of humanity, which in itself became the major driver for the development of most other typical human characteristics, such as loss of fur, manual dexterity (tool use) and an extremely large brain. Most importantly, in the context of the present argument, the notion that australopithecines must be our ancestors gained acceptance because it is the most plausible assumption, in view of the above scenario. Still today, it is almost self-evident, and therefore still generally accepted, that the African bipedal and ape-size brained australopithecines, the genera Australopithecus and Paranthropus, existing in Africa between 4.2 Ma and 0.7 Ma, must have been intermediate in the evolutionary transition from a knuckle-walking ape ancestor with a small brain to an obligate, orthograde bipedal and large(r)-brained member of the genus Homo. 2.2. The Dominant Savannah Paradigm Stems from Parsimonious Reasoning Prior to the Discovery of the First Australopithecine Fossil: 1809–1925 Bender et al. [23] reviewed the history of the origin and evolution of the dominant paradigm. The review was co-authored by Phillip V. Tobias, the direct successor in the chair of Raymond Dart (University Witwatersrand, South-Africa) and the custodian of the world’s largest collection of A. africanus specimens. As mentioned above, Jean-Baptiste de Lamarck was probably the first (in 1809) to formulate a hypothesis on human bipedalism within an evolutionary context [37]: “As a matter of fact, if some race of quadrumanous animals, ..., were to lose, by force of circumstances or some other cause, the habit of climbing trees and grasping the branches with its feet in the same way as with its hands, in order to hold on to them; and if the individuals of this race were forced for a series of generations to use their feet only for walking, and to give up using their hands like feet; there is no doubt … that these quadrumanous animals would at length be transformed into bimanous, and that the thumbs of their feet would cease to be separated from the other digits, when they only used their feet for walking.” (cited by Bender [23]; underlining is ours). Another self-evident view, already implicit (see our underlining below) in the writings of Lamarck, concerned the notion that this terrestrial adaptation was one of adaptation to open plains: “Furthermore, if the individuals of which I speak were impelled by the desire to command a large and distant view, and hence endeavoured to stand upright...” (cited by [23]). Charles Darwin (1871) [40] largely embraced Lamarck’s point of view of a changing environment as the reason to develop orthograde bipedalism (underlining is ours): “As soon as some ancient member in the great series of the Primates came, owing to a change in its manner of procuring subsistence, or to a change in the conditions of its native country, to live somewhat less on trees and more on the ground, its manner of progression would have been modified; and in this case it would have had to become either more strictly quadrupedal or bipedal.” (cited by [23]). Darwin’s assertion that bipedalism conferred selective advantage because it freed the hands and thus enabled tool use was also first stated by Lamarck. Alfred Russell Wallace (in 1898) [41] also explicitly mentions open plains as the likely place of human evolution (underlining is ours): “It has usually been considered that the ancestral form of man originated in the tropics, where vegetation is most abundant and the climate most equable. But there are some important objections to this view. The anthropoid apes, as well as most of the monkey tribe, are essentially arboreal in their structure, whereas the great distinctive character of man is his special adaptation to terrestrial locomotion. We can hardly suppose, therefore, that he originated in a forest region, where fruits to be obtained by climbing are the chief vegetable food. It is more probable that he began his existence on the open plains or high plateaux of the temperate or sub-tropical zone.” (cited by [23]). Steinmann (in 1908) [42] envisaged a pithecoid creature as the last common ancestor of Homo and Pan, with a similar quadruped locomotion as today’s apes living in forests. He also hypothesized that climatic changes had gradually thinned the forests and transformed them into savannah regions. The arboreal ancestors of humans were then forced to adapt to the modified conditions by occasionally adopting an upright or semi-upright posture, similar to today’s apes. Few modifications of these intermediate stages, affirmed Steinmann, would be necessary on the road towards permanent bipedalism [23]. In agreement with Darwin [40], he already considered other typically human traits, including the evolution of versatile/dexterous hands and increased mental faculties (increased brain size), a consequence of this crucial phase in early human evolution. Barrell (in 1917) [43] specified hunting and carnivory as characteristics of such a terrestrial bipedal ape (underlining is ours): “Did a similar climatic change in the Tertiary period acting on a species of large brained and progressive anthropoid apes isolated from the regions of continued forest compel them to adapt themselves to a terrestrial life or die? Did the gradual dwindling, leading even to the extermination of forests, in a region from which the forest fauna could not escape, produce a rigorous natural selection which transformed an ape, largely arboreal and frugivorous in habits, into a powerful, terrestrial, bipedal primate, largely carnivorous in habit, banding together in the struggle for existence and by that means achieving success in chase and war?” (cited by [23]). Bender et al. [23] point out that the idea of “the trees leaving the apes” became the foundation of the savannah hypotheses. It was based on a very peculiar view of the luxurious forests as a place of evolutionary stagnation in contrast to the demanding open plains as a place of development of mankind. They concluded: “We argue that the savannah hypotheses reached a high level of consensus in early and modern palaeoanthropology not because they arose through an objective interpretation of empirical evidence, as commonly believed, but mainly because they relied on the straightforwardness implied in the open plains ideas and the lack of alternative hypotheses to contextualize human evolution.” From this brief recapitulation of the early history of paleoanthropological thinking, it is clear that bipedal locomotion is viewed as the single most important derived hominin trait, ever since Lamarck [37]. This view is still at the center of the ruling paradigm, as exemplified by the following recent quote: “The acquisition of terrestrial bipedal locomotion was one of the defining events in hominin evolution.” [44]. It is most important to stress that the ‘Man the mighty hunter’ narrative (see Section 2.3) had already been explicitly formulated e.g., [43], even before the first relevant fossil was known. The fossil finds of bipedal australopithecines corroborated their hominin status (see Section 2.3), at least as they were interpreted through the lens of the dominant paradigm. 2.3. Interpretations of Australopithecine Fossils and of Their Paleo-Ecological Context Appeared to Corroborate the Dominant Savannah Paradigm The first find of an australopithecine by Raymond Dart (in 1925) [45] consisted only of an infant skull without postcranial remains. Dart [45] interpreted it as fitting with the reigning paradigm, that our ancestors adapted to a demanding open landscape by evolving habitual terrestrial bipedalism and carnivory. According to his interpretation, the Taung child (2.3 Ma), Australopithecus africanus, found in a cave on the grassland savannah in Bechuanaland, South-Africa, was an early hominin because it combined an ape-sized brain with at least two characteristics that were considered to be derived in our genus: bipedality, on the basis of the more ventral position of the foramen magnum, and human-like dentition, because of the parabolic shape of the mandibular tooth row instead of the ape-like U-shape. Below (see Section 3.4), we address how these assumptions should be treated with caution. Furthermore, he made a few (more) erroneous assumptions, strengthening his belief that he had discovered a missing link between ape and man, while reinforcing his belief in the savannah paradigm. First, he assumed that the present open, dry grassland environment of Bechuanaland had prevailed at the time of the Taung child [45]: “It will appear to many a remarkable fact that an ultra-simian and pre-human stock should be discovered, in the first place, at this extreme southern point in Africa,..., for one does not associate with the present climatic conditions obtaining on the eastern fringe of the Kalahari desert an environment favourable to higher primate life. It is generally believed by geologists... that the climate has fluctuated within exceedingly narrow limits in this country since Cretaceous times.” As such, the location of that first find fitted perfectly with the prevailing view that bipedalism had originated as an adaptation to more open terrain. It has, however, been recognized for almost four decades now that there was no increase in open country at the time the Taung child fossilized (see Section 3.1). A second flawed assumption by Dart was that the associated broken animal bones, teeth and horns had been used by A. africanus as weapons [46]. More precise analysis of the cave contents has since clarified that the cave had been home to hyenas and that the Taung child had probably been their prey, rather than kin of predator apes e.g., [47,48,49]. Finally, Dart [45] concluded that the Taung child, as an adult, would have been smarter and fiercer than extant African apes: “We must therefore conclude that it was only the enhanced cerebral powers possessed by this group which made their existence possible in this untoward environment.” And “...—a more open veldt country where competition was keener between swiftness and stealth, and where adroitness of thinking and movement played a preponderating rôle in the preservation of the species”. This was a rather expeditious conclusion, which does not follow from his analysis of brain size: “It is therefore reasonable to believe that the adult forms typified by our present specimen possessed brains which... equalled, if they did not actually supersede, that of the gorilla in absolute size.” Moreover, this expeditious deduction contradicts several straightforward observations, such as the fact that the monkey brain of the (quadrupedal) baboon apparently suffices for a life on the savannah or that the brain size of australopithecines is mostly within the range of that of extant African apes (see Section 3.4.1.8) or that some chimps are savannah-adapted [50]. As such, based only on an infant skull, and in combination with several erroneous and hastily formulated assumptions, Dart [45], as many have since, reinforced the savannah paradigm and started a new one, i.e., that australopithecines are hominins, our closest relatives and/or ancestors. Dart’s view was initially not shared by primatologists e.g., [22,51,52], but was immediately embraced by Robert Broom, who confirmed that the Taung child fitted perfectly within the classical ‘belief’: “Before Australopithecus was discovered some of us believed that the ancestor of man would be found in an anthropoid ape which had left the forest and taken to living on the plains and among the rocks; and here we have just such a form. ... Personally, I believe that Australopithecus is very near to the human ancestor, ... Certainly it is not at all nearly allied to the chimpanzee.” [53]. Bender et al. [23] suggest that we probably owe the currently widespread popular view that we descend from open plain hunters to the American playwright and scientific writer, Robert Ardrey, who wrote: “We were born risen apes, not fallen angels, and the apes were armed killers besides.” His books, such as African Genesis and The Hunting Hypothesis [54,55], with detailed accounts of Dart’s paleoanthropological ideas, became worldwide bestsellers, and strongly influenced the popular and scientific discourse of early humans as aggressive primates hunting on African open plains. Ardrey inspired several prominent scientists and science writers, among them Konrad Lorenz, Desmond Morris and Lionel Tiger, and his writings were well reviewed, by, among others, Edward O. Wilson [56]. To Sherwood Washburn, A. africanus was “already a hunter” (quote by [56]). The view that we descend from already murderous apes is also explicitly the theme of more recent work, e.g., Demonic Males: Apes and the Origins of Human Violence [57]. The discovery in 1974 of Lucy (A. afarensis) [58,59], an almost complete skeleton of an upright/vertical/orthograde ape, with the body and brain size of extant African apes (resembling the bonobo quite well, see Section 3.4.2), further perfected the picture of Australopithecus as the missing link between the knuckle-walking, small-brained Homo/Pan last common ancestor and the upright bipedal and large-brained human being. The location, i.e., the Ethiopian badlands, devoid of extant apes, and the timing—in between the Homo/Pan split and the appearance of the first genuine Homo fossils in Africa—added further credibility to the predominant savannah paradigm and to the conviction that australopithecines are our direct ancestors. The view that our ancestors had to adapt to open landscape was further popularized by Yves Coppens [60], co-discoverer of A. afarensis, as the “East Side Story”. In the East Side Story version of the predominant savannah paradigm, ancestors of chimps and gorillas remained in the forests to the west of the African rifts, whereas australopithecines, the putative human ancestors, adapted to the open landscapes to the east of the rifts, in Kenya and Ethiopia and parts of South-Africa. Soon thereafter, new finds, including, among others, that of Sahelanthropus in Toumaï (Chad), 2500 km to the west of the Rift Valley [61], compelled Coppens to abandon the East Side Story, at least with regard to the location in East Africa [62]. However, and illustrative of the stubbornness of the savannah paradigm, he continues to insist that adaptation to open country is the driving force explaining hominin bipedality: “East side story: It is rightly abandoned, in the sense that it is no longer East. But it is preserved as far as the story itself is concerned. The story itself, for me is the common ancestors who lived in the forest, ten million years ago and who had descendants who, for a reason of climate change, were found by chance in forest environments and others in a savannah environment. Additionally, those from a forest environment became ancestors of chimpanzees, then chimpanzees themselves. Additionally, those in open (or savannah) environments, in this case let’s say open forest, stood up to adapt to this much more open environment.” [63]. We note that Coppens weakens the strong grassland savannah paradigm (open country, dry, hot) to ‘open forest’, i.e., much less demanding mosaic savannah than the one imagined by e.g., Dart [45].

3. Critical Considerations as Concerns the View that Australopithecines Are Hominins that Evolved Bipedalism from a Knuckle-Walking Ancestor in Response to Living on Open Plains

3.1. Savannah-based Narratives to Explain the Bipedalism of Australopithecines Lack Substance: There Was No Increase in Savannah at the Time of the Early Australopithecines While australopithecine bipedalism has been regarded as an adaptation to open environment, ever since their first description in 1925, it has been realized that ‘grassland savannah’ is no longer a candidate environment to explain australopithecine bipedality [24,49,64,65,66,67,68]. “In fact, the immense plains and the immense herds on them are relatively recent aspects of the African environment, much more recent than the origin of the human family.” [64]. Tobias, who held the chair of Raymond Dart, was understandably initially sympathetic to the savannah model. Nevertheless, in 1995, he exclaimed: “We were all profoundly and unutterably wrong!”; “Open the window and throw out the savannah hypothesis; it’s dead and we need a new paradigm.” and “All the former savannah supporters (including myself) must now swallow our earlier words in the light of the new results from the early hominid* deposits.” [66]. (*: Here, Tobias uses ‘hominid’ in the sense of ‘hominin’.) Reed [49] concluded that the Pliocene gracile australopithecines “existed in fairly wooded, well-watered regions”, whereas the Pleistocene robust australopithecines existed “in similar environs and in more open regions, but always in habitats that include wetlands”. The evidence for wetland habitats of fossil apes and australopithecines has been extensively reviewed [9,10,11,12]. In summary, it should have been clear since at least the 1990s, e.g., [49,64,65,66], from the correct interpretation of the paleo-ecological context in which the early australopithecines fossilized, that adaptation to open plains could not have been the instigating factor to explain their bipedalism. Still, only recently, White [68] described authors advocating what he considered “the obsolete mind-set of a savanna-based origin of humanity...” as ‘savannaphils’. He concluded: “..., despite valiant efforts at its resurrection, the hypothesis that opening grasslands led to hominid emergence and bipedality now stands effectively falsified.” A further nail to the coffin of the savannah-based narratives that attempt to explain how australopithecines achieved bipedality as an adaptation to running over open plains, was provided by more and better-preserved australopithecine fossils. It is now clear that australopithecines had several ape-like climbing adaptations, indicative for living in forested areas instead of on open, tree-poor terrains. We review this anatomical evidence below (see Section 3.4). 3.2. The Homo/Pan Ancestor Was Not Quadrupedal, Semi-erect, Chimpanzee-like, but Already Occasional/Habitual Orthograde On the basis of more recent fossils, discovered in the 1990s and at the turn of the millennium, it seems that another parsimonious assumption, at the basis of the dominant paradigm, is most probably erroneous: the Homo/Pan last common ancestor was not chimpanzee-like, i.e., knuckle-walking and semi-erect, as has been generally accepted for almost two centuries. Indeed, several pre-australopithecine orthograde apes, indicative of possible occasional or habitual bipedalism, are now known: Sahelanthropus tchadensis (Toumaï, Sahel) [69], between 7.0 and 6.0 Ma, with a brain volume of 320–380 cc, is considered by the authors as a possible hominin, although a more recent analysis classifies this species consistently as a stem hominid [70]. The well-preserved skull showed small teeth and canines, and a rather flat face. The authors suggest that S. tchadensis was bipedal, although this has been disputed [71]. Orrorin tugenensis (Tugen Hills, Kenya) [32], between 6.15 and 5.80 Ma, with an unknown brain volume, is considered by its authors as one of the oldest hominins. It has human-like teeth with thick (3 mm) enamel, smaller than those of australopithecines, although we argue that enamel thickness as a phylogenetic indicator should be interpreted with caution (see Section 3.4.1.1). Three fragments of femur reveal that O. tugenensis was habitually bipedal [32], with the femur closer morphologically to that of humans than to australopithecines. Ardipithecus ramidus (Afar, Ethiopia) [4,72,73], between 4.51 and 4.32 Ma, with a brain volume of 300–330 cc, shows a mosaic of ‘derived’ and ‘primitive’ features that suggest it was a facultative biped [73]. Ardipithecus and Orrorin were possibly “better bipeds” than the later australopithecines [4,8,73]. Even before the discoveries of the abovementioned fossils, it had been suggested that the African ape ancestors were already partially orthograde and/or bipedal [22,28,29,74]. Moreover, orthogrady was present in numerous still older hominoids. The Eurasian and eastern African Miocene fossil apes (23–5 Ma) reveal that the early “crown” hominoids, such as Morotopithecus bishopi (22–18 Ma), stood and/or moved with an orthograde posture [5]. Keith [75] recognized “the antiquity of orthograde posture”, pointing to hylobatids and to late-Oligocene/early-Miocene dryopithecines. Morton [22] had also realized that the bipedalism of early hominoids is not in disagreement with the orthograde posture of extant hylobatids: “The gibbon’s primitive position in the anthropoid group has added value in that it strongly suggests the linking of our modern bipedalism with an early stage of erect arboreal habits in the ancestral anthropomorphous stem.” and that: “Tree life is the only form of environment, and brachiation the only locomotive habit whereby the structures about the hip-joints would be given the opportunity to become gradually modified for an erect posture.” A study of the frequency of bipedalism in almost 500 zoo-housed hominoids and cercopithecines found that all could move bipedally, with hylobatids engaging most frequently in bipedal locomotion [13]. All of these data support an orthograde and, at least partially, arboreal last common ancestor of the hominoids (apes and humans). 3.3. Knuckle-Walking May Not Be an Ancestral Characteristic of the Homo/Pan/Gorilla Ancestor but May Have Evolved in Parallel in Both Pan and Gorilla The almost complete lack of evidence of knuckle-walking in australopithecine fossils may be an important reason why paleoanthropologists conclude that there are no African ape fossils, and that australopithecines cannot be the ancestors of extant African apes. However, in addition to the notion that no fossil apes knuckle-walked (see Section 3.2), convincing arguments have been put forward that knuckle-walking evolved independently in the African great apes (Pan and Gorilla). Both support their weight while standing and walking on the middle phalanx (knuckle) of their fingers (mainly III and IV), i.e., they knuckle-walk. It is important to note that the term “knuckle-walking” implies a mode of locomotion (gait), although extant African apes also stand motionless on their knuckles. Morton [22] uses perhaps the more appropriate term ‘semi-erect posture’. Knuckle-walking is a most unusual mode of posture/locomotion, present in only these two genera among primates. Such an unusual form of locomotion in two closely related taxa, one of which shares a common ancestor with hominins, leads to the straightforward assumption that knuckle-walking represents an intermediate stage between the supposed quadrupedality of ancestral apes and human upright bipedality. In other words, it seems more parsimonious to assume that the Homo/Pan/Gorilla last common ancestor was knuckle-walking, and that Pan and Gorilla retained the ancestral knuckle-walking posture/gait while hominins evolved obligate orthograde bipedality. Some authors have argued in support of proto-knuckle-walking in the Homo/Pan/Gorilla ancestor. For example, Homo, Pan, and Gorilla share several derived wrist characters, including—uniquely among catarrhines—the lack of a distinct os centrale. This absence has been interpreted as offering increased stability in the wrist [76], possibly indicative of a common ancestry of proto-knuckle-walking, from which the living African apes have diverged minimally, according to Begun [77]. Ancestral knuckle-walking has been supported by the claim that A. anamensis and A. afarensis retained specialized wrist morphology, assumed to be associated with knuckle-walking [78]. A study based on ‘morphological integration’ methods supports the common origin of knuckle-walking in gorillas and chimpanzees [79]. Nevertheless, almost a century ago, Morton [22] argued extensively and convincingly that knuckle-walking did not represent an intermediate stage preceding bipedalism, but that it was rather a reversion from bipedalism towards quadrupedalism: “The semierect posture of the great apes is not an advance toward human bipedism, but it is direct evidence of the manner in which exaggerated brachiating adaptations have impaired earlier mechanical advantages for bipedism. The great apes have actually reverted to quadrupedism...” The parallel evolution of the semi-erect posture of the great African apes was also recognized by Morton [22]: “The idea is quite commonly held that the terrestrial semierect posture of the great apes represents an approach toward the upright posture of man. Although the close relationship between the two groups seems to support this idea,... there is nothing homologous in their respective terrestrial postures.” Several recent studies corroborate his century-old and largely overlooked view. Based on an analysis of morphological, behavioral and ecological data, it was concluded that knuckle-walking evolved in parallel in Pan and Gorilla [14,15]. Others [17], comparing monkeys and apes, concluded that the variation evident in African ape wrist morphology can only be explained if the independent evolution of two fundamentally different biomechanical modes of knuckle-walking is recognized: an extended wrist posture in an arboreal environment (Pan) versus a neutral, columnar hand posture in a terrestrial environment (Gorilla). These authors concluded that humans did not evolve from a knuckle-walking ancestor. Simpson et al. [19] agree that forelimb ontogeny and anatomy indicate that knuckle-walking was independently acquired by Pan and Gorilla lineages. Rosen et al. [13] also concluded that knuckle-walking evolved in parallel in Pan and Gorilla. In other words, knuckle-walking in both Pan and Gorilla is an example of homoplasy rather than a consequence of homology. The absence of knuckle-walking from the human evolutionary pathway is also apparent when human infants are on all fours. They invariably place the palms flat on the ground with the fingers completely extended [22,80]. With the exception of Pongo (fist-walkers), and Pan and Gorilla (knuckle-walkers), all primates, including human infants, are digiti-palmigrade [27]. Different reasons for the evolution towards knuckle-walking have been suggested. Morton [22] suggested that knuckle-walking was a consequence of the very heavy upper body of extant African apes, relative to the pelvis and hindlimbs, which resulted in the necessity to support their forebody with their forelimbs, and that knuckle-walking was more practical for gorillas and chimpanzees due to their proportionally longer arms relative to legs: “The great apes have actually reverted to quadrupedism, and the semierect position of their bodies is merely the physical result of their long arms and short legs.” [22]. A comparable conclusion was reached independently by Simpson et al. [19]: “... we argue that it was adopted by African apes as a means of ameliorating the consequences of repetitive impact loadings on the soft and hard tissues of the forelimb by employing isometric and/or eccentric contraction of antebrachial musculature during terrestrial locomotion.” Edelstein [27] suggested that certain features of Pan, e.g., the larger pelvis, the slightly smaller cranial capacity, smaller postcanine teeth, and somewhat larger canines, often interpreted as primitive compared to A. afarensis, “could have accompanied the evolutionary reversal associated with the habitats involved in the selection of knuckle-walking”, whereby he assumed that knuckle-walking was an adaptation to more stooped locomotion in dense forests. Ward et al. [20] analysed the pelvis of Rudapithecus hungaricus (10 Ma), an orthograde ape with a more flexible lower back and a shorter pelvis than extant apes. Modern African apes have long, high iliac blades (pelvis) and a short, stiff lower back, whereas humans have longer, more flexible lower backs. They note that, if humans evolved from an African ape-like body, substantial changes to lengthen the lower back and shorten the pelvis would have been required to acquire orthograde bipedalism, but if humans evolved from an ancestor more like Rudapithecus, this transition would have been much more straightforward. When Rudapithecus came down to the ground, it might have had the ability to stand upright more like humans do. Ward et al. [20] conclude that the longer lower pelvis in great apes must have evolved independently in each of the lineages Pan and Gorilla and that the last common ancestor of Pan and Homo “may have been unlike extant great apes in pelvic length and lower back morphology.” By implication, gorillas and chimpanzees would have evolved from a long-backed ancestor that was already able to stand and possibly walk on two legs, as such not excluding australopithecines as their ancestors. This also would mean that at some later point during their own evolution, they each dropped to their knuckles and independently created their own modes of walking. McCollum et al. [81] compared the type and number of vertebrae (axial morphology) in hominoids. They established 3 to 5 lumbar vertebrae and 5 to 8 sacral vertebrae in P. paniscus, compared to 5 (exceptionally 6) lumbar vertebrae and 5 to 6 sacral vertebrae in H. sapiens. From their findings, along with the six-segmented lumbar column of early Australopithecus and early Homo, they suggest that the last common ancestor of the hominoids possessed a long axial column and long lumbar spine and that reduction in the lumbar column occurred independently in humans and in each ape clade, and continued after separation of the two species of Pan as well. Thorpe et al. [3] concluded that there is now a broad consensus that there is a clear lack of any convincing fossil evidence for knuckle-walking in crown hominoids or early hominins and that “the long reign of the knuckle-walking paradigm must be declared over”. 3.4. Indications that Australopithecines Are Not Hominins, but rather Panins and/or Gorillins: Similarities and Differences between Australopithecines, Extant African Apes and Homo It is intriguing that any doubts that have been put forward with regard to the hominin status of australopithecines are largely ignored by the wider paleoanthropological community, even though the original savannah hypothesis has been largely abandoned (see Section 3.1) and that it is becoming clear that most earlier hominoids were already orthograde to at least some degree (see Section 3.2) and that knuckle-walking is very probably a derived trait in Pan and Gorilla (see Section 3.3). Ever since they were first described, it has been clear that australopithecines have more in common with African apes than with humans. This is also the case for several late australopithecines, some of which are contemporary with early Homo, e.g., [29,82,83]. It has even been suggested that the late australopithecines are the most ape-like: “The evolution of the australopithecine crania was the antithesis of the Homo line. Instead of becoming less apelike, as in Homo, they became more ‘apelike’.” [84]. After some initial reluctance by several primatologists (see Section 2.3), australopithecines became generally accepted as hominins. Despite this general acceptance, there has been ongoing uncertainty and Tobias himself [1] questioned whether A. africanus was indeed a hominin. The principal australopithecine traits that have been considered as hallmarks of humanity are orthograde bipedality and facial morphology (including reduced prognathism and small dentition). The former happens to be plesiomorphic, while the latter is too prone to evolutionary change to be useful as phylogenetic markers. Below, we briefly readdress the suite of anatomical traits shared by australopithecines and extant African apes, and we indicate how strongly these australopithecine traits differ from those of extant and extinct Homo. In addition, many of the differences between australopithecines and extant African apes could be seen as derived in apes, and several of the similarities between australopithecines and Homo, considered to be indicative for the hominin status of australopithecines, could instead be primitive traits of stem hominoids, and therefore shared by most hominoids. A number of more elaborate reviews were provided previously [1,28,29,30]. 3.4.1. The Skull and Facial Features While Dart’s claim that the A. africanus infant was a hominin received some support from the start (see Section 2.3), it was initially mostly strongly opposed. Keith [51] stated: “The skull is that of a young anthropoid ape ... showing so many points of affinity with the two living African anthropoids, the gorilla and chimpanzee, that there cannot be a moment’s hesitation in placing the fossil form in this living group. At most it represents a genus in the gorilla-chimpanzee group.” Hrdlička, founder of American Journal of Physical Anthropology, the first after Dart to examine the Taung skull, concluded: “The skull itself is that of an anthropoid ape approaching rather closely in size and form the chimpanzee” [85]. Woodward [52] could “see nothing in the eye-sockets, nasal bones and canine teeth which was nearer to the human condition than those parts of the skull of a modern young chimpanzee.” (cited by [1]). Overall, the Taung child skull looks very much like that of a bonobo infant (Figure 1). Comparing infants/juvenile skulls to make assertions about phylogeny may be problematic because young individuals may resemble each other more than adult representatives of the same species. On the other hand, derived features are more apparent in adult representatives on top of sexual dimorphism differences, and therefore juvenile characters might be more relevant with regard to phylogenetic considerations.
Figure 1. (a). Front views of skulls of human child, Taung child, bonobo child. (b). Oblique front views of infant skulls of Pan, Gorilla, Homo (top, from left to right), and Australopithecus africanus child (Taung child) (bottom).
Following the above authors [51,52,85], many others have emphasized the striking similarity of australopithecine fossil skulls with those of extant African apes. Johanson and Edey [59] considered the composite skull, reconstructed mostly from A.L.333 specimens (A. afarensis), to look very much like a small female gorilla. An extensive and detailed review of the literature concerning comparative skull morphology was provided by Verhaegen [29,30], supporting the morphological similarities between australopithecines and the African apes. 3.4.1.1. Diet, Dentition and Dental Development The microwear of early australopithecines strongly indicates frugivory [9,86], again in agreement with the faunal assemblages, which suggest a mosaic habitat that is better described as open-forest or woodland rather than savannah or bushland (see Section 3.1), in opposition to the ‘running and hunting on the open grassland savannah’ scenarios, addressed above (see Section 2.3). While dentition is clearly indicative of the diets of a certain species, we think that, in general, one should refrain from over-relying on dentition and skull morphology to decide on its taxonomic position, exactly because skull morphology very much depends on dentition, which in turn is very adaptable to the type of diet. Indeed, McCollum [87] concluded that the overall skull morphology of the robust australopithecines can be considered to be the developmental consequence of a unique combination of very large, especially broad molars and premolars with small incisors and canines. This is not in disagreement with the previous suggestion that the genus Paranthropus, which lumps together the southern African and eastern African robust australopithecines, is paraphyletic [28,29] (see Section 4.3). The molar and premolar enamel of australopithecines is thicker than that of extant apes and this has been a major argument to argue for their hominin status, but in fact it is the thinner enamel of extant African apes that is derived. The conclusion of Martin [88] is unambiguous: “Thin fast-formed (pattern 3) enamel represents the ancestral condition in hominoids; it increased developmentally to thick pattern 3 enamel in the great ape and human clade and that condition is primitively retained in Homo and in the fossil hominoid Sivapithecus (including ‘Ramapithecus’). Enamel thickness has been secondarily reduced in the African apes and also, although at a different rate and extent, in the orang-utan. Thick enamel, previously the most important characteristic in arguments about the earliest hominid*, does not therefore identify a hominid*.” and “Of the living members of the great ape and human clade, only Homo sapiens retains the condition of enamel thickness and development from the common ancestor of the clade and can therefore be regarded as the most dentally primitive member of it”. *: Here, the author uses ‘hominid’ meaning ‘hominin’. In summary, all hominoids had initially thin enamel, the hominids (including the australopithecines) developed thicker enamel, and this was reduced secondarily in all extant great apes but not in our lineage, giving the false impression that australopithecines are our closest relatives. In conclusion, dentition, enamel thickness and tooth microwear are strongly related to diet, but may have limited phylogenetic value, given their malleability and evolvability. Several skull features, in fact, may be the result of homoplasy rather than being useful indicators of homology. Also, the dental development of australopithecines links them more closely to African apes than to the genus Homo. Initially, comparisons of the pattern of tooth formation and eruption in A. africanus with that of humans and apes, was interpreted as indicating a human-like rather than ape-like maturation process, because the Taung child was thought to have died at about 6 years of age [89]. Bromage and Dean [90], however, estimated the age at death for several young specimens of A. africanus, A. afarensis and P. robustus by counting incremental growth lines on the teeth, and could not come up with any ages greater than about 3 years. This finding places Australopithecus on an ape-like schedule of development. Bromage [91] concluded that, in addition to similarities in facial remodelling, Australopithecus in general, had maturation periods similar to those of the extant chimpanzee. Ape-like dental eruption of australopithecines has been confirmed by Smith [92] as well as Conroy and Vannier [93,94]. The robust South African australopithecines display a unique pattern of dental development that is only superficially similar to that of humans, while in A. africanus the overall pattern of dental development is similar to that in chimpanzees [1,95,96]. Smith [97] confirmed that the patterns of dental development of gracile australopithecines and H. habilis remain classified with the African apes. A clear difference between robust australopithecines and the extant African apes are the small canines in the former. This has been interpreted as a similarity with our genus, but may instead be a primitive characteristic, also present, for example, in S. tchadensis [69,71] between 7.0 and 6.0 Ma. It is possible, in fact, that the larger canines in Pan and Gorilla are related to increased aggressive male behaviour and that they are a relatively late development. Indeed, the (less aggressive) bonobos “have relatively small and only slightly dimorphic canine teeth” [98]. Edelstein [27], as mentioned above, argued that the larger pelvis, the slightly smaller cranial capacity, the smaller postcanine teeth and the somewhat larger canines of Pan could have accompanied the evolutionary processes of adaptation to the forest habitats involved in the selection of knuckle-walking. 3.4.1.2. Skull Thickness and Skull Crests Australopithecine skull bone thickness is comparable to that of most mammals including apes, but totally different from the pachyosteosclerosis seen in early-Pleistocene Homo erectus [99] and in several semi-aquatic and aquatic species [100]. Sagittal crests are related to diet, as they predominantly exist for the attachment of powerful jaw muscles needed to chew tough food such as nuts, sedges and bamboo. Skull crests are present in the robust australopithecines, but may have more to do with diet [87], than having phylogenetic value. Still, crests are also present in Gorilla, but not in Homo. 3.4.1.3. Mandibular Jaws and Prognathy Extant African apes have prognathic faces that project forward of the anterior cranial fossa, whereas humans have orthognathic faces, that lie almost entirely beneath the anterior cranial fossa. Australopithecine facial prognathy is variable. It has been shown that prognathy (among several other skull characteristics) is comparable between P. paniscus and the almost complete skull of A. africanus Sts.5 [98]. According to Ferguson [84], A. africanus Sts.5 falls well within the range of P. troglodytes and is markedly prognathous or hyperprognathous. The mandibular ramus morphology of an A. afarensis specimen was described to closely match that of gorillas, with the clear ape-like U-shape [101]. Not only dentition, but also jaw muscles and overall skull morphology (prognathy, skull crests) seem to evolve readily, with dentition and jaw muscles adapting to diet, and prognathy and skull crests in turn adapting to dentition and jaw muscles [87]. Therefore, overall skull morphology may tell us much about the diet of a taxon, but may be too evolvable to have primarily phylogenetic importance. 3.4.1.4. Nasal Region The nasal region of australopithecines is characterized by a flat, non-protruding nasal skeleton which does not differ qualitatively from the extant non-human hominoid pattern, but which is in marked contrast to the protruding nasal skeleton, characteristic and exclusively found in Homo skulls [102]. Dart [45] had claimed that Taung differs from all living apes by the unfused nasal bones, but Schultz [103] pointed out that this claim could not be maintained in view of the very considerable number of cases of separate nasal bones among orang-utans and chimpanzees of ages corresponding to that of the Taung child. Eckhardt [104] showed that the nasal bone arrangement, suggested as a derived key diagnostic of P. robustus, is found in an appreciable number of greater apes, particularly clearly in some chimpanzees, and McCollum [105] noticed the overall similarity of the non-robust Australopithecus subnasal morphological pattern with that of the chimpanzee. Also, the pattern of pneumatization of A. afarensis is found only in the extant apes among other hominoids [106]. Accordingly, Conroy and Vannier [93] were intrigued that in Taung the pneumatization extends into the zygoma and hard palate, because an intrapalatal extension of the maxillary sinus has only been reported in chimpanzees and robust australopithecines among higher primates. 3.4.1.5. Air Sacs The human hyoid (lingual bone or tongue bone) lacks the large and distinctive bulla of the chimpanzee hyoid, into which the air sacs extend [107]. The morphology of the chimpanzee hyoid is typical of that found in great apes. The bulla-shaped body of the hyoid in A. afarensis DIK-1-1, Pan and Gorilla has been interpreted to represent the primitive condition, rather than the more shallow, bar-like hyoid body of modern humans and Pongo [108]. It can be assumed that the bulla-shaped body of A. afarensis DIK-1-1 almost certainly reflects the presence of laryngeal air sacs, which are characteristic of African apes, but absent in humans. It has been suggested that air sacs have been lost during the presumed transition from Australopithecus to Homo, as a preadaptation to speech [107,109], an excellent illustration of what Phillip V. Tobias [1] named ‘australopithecine anthropocentrism’ (see 4.3). We consider the putative presence of air sacs in A. afarensis as another striking similarity with extant apes. 3.4.1.6. Position and Orientation of the Foramen Magnum Dart [45] interpreted the relatively ventral position of the foramen magnum in the infant skull of Taung as conclusive evidence for bipedal locomotion in A. africanus. However, Taung was perhaps 3.5 years of age at death [90], and the foramen magnum may not have yet achieved its adult position. As such its position is not a solid indicator of bipedality in A. africanus [110]. Indeed, young gorillas and chimpanzees have foramina magna more ventral than adults and well within the range of A. africanus Sts.5 e.g., [80,111]. Second, even in adults, the position of the foramen in gracile (Sts.5) and robust (KNM-ER 406) australopithecines has the same index as in bonobos [106]. In Lucy, the foramen magnum is at the same ventral position as Homo, which was considered as a clear indication for upright bipedal locomotion, but Kimbel and Rak [112] stated: “..., in contrast to the data on foramen magnum position, which align A. afarensis and other australopiths with modern humans, the data on orientation of the foramen situate the australopiths in an intermediate position between modern apes and humans, but closer to the former (chimpanzees, specifically)”. The position of the foramen magnum and the midfacial angle in AL 822-1, a female A. afarensis (3.1 Ma), are even more ape-like than in the extant gorilla [112]. The difficulties in linking foramen magnum indices to the degree of orthogrady have been outlined in detail recently [113]. Locomotor adaptations, for example, may not be the sole factors responsible for the anteroposterior position of the foramen magnum in primates. Although the more anterior positioning of the foramen magnum in Homo compared to most other primates may be linked to reduced locomotor versatility of the head and reduced head balancing as a consequence of obligate orthograde bipedalism, it is also possible that this position could be (partially) explained by mandibular and cranial vault shape modifications of the skull, due to changes in diet, mastication, and encephalization [114,115]. It is also of interest to note that the completely arboreal gibbons and the quadrupedal South American monkey, Saimiri, have the most centrally placed foramen magnum of all primates [116], suggesting that this feature has more to do with differences in how the head is carried and with facial length, or may be primitive in apes. In conclusion, the position of the foramen magnum in australopithecines is not particularly human-like as initially claimed, and in general, its value as a defining characteristic of orthograde bipedalism may be limited. 3.4.1.7. Semicircular Canals of the Vestibular Labyrinth Cross-sectional images of the bony labyrinth, obtained by high-resolution computed tomography, showed that the earliest species to demonstrate the modern human morphology, in which an almost 90° rotation of the labyrinth has occurred, is H. erectus/H. ergaster [117]. In contrast, the semicircular canal dimensions in crania attributed to Australopithecus and Paranthropus resemble those of the extant great apes [117]. This has been interpreted as indicating that H. ergaster was fully bipedal for running [118], but this is only one of a number of possible explanations [119]. The orientation of the labyrinth not only represents another similarity between australopithecines and extant apes, but—if orientation of the labyrinth is taken as indicative for upright running [118]—it is also a further indication that australopithecines were not well adapted for orthograde bipedal running. 3.4.1.8. Brain Size (and Growth) The published values for brain sizes of australopithecines and extant African apes diverge substantially but are overall in the same range. It has been claimed that there is actually no major transition between Australopithecus and Homo [120] and that early hominins were substantially more encephalized than previously thought, on the basis of a novel 3/5 scaling exponent for primate brain-body evolutionary allometry [121]. According to Gunz et al. [122], comparison of infant to adult endocranial volumes indicates protracted brain growth in A. afarensis, likely critical for the evolution of a long period of childhood learning in hominins. However, recent detailed analysis assessed that “adult (and subsequently estimated neonatal) brain size in australopiths is absolutely intermediate between chimpanzees and gorillas” and that “average annual rates (ARs) of brain growth and proportional size change from birth (PSC) ... for A. afarensis are significantly lower than those of chimpanzees and gorillas. Both ARs and PSCs for A. africanus are similar to chimpanzee and gorilla values. These results indicate that ... high rates of brain growth did not appear until later in human evolution.” [123]. This is in agreement with dental development in australopithecine infants (see Section 3.4.1.1), which indicates maturation periods similar to those of the extant chimpanzee [90,92,93,94,97]. Gunz et al. [122] scanned eight fossil crania of A. afarensis using conventional and synchrotron computed tomography and inferred key features of brain organization from endocranial imprints and explored the pattern of brain growth by combining new endocranial volume estimates with narrow age at death estimates for two infants. Contrary to the statement of Cofran [123], e.g., that early hominins were derived in some aspects of brain anatomy, Gunz et al. [122] concluded that sulcal imprints revealed an ape-like brain organization and no features were derived towards humans. The latter is in agreement with the conclusions of Falk et al. [124] that the skull endocast, dentition, facial growth and possibly foramen magnum position of the Taung child strikingly resemble those of apes. Simons [89] stated: “Dart’s enthusiasm for A. africanus as a human ancestor was occasioned by his misidentification of the lamboid structure as the lunate sulcus and thus reading a human-like sulcal pattern in the natural endocast of the brain of the Taung child”. 3.4.2. Body Size, Body Proportions and Strength The estimated adult height (cm) of A. afarensis (females: 110; males: 135) and the adult weight (kg) (females: 30; males: 45) is completely in line with that of the bonobo. According to Zihlman et al. [98], P. paniscus provides a suitable comparison for Australopithecus Sts.5, with comparable body size, postcranial dimensions and cranial and facial features. Kano [125] concluded that bonobos have body proportions comparable to those of australopithecines, including a spine that enters the skull lower than in chimpanzees, differing from A. afarensis only by the degree of adaptation to bipedalism. The drawing by Zihlman [126] in the New Scientist illustrates how “remarkably well” Lucy’s fossil remains match up with the bones of the bonobo. According to Green et al. [127], however, the upper:lower limb-size proportions in A. afarensis are similar to those of humans and significantly different from all great ape proportions, while A. africanus is more similar to the apes, and significantly different from humans. The claim of Green et al. [127], however, is contradicted by the statement that long legs relative to body mass are first unequivocally present in H. erectus 1.8 Ma, whose relative leg length (assessed from the femur) is possibly up to 50% greater than in A. afarensis [118]. Reno et al. [128] claim that sexual dimorphism in A. afarensis is comparable to that of Homo, but the sexual dimorphism of australopithecines is generally accepted to be much more pronounced than in Homo, in between Pan and Gorilla e.g., [129,130]. Unlike in the extant African apes, it is not accompanied with large canines (in males) and therefore possibly not due to male aggression or male/male competition. Lee [129] found that the sexual dimorphism of A. afarensis differs not only from that of humans, but also from that of chimpanzees. Still, some intriguing sexual dimorphic traits of australopithecines might be explained through comparison with chimpanzees. Dominant chimps (males and large female individuals) monopolize the terrestrial feeding places, where the low-hanging fruits can be gathered with the help of postural bipedality, whereas the smaller females are forced to be more arboreal [35]. This may coincide with suggested feeding pattern differences, whereby male australopithecines could have traits more associated with terrestriality, and females more with arboreality [131]. Comparison of trabecular bone density between different taxa is difficult, with different degrees of similarity depending on limb position [132], but bone thickness is very different when australopithecines are compared to H. erectus, which is typically characterized by very heavy bones (pachyosteosclerosis), especially in the skull (see Section 3.4.1.2), as reviewed by Munro and Verhaegen [99]. 3.4.3. Forelimbs and Tool Production/Use Long and curved manual phalanges were present in, e.g., A. afarensis DIK-1. The manual digits of A. afarensis are just as curved as in apes, and the bar/glenoid angle is not significantly different from apes [108,131,133], indicative of regular vertical climbing and arm-hanging, as in apes [35]. According to Lewin [133], Lucy’s Pan-like tree-climbing tendencies can be inferred from her elongated curved feet and hand finger bones. Australopithecines are believed to express Homo-like traits in hand structure (long thumbs and short fingers), and this has been interpreted as enabling delicate motor skills needed for stone tool-making [134]. However, thorough analysis assessed that A. afarensis had shorter thumbs than previously thought, with proportions more closely resembling those of gorillas and not enabling the precision grip of hominins [135]. Some fossil stone artefacts have been assigned to australopithecines [136], and this has been interpreted as confirmation that australopithecines were on the road to humanity. However, besides the fact that chimps and bonobos are known to display various types of tool use e.g., [137], the bonobos Kanzi and Pan-Banisha exhibit basic Oldowan flake production abilities [134]. Chimp stone artefacts resemble those of the earliest East African sites assigned to australopithecines [138]. Even capuchin monkeys (genera Cebus and Sapajus) use stone tools to crack shellfish and/or nuts [139,140]. A recent analysis [141] concludes that “two pre-requisites for the emergence of early lithic technologies—lithic percussion and the recognition of sharp-edged stones as cutting tools—might be deeply rooted in our evolutionary past (as old as 13 Ma).” These findings raise questions about the value of stone artefacts as markers of humanity and their validity as proof that australopithecines were closer to humans than to African apes. 3.4.4. Thorax and Shoulders and Back Muscles The thorax, shoulders and back muscles of australopithecines are indicative of climbing and arboreality [142]. Alemseged et al. [108] noticed a Gorilla-like scapula in A. afarensis DIK-1. Moreover, the similar cone-shape of the ribcages of A. afarensis (Lucy, A.L.288) and the chimpanzee is apparent [134], and very different from the barrel-shape of the human ribcage (see Figure 3 in Hunt [35]). 3.4.5. Pelvis, Bipedalism and Orthograde Posture The australopithecine pelvis morphology—together with the position of the foramen magnum in the skull (see Section 3.4.1) and the hindlimb anatomy (see Section 3.4.6)—has been considered as indicative of their clear terrestrial bipedality and therefore as evidence for their hominin status [1,59]. Also the short iliac bones of A. africanus are considered as indications of their hominin status. According to Hunt [35], however, Lucy’s very wide hips made her unsuited to bipedal locomotion and she was most likely a postural biped. Interestingly, bipedality in chimpanzees is mostly postural [35]. Others concluded that Lucy’s pelvis is as distinct from the human’s as it is from the chimpanzee’s e.g., [131]. In fact, the low pelvises of monkeys and humans, and of australopithecines, are probably the ancestral hominoid condition, a conclusion already reached by the primatologist Straus [143], almost a century ago: “The human ilium would seem most easily derived from some primitive member of a preanthropoid group, a form which was lacking many of the specializations, such as reduction of the iliac tuberosity and anteacetabular spine and modification of the articular surface, exhibited by the modern apes. I wish to emphasize here that the anthropoid-ape type of ilium is in no sense intermediate between the human and lower mammalian forms. Its peculiar specializations are quite as definite as those exhibited by man, so that it appears very unlikely that a true anthropoid ape form of ilium could have been ancestral to the human type”. Rosen et al. [13], who concluded that knuckle-walking evolved in parallel in Pan and Gorilla, considered the human body form, particularly the long lower back and orthograde posture, as primitive, although the latter view is not shared by Lovejoy [144]. These observations further support knuckle-walking as a derived trait in both Pan and Gorilla e.g., [14,15,22] (see Section 3.3). Hunt [34,35] carried out one of the few detailed studies on the bipedalism of chimpanzees. He observed 97 instances of bipedality among 21 wild chimpanzees for a total time of over 700 h, and noticed that 96% of bipedalism was postural (facultative). In 42.3% of the terrestrial instances, and 93.0% of the instances in terminal branches of trees, a forelimb oriented in an arm-hanging position stabilized the bipedal posture. Bipedal locomotion made up less than 4% of the instances, and larger chimpanzees were the most bipedal. Bipedalism among chimpanzees was much less likely to occur in open habitats and was observed to occur mainly in forested environments, contradicting once more the dominant open plain paradigm. Hunt [35] hypothesized that: “bipedal postural feeding may have been a preadaptation to the fully realized locomotor bipedalism in Homo erectus.”, but, in agreement with Stern, Jr. and Susman [131], we suggest that australopithecine bipedalism was predominantly postural (facultative) as well. The australopithecines certainly were not built for endurance running (Table 1 in [118]). Finally, according to the detailed analysis by Steudel [145], human bipedal locomotion is not more energy efficient than e.g., chimpanzee locomotion. 3.4.6. Hindlimbs, Feet and Bipedalism It is generally accepted that australopithecines were more bipedal than extant African apes, not only because of the more ventral foramen magnum (see Section 3.4.1) and the orthograde posture (see Section 3.4.5.), but also because of the more human-like orientations (although with a rather ape-like anatomy) of the distal femoral (knee) and tibial (ankle) articulations of A. afarensis [131] and the broader calcaneus. Still, the more human-like features of the australopithecine hindlimbs have been assessed to be less abundant than the more ape-like features [25,28,29,30]. Moreover, the long femoral neck and the valgus knee could be plesiomorphic, i.e., correlated with bipedalism, orthogrady and/or lateral movement of the hindlimbs, already present in the ancestral hominoids and possibly in relation to aquarborealism [12,146]. Several studies found that Homo, when compared to both Pan and Australopithecus, has substantially larger articular surface areas relative to body mass in most joints of the lower body e.g., [118]. Harcourt-Smith [147] reviewed the huge diversity of foot morphology among australopithecines. Interestingly, none of the features that are suggested to enable the endurance running of Homo are present in australopithecines. All 26 anatomical characteristics that improve endurance walking, or running, or both, are only apparent in Homo (Table 1 in [118]), and most improve only running. Consequently, if australopithecines were our ancestors, and if running over open plains was the evolutionary force that shaped our genus, it appears that we started to run over long distances without first learning how to walk. With regard to foot morphology, there is huge variation in australopithecine foot structures. What appears to be the case is that most australopithecines were capable of different modes and different degrees of bipedal locomotion, while some maintained—or developed—a more arboreal climbing foot. Bramble and Lieberman [118] indicated numerous profound differences between the feet of Australopithecus and Homo. For example, the transverse groove into which the Achilles tendon inserts on the posterior surface of the heelbone is chimpanzee-like in size in three early australopithecine Hadar specimens and contrasts with the substantially wider and taller attachment area characteristic of H. sapiens. The Hadar and Sterkfontein specimens, meanwhile, had a partial arch only, as indicated by the enlarged medial tuberosity of the navicular, which is also enlarged in chimpanzees, but diminutive and not weight-bearing in Homo. Modern humans have a large heelbone relative to body size and display a uniquely convex cuboid facet, resulting in a more rigid midfoot, generally assumed to facilitate bipedalism. In general, the more arboreal great apes display relatively deeper cuboid facet pivot regions for increased midfoot mobility. The juvenile Dikika apparently had a grasping foot, and so too did ‘Little Foot’ (Stw 573). Tobias [1]: “The talus is human-like, though not perfectly so; the navicular shows both human-like and apish traits; while the medial cuneiform and first metatarsal bones have revealed to Clarke and myself (1995) that the great toe diverged from the lateral four toes, as in apes...” Even ‘Lucy’ had a slightly abducted hallux [148]. The famous footprints of Laetoli (3.7–3.5 Ma) [149], indicating bipedal gait, are still the subject of profound dispute. The numerous problems associated with the interpretation of the Laetoli footprints, and with ichnotaxonomy in general, were addressed in detail by Meldrum et al. [150] and Harcourt-Smith [151]. Meldrum et al. [150] could not conclude that an arch was present, found that the hallux was Gorilla-like, established midtarsal flexibility (an adaptation to arboreality) and saw no definite proof of human-like footprints. Comparison—admittedly superficial—of the Laetoli footprints with those of a human and Gorilla beringei (Figure 2) suggests more similarity of the Laetoli footprints to that of Gorilla. The fragmentary hominin skeletal fossils from the Upper Laetoli Beds are all attributed to Australopithecus. This suggests that the footprints were made by A. afarensis, which is not in contradiction with the previous suggestion that A. afarensis is possibly ancestral/more closely related to Gorilla [28,29,30]. Moreover, Harper et al. [152] found that A. afarensis demonstrates the most human-like heelbone and concluded that this was consistent with obligate bipedalism. But, if A. afarensis was ancestral to Gorilla, its bipedalism may not necessarily be indicative of human bipedalism, as G. beringei beringei is terrestrial for 95% of the time [152]. Anaya et al. [153] suggest that the possession of a rigid foot could represent a conserved trait, whereas the specialized pedal grasping mechanics of extant apes may be more derived. Interpreted this way, extant great apes would no longer possess a rigid foot because they reverted to a more arboreal lifestyle after each genus diverged from a common, already orthograde, ancestor. Furthermore, it’s worth noting that certain morphological features considered indications of habitual or even obligate bipedalism appear early in the fossil record and then seem to disappear. For instance, A. anamensis (4.2 Ma) possessed a modification of the talar trochlea that permitted the leg to pass directly forwards during mid-stance, whereas later australopithecines did not possess this more ‘human-like’ condition. In addition, some late australopithecines, such as A. sediba (1.98 Ma), display more arboreal post-cranial characters than earlier australopithecines. A. sediba had a more arboreal foot with an opposable big toe [152,154,155]. This appears to show that australopithecines may have been becoming less bipedal and more arboreal over time. According to Verhaegen [29], the later australopithecines were probably more knuckle-walking than the earlier australopithecines: “The boisei ulnae O.H.36 and L.40-19 and humerus KNM-ER 739 were of Gorilla robusticity and length (McHenry, 1991, 1992; Howell & Wood, 1974; Senut, 1980; Leakey, 1971; Aiello & Dean, 1990, pp. 367–369), and the curvature and the cross-section of L.40–19 are reminiscent of knuckle-walkers (Howell & Wood, 1974).” Leakey et al. [82], addressing the late australopithecine Paranthropus boisei, suggested: “the Rudolf australopithecines, in fact, may have been close to the ‘knuckle-walker’ condition, not unlike the extant African apes.” Moreover, it has been suggested that Ardipithecus and Orrorin were possibly better bipeds than the later australopithecines [8,32,73], which is not in contradiction with the possibility that these species were intermediate between a more orthograde ancestor and the knuckle-walking extant African apes. In fact, knuckle-walking may have developed in the late australopithecines instead of being initially present in the early australopithecines.
Figure 2. Footprints of (from left to right) Homo sapiens, Gorilla beringei (Eastern gorilla), and of extinct African hominid, possibly Australopithecus afarensis, at Laetoli 3.5–3.7 Ma [149]. Not to relative scale.
In fact, the analysis of Bramble and Lieberman [118] reveals the profound differences between australopithecines and Homo, especially with regard to bipedal locomotion, exactly the trait for which australopithecines have been regarded as hominin. It was the bipedality of A. africanus that led Tobias [1] to conclude that it was a hominin after all, despite its numerous “apish... evolutionary stigmata” (see Section 4.3). Recently, Wiseman [156] compared a total of 36 muscles of the pelvis and lower limb of A. afarensis specimen AL 288-1 with those of H. sapiens. These were reconstructed using three-dimensional polygonal modelling, guided by imaging scan data and muscle scarring. Since the author assumed that A. afarensis is a hominin, the data were compared only with H. sapiens (but not Pan). Nevertheless, the author concluded that overall the thigh of AL 288-1 was probably more muscular and less fatty than the human thigh. The author stated that this is similar to the bonobo, an active, arboreal species. It would be of interest to learn about the results of a direct comparison with P. paniscus. It is worthwhile to conclude this section with a long quote from Phillip V. Tobias [1] (whereby his use of “hominid” corresponds to the use of “hominin” in the present publication): “After decades in which Dart, Broom, Sollas, Clark, Robinson, Schepers and others strove to show how human Australopithecus was, and ultimately were so successful that virtually everyone came to accept Australopithecus as a hominid, it may seem strange that in the final decade of the century we are bringing to light more and more apish features of this extraordinary species.... Falk (1989) and Tobias (1981) revealed how strikingly ape-like the brain of A. africanus was: both in quantity and in quality. Dean and Wood (1981) and Tobias (1967) showed how apish the base of the cranium was, with its ape-like petrous pyramids. Conroy and Kuykendall (1995) and Moggi-Cecchi (1997) laid bare the ape-like pattern of tooth formation—and so did Beynon and Dean (1988) for the rate of tooth formation. Benade (1990) demonstrated some curious ape-like features of the spinal column. Berger (1994) produced the same from the shoulder and from the knee joint (Berger and Tobias 1996); while Brauer et al. (n.d.) inferred the ape-like great toe of “Little Foot”. McHenry and Berger (n.d.) found apish bodily proportions in A. africanus. Everybody, everything, was crying Ape! After long years of australopithecine anthropocentrism, we have entered upon a period of rampant australopithecine apishness! So much so that the custodian* of the largest collection of A. africanus specimens could stand before the Royal Anthropological Institute and feel justified in asking: Was A. africanus a hominid? We should not be surprised to find many ape-like traits in the australopithecines. From the beginning Dart saw apish features: he called the species Australopithecus—the southern ape, not Australanthropus—the southern man.” * Here, Phillip V. Tobias is referring to himself.

4. If Australopithecines Are Hominins, Where Are the Fossils of Extant African Apes?

According to the dominant paradigm, the Homo/Pan/Gorilla ancestor was a semi-erect, knuckle-walking ape, rather similar to extant African apes. Within this paradigm, the lack of indications of knuckle-walking in the hominid fossils—but see Leakey et al. [82] suggesting that the ‘Rudolf’ australopithecines may have been close to the knuckle-walker condition, and Verhaegen [28]—implicitly leads to the assumption that there are no African ape (panin and gorillin) fossils. This presumed lack is exemplified by the following recent quote: “Even worse, relatively complete fossil apes undisputedly assigned to the early members of the gorilla and chimpanzee lineages remain to be found.” [6]. This presumed extreme paucity of ape fossils is rather counterintuitive, for several reasons, as pointed out previously [24]. In fact, one would expect more ape fossils than hominin fossils, since in optimum habitats, chimpanzees reach population densities of up to 4 individuals/km2 [24], while human population density in the Lower Paleolithic has been estimated to amount to no more than 0.004 individuals/km2 [24]. Moreover, 3 Ma, the ancestral Pan lineage had the largest effective population size of all hominine lineages [157]. Even if the human population density figure is an underestimation, the authors expected to find manifold more ape fossils in the same fluviatile deposits that yielded extinct hominins. Yet, Kleindienst et al. [24] are also of the opinion: “... there are none at all...”. The following explanations have been considered: 4.1. Extant Apes Live in Forests Where Fossilization Was Difficult Compared to the Savannah Environments in Which Australopithecines Lived Kleindienst et al. [24] dismissed the explanation that, compared to extinct hominins, extinct panins and gorillins inhabited habitats where fossilization was difficult. They pointed to the hundreds of fossils of dryopithecines, deposited in very diverse habitats (e.g., dense tropical forests and mountain forests) through different types of landscapes (grass savannahs, swamp forests, oak forests) in an area spanning Eurasia and East Africa, between 10 and 8 Ma. Moreover, the current geographical range of P. troglodytes includes 40% predominantly savannah, woodland and dry forest and about 55% rain and swamp forests, not very different from dryopithecines, and quite similar to where extinct australopithecines have been found (see Section 3.1). Kleindienst et al. [24] noted that most of the Plio-Pleistocene australopithecine fossil sites were deposited in lake and river shore zones and constituted excellent chimpanzee habitats. Also contrary to the “lack of fossilization” argument is the notion that extant Asian orangutan Pongo has several fossil relatives, although fossilization of orangutans is much less likely than it is for chimpanzees because they are more explicitly forest apes [24]. 4.2. We Have Not Searched the Right Places An idea related to the taphonomic bias argument above assumes that we have been searching for ape fossils in the wrong place. According to Pilbeam [158], paleoanthropologists could be “like the drunk looking for his keys under the lamppost where it was light rather than where he had dropped them, working with what we had rather than asking whether or not that was adequate.” This explanation is also reflected by the following recent quote: “extinct nonhominin hominines have yet to be found in largely unexplored regions of Africa, explaining the virtual lack of a gorilla and chimpanzee fossil record.” [6]. In the meantime, there have been several paleoanthropological expeditions outside Eastern and Southern Africa, leading for example to the discovery of S. tchadensis (in the Sahel) [61,69,71], but still, according to the interpretations given within the dominant paradigm, no ancestors of the extant African apes have been unearthed. 4.3. We Misinterpreted the Fossils of Extant African Apes to Be Hominin Fossils: Australopithecine Anthropocentrism A third possibility, defended here, has been raised previously [24,28,29], but has been largely neglected. Kleindienst et al. [24] remarked: “More than once in paleontological research this kind of gap has been discovered to be not a lack of fossil evidence, but a lack of recognition of existing fossils as what they were within the correct evolving lineage.” Starting from the assumption that the Homo/Pan/Gorilla ancestor was knuckle-walking, none of the australopithecine fossils qualified as a possible African ape ancestor, given the almost complete absence of any signs of knuckle-walking in australopithecines. However, the assumption that the last common ancestor was knuckle-walking has now largely been abandoned (see also Section 3.3). The notion that the last common ancestor was not semi-erect, but already orthograde (brachiating, hanging, climbing) and eventually partially bipedal (possibly for wading in swamp forests) means that the use of orthograde bipedalism as a criterion to classify australopithecines as hominins is no longer valid. Moreover, the notion that knuckle-walking evolved independently in Pan and Gorilla (see Section 3.3) means that the absence of knuckle-walking in (early) australopithecines does not exclude them from being ancestors of Pan and/or Gorilla. In conclusion, we suggest that there are plenty of panin and gorillin fossils in Africa, only we failed to recognize them as such. In agreement with Kleindienst et al. [24], we might paraphrase Pilbeam [158] and state that classical paleoanthropologists could be “like the drunk who is looking for his keys in broad daylight, without realizing that he is staring at them, not recognizing that the australopithecines are the legitimate ancestors of nonhominin hominines.” Instead of considering graciles as ancestral to Pan and robusts as ancestral to Gorilla, as suggested by Kleindienst et al. [24], Verhaegen [29] considered the eastern African australopithecines, such as A. afarensis (incl. Lucy) as well as P. boisei to be ancestral or closer to the genus Gorilla, and the South-African australopithecines, A. africanus (incl. the Taung child) and P. robustus, ancestral or closer to the genus Pan. This would mean that the genera Australopithecus and Paranthropus are paraphyletic. Instead, according to Verhaegen [29], the South-African and eastern African australopithecines evolved in parallel, presumably both in response to the same climatic changes, from late-Pliocene graciles (A. africanus (3.3–2.1 Ma) and A. afarensis (3.9–2.9 Ma)) in wetter, more forested environments (swamp forests) over early-Pleistocene robusts (respectively, P. robustus (2.0–0.7 Ma) and P. boisei (2.3–1.2 Ma)) in more open, somewhat drier and more mosaic environments—but always near large bodies of permanent water such as papyrus swamps, to extant African apes (respectively, Pan and Gorilla), more on the forest floor. The South-African australopithecines may have been more omnivorous, and the eastern African australopithecines more herbivorous – chewing tougher plants, which may still be reflected in the diets of Pan and Gorilla, respectively. In summary, the majority of australopithecine traits and functionalities, and their habitat and diet, are reminiscent of extant (African) apes, even despite the fact that the latter possess some strongly derived characteristics, such as knuckle-walking. Because the Homo/Pan/Gorilla last common ancestor was possibly even more bipedal than australopithecines (see Section 3.2), it is unlikely that the similarities between extant African apes and australopithecines can be explained as African apes still resembling the last common ancestor. Rather, it would be the australopithecines that more closely resemble the extant African apes. Given the insights that have steadily accumulated during the last decades, it is intriguing that the status of australopithecines as hominins has hardly been questioned, even in recent reviews e.g., [6], despite previous explicit suggestions that australopithecines are possibly not hominins [1,10,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,98,159]. A possible evolutionary tree, with australopithecines as panin and gorillin ancestors, is presented below (Figure 3).
Figure 3. Possible evolutionary tree of Homo, Pan and Gorilla with australopithecines as putative panin and gorillin ancestors. Legend: dotted black arrows and lines: possible phylogenetic relatedness; thick blue arrows: migration directions. Time scale at bottom (Ma: million years ago) is not linear.

5. Australopithecines Are Possibly Not Hominins Because Our Ancestors May Not Have Been Present in Africa in between 4 and 3 Ma

There are genetic indications that our direct ancestors may not have been present in Africa between 4 and 3 Ma. Corroboration of this absence would definitely exclude australopithecines as our ancestors. The absence of our ancestors from Africa between 4 and 3 Ma is substantiated by the difference of our genome with that of all African primates with respect to endoretroviral genetic elements PtERV1 and PtERV2 (Pt: Pan troglodytes; ERV: endoretroviral). These are present in all tested African primates, but absent from all tested Asian primates, and from Homo. Both chimpanzee and gorilla genomes harbor more than 100 copies of PtERV1 [160]. The presence of these PtERVs at different insertion sites in the genomes of both species (as well in the genomes of all other African primates), is a clear indication of separate infections, excluding homology, i.e., excluding inheritance from a common ancestor. These endoretroviral infections are estimated to have occurred between 4 and 3 Ma [160], at the very time australopithecines were diversifying in Africa. Yohn et al. [160] dismissed the possibility of geographic isolation as an explanation for the absence of PtERVs from the human genome, based upon the assumption that australopithecines are our ancestors and therefore that our ancestors had to be present in Africa during the period that PtERVs were infecting all African primate lineages [160]. This is another example of the possible consequences of ‘australopithecine anthropocentrism’, as it was called by Tobias [1]. It has been suggested that our lineage was specifically resistant to infection by PtERVs, while at the same time increasing our susceptibility to HIV-1, because of human TRIM5α [161]. However, this suggestion is not supported by their data, since the Pan TRIM5 is identical to that of Homo in the region of importance, while this species was not protected against PtERV infection. In addition, the sooty mangabey (Cercocebus atys) has a TRIM5a sequence that would offer protection according to the same authors [161], but the ancestors of this species were clearly infected with PtERVs [162]. Moreover, another study [163] could not confirm the results of Kaiser et al. [161]. Therefore, we suggest reconsidering the possibility of geographic isolation. This could have been caused by the presence of our ancestors within Africa, but in coastal econiches separated from other primates [164] or by the complete absence of our direct ancestors from Africa during the Pliocene [162]. The earliest genuine indications of the existence of Homo are found almost simultaneously in Eurasia and Africa (early-Pleistocene, approx. 2.0–1.7 Ma), which leaves open the possibility that the genus Homo originated outside of Africa (not further discussed here, but presented in Figure 3). The very recent description of Anadoluvius turkae in Anatolia 8.7 Ma [70] and the suggestion of the authors that hominines may have originated in Eurasia during the late Miocene certainly do not contradict the possibility that our direct ancestors were absent from Africa before 2.0 Ma. Most importantly, in the context of the present argument, in the event that the absence of direct Homo ancestors from Africa could be substantiated, the australopithecines would be excluded definitely as hominins.

6. Conclusions

We suggest that australopithecines may not be our closest relatives and/or our direct ancestors and support our opinion by means of (i) a review of the history of paradigms in paleoanthropology, (ii) an overview of several unexpected insights of the last decades, and (iii) a comparison of the morphology of australopithecine and Homo fossils and of extant African apes and humans. The hypothesis that the hominin lineage alone became bipedal, as an adaptation by a semi-erect chimpanzee-like ancestor to deforestation and thus to living on the ground, has been held for more than two centuries, ever since Lamarck [37]. This line of reasoning was parsimonious, given the scarcity of fossil evidence during the 19th and early 20th century. It is also quite understandable that the fossil finds of australopithecines, bipedal apes with the brain size of extant apes, living in a period in-between the estimated date of the Homo/Pan split and the first Homo fossils, seemed to corroborate the predominant grassland savannah paradigm, also because they were found in what are now open landscapes, devoid of extant apes. During the 1990s, however, different clues prompted several paleoanthropologists to conclude that the savannah hypothesis as an explanation for the bipedalism of australopithecines was not only wrong, but also superfluous. First, grassland savannahs had not been expanding in Africa at the time of the first australopithecines (approx. 4 Ma) [9,35,64,65,66,67,68]. Second, australopithecines were shown to have clear climbing adaptations, and to have lived in wetland, woodland or forest margins e.g., [9,10,11,12,34,49], they did not run long distances over open plains to chase prey or to scavenge carcasses because they were poor runners [118] and because they were largely vegetarian [9,165]. In the meantime, other studies e.g., [14,15,16,17] indicated that knuckle-walking was not an ancestral characteristic of the hominids, but had been derived independently in Pan and Gorilla. In other words, there had never been a knuckle-walking Homo/Pan ancestor from which australopithecine bipedalism had to evolve. This view has been corroborated by more recent studies [18,19,20]. Finally, at the turn of the century, different fossil bipedal and/or orthograde apes, clearly predating the australopithecines, were discovered: Sahelanthropus tchadensis (Toumaï, Sahel - between 7.0 and 6.0 Ma) [69,71], Orrorin tugenensis (Tugen Hills, Kenya—between 6.15 and 5.80 Ma) [32], and Ardipithecus ramidus (Afar, Ethiopia—between 4.51 and 4.32 Ma) [4,72,73]. This indicated that orthogrady and/or bipedalism was present in several, if not in most, fossil ape taxa [4,5,6], mostly from (wet) woodlands, not grassland savannah. This further supports the realization that knuckle-walking had been derived independently in extant Pan and Gorilla. Therefore, there is no need to explain the bipedalism of australopithecines as a newly acquired characteristic starting from a knuckle-walking Homo/Pan ancestor and as a characteristic typical for the hominin lineage, or as an adaptation to living on grassland savannah. These observations and insights, together with the realization that australopithecines are much more ape-like than previously recognized e.g., [1,28,29,30], confirm earlier suggestions that australopithecines were possibly close relatives of, or ancestral to, the extant African apes instead of ancestral to humans [22,24,26,27,28,29,30,31,51,75]. Indeed, even Phillip V. Tobias, one of the world’s leading authorities on australopithecines, the director of the world’s largest collection of A. africanus specimens, and the protégé and direct successor of Raymond Dart [166], dared to ask whether A. africanus was a hominin. Instead of viewing the australopithecines as transitional between a quadrupedal, knuckle-walking Homo/Pan ancestor on the road to fully bipedal humans, we have here presented several arguments to view them as transitional between an already predominantly orthograde ancestor, with bipedal posture/gait, and the extant knuckle-walking African apes. We state that it cannot be excluded that australopithecines evolved from Miocene and Pliocene apes that were already orthograde, as represented by fossils such as Sahelanthropus, Orrorin and/or Ardipithecus, and that they gave rise to the extant semi-erect chimpanzees, bonobos, and gorillas. In fact, detailed analyses, pointing this out, were published already more than three decades ago by one of us [28,29,30]. Although many paleoanthropologists will claim that they have abandoned the grassland savannah hypothesis as a valid explanation of how australopithecines evolved habitual bipedalism from a knuckle-walking ancestor, it is most intriguing that many have adopted the view that adaptation to mosaic savannah was a sufficiently strong selective pressure to explain how—two million years later—Homo developed obligate bipedalism from already orthograde bipedal australopithecine ancestors. The harsh open grassland savannah narratives had at least some logic and credibility. They explained the presumed major transition from a quadrupedal Homo/Pan last common ancestor to a bipedal australopithecine as an adaptation to strong environmental changes. The mosaic savannah paradigm tries to explain how Homo evolved obligate orthograde bipedalism from already habitual orthograde bipeds (australopithecines), in a much less-demanding type of savannah, the Pleistocene woodland mosaic savannah e.g., [62,151,167,168]. In summary, the mosaic grassland hypothesis states that we had to develop obligate bipedalism from already habitual bipeds only to run quickly from one spot of forest to another. As Elaine Morgan [169] put it aptly: “The original savannah model – though it did not stand the test of time – was argued in strong and clear terms. We are different from the apes, it stated, because they lived in the forest and our ancestors lived on the plains. The new watered-down version suggests that we are different from the apes because their ancestors, perhaps, lived in a different part of the mosaic. Say what you will, but it does not have the same ring to it.” Furthermore, since orthogrady and, at the very least, facultative/postural bipedalism were already present in the Homo/Pan ancestor, it follows that our bipedalism can no longer be regarded as the initial and necessary condition that made possible our other numerous anomalous characters, including large brain size, also because brain size did not increase significantly during the millions of years that the bipedal—probably nonhominin—australopithecines and their predecessors existed. Finally, considering australopithecines as possible ancestors of extant African apes, might help to resolve several enigmas, such as the counterintuitive lack of fossils of the once numerous African apes and the unexpected absence of PtERV-elements in our genome [160,161,162,163]. The argument regarding the hominin status of the australopithecines may be settled in a more definite manner. Because protein can survive longer in fossilized tissues than DNA, it is possible to perform protein analysis in fossilized bone, as has been done for Mesozoic dinosaurs [170]. The most abundant protein in bone is collagen type 1, containing one collagen 1α2 chain, for which there are six (mostly aliphatic) amino acid differences between Pan and Homo (pers. comm., Anne J. Kleinnijenhuis, Triskelion, Utrecht, the Netherlands). Paleoproteomics might therefore contribute to clarify the phylogenetic relatedness of australopithecines with Homo, Pan and Gorilla. At the very least, the above data and considerations should prompt the paleoanthropological community to consider the possibility that we have been barking up the wrong ancestral tree, for almost a century.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.V. (Mario Vaneechoutte), M.V. (Marc Verhaegen); Investigation, F.M., M.V. (Mario Vaneechoutte), M.V. (Marc Verhaegen), S.M.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, M.V. (Mario Vaneechoutte); Writing—Review & Editing, F.M., M.V. (Mario Vaneechoutte), M.V. (Marc Verhaegen), S.M.; Visualization, F.M., M.V. (Marc Verhaegen).

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

References

1.
Tobias PV. Ape-like Australopithecus after seventy years: Was it a hominid?  J. Royal Anthropol. Inst. 1998, 4, 283–308. [Google Scholar]
2.
Wood B, Richmond BG. Human evolution: taxonomy and paleobiology.  J. Anat. 2000, 196, 19–60. [Google Scholar]
3.
Thorpe SK, Holder RL, Crompton RH. Origin of human bipedalism as an adaptation for locomotion on flexible branches.  Science 2007, 316, 1328–1331. [Google Scholar]
4.
Crompton RH, Vereecke EE, Thorpe SKS. Locomotion and posture from the common hominoid ancestor to fully modern hominins, with special reference to the last common panin/hominin ancestor.  J. Anat. 2008, 212, 501–543. [Google Scholar]
5.
Thorpe SKS, McClymont JM, Crompton RH. The arboreal origins of human bipedalism. Antiquity 2014, 88, 906–926. [Google Scholar]
6.
Almécija S, Hammond AS, Thompson NE, Pugh KD, Moyà-Solà S, Alba DM. Fossil apes and human evolution.  Science 2021, 372, eabb4363. [Google Scholar]
7.
Gebo DL, MacLatchy L, Kityo R, Deino A, Kingston J, Pilbeam D. A hominoid genus from the early Miocene of Uganda.  Science 1997, 276, 401–404. [Google Scholar]
8.
White TD, Lovejoy CO, Asfaw B, Carlson JP, Suwa G. Neither chimpanzee nor human, Ardipithecus reveals the surprising ancestry of both.  Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 4877–4884. [Google Scholar]
9.
Verhaegen M, Puech P-F. Hominid lifestyle and diet reconsidered: Paleo-environmental and comparative data.  Hum. Evol. 2000, 15, 151–162. [Google Scholar]
10.
Verhaegen M, Puech P-F, Munro S.  Aquarboreal ancestors?  Trends Ecol. Evol. 2002, 17, 212–217. [Google Scholar]
11.
Verhaegen M, Munro S, Vaneechoutte M, Bender-Oser N, Bender R. The original econiche of Homo. In Ecology Research Progress; Nova Science Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 1–34.
12.
Verhaegen M, Munro S, Puech P-F, Vaneechoutte M. Early hominoids: Orthograde aquarboreals in flooded forests? In Was Man More Aquatic in the Past? Fifty Years after Alister Hardy—Waterside Hypotheses of Human Evolution; Bentham eBooks: Sharjah, UAE, 2011; pp. 67–81. 
13.
Rosen KH, Jones CE, DeSilva JM. Bipedal locomotion in zoo apes: Revisiting the hylobatian model for bipedal origins.  Evol. Hum. Sci. 2022, 4, e12. [Google Scholar]
14.
Dainton M, Macho GA. Did knuckle walking evolve twice? J. Hum. Evol. 1999, 36, 171–194. [Google Scholar]
15.
Dainton M. Did our ancestors knuckle-walk?  Nature 2001, 410, 324–325. [Google Scholar]
16.
Filler AG. The Upright Ape; Career Press: Franklin Lakes, NJ, USA, 2007.
17.
Kivell TL, Schmitt D.  Independent evolution of knuckle-walking in African apes shows that humans did not evolve from a knuckle-walking ancestor.  Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 14241–14246. [Google Scholar]
18.
Morimoto N, Nakatsukasa M, Ponce de León MS, Zollikofer CPE. Femoral ontogeny in humans and great apes and its implications for their last common ancestor.  Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 1930. [Google Scholar]
19.
Simpson SW, Latimer B, Lovejoy CO. Why do knuckle‐walking African apes knuckle‐walk?  Anat. Rec. 2018, 301, 496–514. [Google Scholar]
20.
Ward CV, Hammond AS, Plavcan JM, Begun DR. A late Miocene hominid partial pelvis from Hungary.  J. Hum. Evol. 2019, 136, 102645. [Google Scholar]
21.
Crompton RH, Sellers W, Davids K, McClymont J. Biomechanics and the origins of human bipedal walking: The last 50 years.  J. Biomech. 2023, 157, 111701. [Google Scholar]
22.
Morton DJ. Evolution of man’s erect posture (preliminary report).  J. Morphol. 1926, 43, 147–179. [Google Scholar]
23.
Bender R, Tobias PV, Bender N. The savannah hypotheses: Origin, reception and impact on paleoanthropology.  Hist. Philos. Life Sci. 2012, 34, 147–184. [Google Scholar]
24.
Kleindienst MR, Burton FD, Kortlandt A. On new perspectives on ape and human evolution.  Curr. Anthropol. 1975, 16, 644–651. [Google Scholar]
25.
Oxnard CE. The place of the australopithecines in human evolution: Grounds for doubt?  Nature 1975, 258, 389–395. [Google Scholar]
26.
Gribbin JR, Cherfas J. The Monkey Puzzle: Reshaping the Evolutionary Tree; Pantheon: New York City, NY, USA, 1982.
27.
Edelstein SJ. An alternative paradigm for hominoid evolution.  Hum. Evol. 1987, 2, 169–174. [Google Scholar]
28.
Verhaegen MJB. African ape ancestry.  Hum. Evol. 1990, 5, 295–297. [Google Scholar]
29.
Verhaegen MJB. Australopithecines: Ancestors of the African apes?  Hum. Evol. 1994, 9, 121–139. [Google Scholar]
30.
Verhaegen M. Morphological distance between australopithecine, human and ape skulls.  Hum. Evol. 1996, 11, 35–41. [Google Scholar]
31.
Gribbin J, Cherfas J. The First Chimpanzee. In Search of Human Origins; Penguin Books: London, UK, 2001.
32.
Pickford M, Senut B, Gommery D, Treil J. Bipedalism in Orrorin tugenensis revealed by its femora.  C. R. Palevol. 2002, 1, 191–203. [Google Scholar]
33.
Dawkins R, Wong Y. The Ancestor’s Tale. The Dawn of Evolution; Houghton Mifflin Hartcourt: Boston, NY, USA, 2016.
34.
Hunt KD. The evolution of human bipedality: Ecology and functional morphology.  J. Hum. Evol. 1994, 26, 183–202. [Google Scholar]
35.
Hunt KD. The postural feeding hypothesis: An ecological model for the evolution of bipedalism.  S. Afr. J. Sci. 1996, 92, 77–90. [Google Scholar]
36.
Stamos PA, Alemseged Z. Hominin locomotion and evolution in the Late Miocene to Late Pliocene.  J. Human Evol. 2023, 178, 103332. [Google Scholar]
37.
de Lamarck J-B. Zoological Philosophy; An Exposition with Regard to the Natural History of Animals; MacMillan & Co: London, UK, 1914.
38.
Mayr E. The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MT, USA, 1982.
39.
Delisle RG. Debating humankind’s place in Nature. In The Nature of Paleoanthropology; Pearson Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 1860–2000.
40.
Darwin C. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex; John Murray: London, UK, 1871.
41.
Wallace AR. Darwinism: An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection with Some of its Applications; Macmillan: London, UK, 1889.
42.
Steinmann G. Die Geologischen Grundlagen der Abstammungslehre; Engelmann: Leipzig, Germany, 1908.
43.
Barrell J. Probable relations of climatic change to the origin of the Tertiary ape-man.  Sci. M. 1908, 4, 16–26. [Google Scholar]
44.
Ruff CB, Burgess ML, Ketcham RA, Kappelman J. Limb bone structural proportions and locomotor behavior in A.L. 288-1 (“Lucy”).  PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0166095. [Google Scholar]
45.
Dart R. Australopithecus africanus, the man-ape of South Africa. Nature 1925, 115, 195–199. [Google Scholar]
46.
Sussman RW. The myth of Man the Hunter, Man the Killer and the evolution of human morality.  Zygon 1999, 34, 453–471. [Google Scholar]
47.
R.J.G.S. C.K. Brain 1981. The Hunters or the Hunted? An Introduction to African Cave Taphonomy. x + 365 pp., 226 figs. 121 figures. Chicago, London: The University of Chicago Press. Price £24.50. ISBN 0 226 07089 1.  Geol. Mag. 1983, 120, 92–93. [Google Scholar]
48.
Maguire JM. Recent geological, stratigraphic and palaeontological studies at Makapansgat Limeworks. In Hominid Evolution: Past, Present and Future; Alan R. Liss: New York, NY, USA, 1985, pp. 151–164.
49.
Reed KE. Early hominid evolution and ecological change through the African Plio-Pleistocene.  J. Hum. Evol. 1997, 32, 289–322. [Google Scholar]
50.
Lindshield S, Hernandez‐Aguilar RA, Korstjens AH, Marchant LF, Narat V, Ndiaye PI, et al. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in savanna landscapes.  Evol. Anthropol. 2021, 30, 399–420. [Google Scholar]
51.
Keith A.  The Taungs skull.  Nature 1925, 116, 11. [Google Scholar]
52.
Woodward AS. The fossil anthropoid ape from Taungs.  Nature 1925, 115, 235–236. [Google Scholar]
53.
Broom R. The Coming of Man: Was it Accident or Design? Witherby: London, UK, 1933.
54.
Ardrey R. African Genesis: A Personal Investigation into the Animal Origins and Nature of Man; Atheneum: New York, NY, USA, 1961.
55.
Ardrey R. The Hunting Hypothesis: A Personal Conclusion Concerning the Evolutionary Nature of Man; Atheneum: New York, NY, USA, 1976.
56.
Johnson EM. The better bonobos of our nature, 2012. Available online: https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/primate-diaries/the-better-bonobos-of-our-nature/ (accessed on 6 September 2023).
57.
Wrangham R, Peterson D. Demonic Males: Apes and the Origins of Human Violence; Houghton Miflin: Boston, NY, USA, 1996.
58.
Johanson DC, White TD, Coppens Y. A new species of the genus Australopithecus (Primates: Hominidae) from the Pliocene of Eastern Africa”.  Kirtlandia 1978, 28, 1–14. [Google Scholar]
59.
Johanson DC, Edey MA. Lucy: The Beginnings of Humankind; Simon & Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 1981.
60.
Coppens Y. East side story, the origin of humankind.  Sci. Am. 1994, 270, 88–95. [Google Scholar]
61.
Brunet M, Beauvillain A, Coppens Y, Heintz E, Moutaye AHE, Pilbeam D. The first australopithecine 2,500 kilometres west of the Rift Valley (Chad). Nature 1995, 378, 273–275. [Google Scholar]
62.
Coppens Y. L’East Side Story n’existe plus.  La Recherche 2003, 361, 74–78. [Google Scholar]
63.
Raynaud B. 2020. Le blog. Yves Coppens, the interview 2/3. 5th of August 2020. Available online: https://benjaminraynaudleblog.wordpress.com/2020/08/05/yves-coppens-the-interview-2-3/ (accessed on 7 September 2023).
64.
Leakey R, Lewontin R. Origins Reconsidered; Little Brown: London, UK, 1992.
65.
Radosevich SC, Retallack GJ, Taieb M. Reassessment of the paleoenvironment and preservation of hominid fossils from Hadar, Ethiopia.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1992, 87, 15–27. [Google Scholar]
66.
Tobias PV. ‘Little Foot’ and the bearing of recent South African researches on the status of Australopithecus africanus. In Was Man More Aquatic in the Past? Fifty Years after Alister Hardy. Waterside Hypotheses of Human Evolution; Bentham eBooks: Sharjah, UAE, 2011, pp. 3–15.
67.
de Menocal PB. African climate change and faunal evolution during the Pliocene–Pleistocene.  Earth Planet. Sci. Lett. 2004, 220, 3–24. [Google Scholar]
68.
White TD. Reply to: Dominguez-Rodrigo M. Is the “Savanna hypothesis” a dead concept for explaining the emergence of the earliest hominins?  Curr. Anthropol. 2014, 55, 59–69. [Google Scholar]
69.
Brunet M, Guy F, Pilbeam D, Mackaye HT, Likius A, Ahounta D, et al. A new hominid from the Upper Miocene of Chad, Central Africa.  Nature 2002, 418, 145–151. [Google Scholar]
70.
Sevim-Erol A, Begun DR, Sözer ÇS, Mayda S, van den Hoek Ostende LW, Martin RMG, et al. A new ape from Türkiye and the radiation of late Miocene hominines.  Commun. Biol. 2023, 6, 842. [Google Scholar]
71.
Brunet M.  Sahelanthropus or ‘Sahelpithecus’? Reply to Wolpoff et al. Nature 2002, 419, 582. [Google Scholar]
72.
White T, Suwa G, Asfaw B. Australopithecus ramidus, a new species of early hominid from Aramis, Ethiopia.  Nature 1994, 371, 306–312. [Google Scholar]
73.
Lovejoy CO. Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science 2009, 326, 74e1–74e8.
74.
Hasegawa M, Kishino H, Yano T. Dating of the human-ape splitting by a molecular clock of mitochondrial DNA.  J. Mol. Evol. 1985, 22, 160–174. [Google Scholar]
75.
Keith A. Hunterian Lectures on Man’s posture: Its evolution and disorders: Given at the Royal College of Surgeons of England.  Brit. Med. J. 1923, 1, 451–454. [Google Scholar]
76.
Tuttle RH. Knuckle-walking and the problem of human origins.  Science 1969, 166, 953–961. [Google Scholar]
77.
Begun D. Miocene fossil hominids and the chimp-human clade.  Science 1992, 257, 1929–1933. [Google Scholar]
78.
Richmond BG, Strait DS. Evidence that humans evolved from a knuckle walking ancestor.  Nature 2000, 404, 382–385. [Google Scholar]
79.
Williams SA. Morphological integration and the evolution of knuckle-walking. J. Hum. Evol. 2010, 58, 432–440. [Google Scholar]
80.
Schultz AH. The position of the occipital condyles and of the face relative to the skull base in primates.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1955, 13, 97–120. [Google Scholar]
81.
McCollum MA, Rosenman BA, Suwa G, Meindl RS, Lovejoy CO. The vertebral formula of the last common ancestor of African apes and humans.  J. Exp. Zool. Mol. Dev. Evol. 2010, 314, 123–134. [Google Scholar]
82.
Leakey REF, Mungai JM, Walker AC. New australopithecines from East Rudolf, Kenya.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1971, 35, 175–186. [Google Scholar]
83.
Du A, Alemseged Z. Temporal evidence shows Australopithecus sediba is unlikely to be the ancestor of Homo.  Sci. Adv. 2019, 5, eaav9038. [Google Scholar]
84.
Ferguson WW. A new species of the genus Australopithecus (Primates-Hominidae) from the Plio/Pleistocene deposits West of Lake Turkana in Kenya.  Primates 1989, 30, 223–232. [Google Scholar]
85.
Hrdlička A. The Taungs ape.  Am. J. Phys. Anthrop. 1925, 8, 379. [Google Scholar]
86.
Grine FE, Kay RF. Early hominid diets from quantitative image analysis of dental microwear.  Nature 1988, 333, 765–768. [Google Scholar]
87.
McCollum MA. The robust australopithecine face: A morphogenetic perspective. Science 1999, 284, 301–305. [Google Scholar]
88.
Martin L. Significance of enamel thickness in hominoid evolution.  Nature 1985, 314, 260–263. [Google Scholar]
89.
Simons E. Human origins.  Science 1989, 245, 1343–1350. [Google Scholar]
90.
Bromage T, Dean M. Re-evaluation of the age at death of immature fossil hominids. Nature 1985, 317, 525–527. [Google Scholar]
91.
Bromage TG. Taung facial remodelling: a growth and development study. In Evolution; Alan R. Liss: New York, NY, USA, 1985, pp. 239–245.
92.
Smith BH. Dental development in Australopithecus and early Homo Nature 1986, 323, 327–330. [Google Scholar]
93.
Conroy GC, Vannier MW. Dental development of the Taung skull from computerized tomography.  Nature 1987, 329, 625–627. [Google Scholar]
94.
Conroy GC, Vannier MW. The nature of Taung dental maturation continued.  Nature 1988, 333, 808. [Google Scholar]
95.
Beynon AD, Dean MC. Distinct dental development patterns in early fossil hominids.  Nature 1988, 335, 509–514. [Google Scholar]
96.
Conroy GC, Kuykendall K. Paleopediatrics: Or when did human infants really become human?  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1995, 98, 121–131. [Google Scholar]
97.
Smith HB. Patterns of dental development in Homo, Australopithecus, Pan, and Gorilla.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1994, 94, 307–325. [Google Scholar]
98.
Zihlman AL, Cronin JE, Cramer DL, Sarich VM. Pygmy chimpanzee as a possible prototype for the common ancestor of humans, chimpanzees and gorillas.  Nature 1978, 275, 744–746. [Google Scholar]
99.
Munro S, Verhaegen M. Pachyosteosclerosis in archaic Homo: Heavy skulls for diving, heavy legs for wading? In Was Man More Aquatic in the Past? Fifty Years after Alister Hardy—Waterside Hypotheses of Human Evolution; Bentham eBooks: Sharjah, UAE, 2011; pp. 82–105.
100.
Crowell MG, Rahmat S, Koretsky I. Correlation of bone density in semi-aquatic and aquatic animals with ecological and dietary specializations.  FASEB J. 2020, 34, 1. [Google Scholar]
101.
Rak Y, Ginzburg A, Geffen A.  Gorilla-like anatomy on Australopithecus afarensis mandibles suggests Au. afarensis link to robust australopiths.  Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 6568–6572. [Google Scholar]
102.
Campbell RM, Vinas G, Henneberg M. Relationships between the hard and soft dimensions of the nose in Pan troglodytes and Homo sapiens reveal the positions of the nasal tips of Plio-Pleistocene hominids.  PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0259329. [Google Scholar]
103.
Schultz AH. Growth and development of the orang-utan. Contrib. Embryol. 1941, XXIX, 57–110.
104.
Eckhardt R. Hominoid nasal region polymorphism and its phylogenetic significance.  Nature 1987, 328, 333–335. [Google Scholar]
105.
McCollum MA. Subnasal morphological variation in fossil hominids: A reassessment based on new observations and recent developmental findings.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2000, 112, 275–283. [Google Scholar]
106.
Kimbel WH, White TD, Johanson DC. Cranial morphology of Australopithecus afarensis: A comparative study based on a composite reconstruction of the adult skull.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1984, 64, 337–388. [Google Scholar]
107.
Dunn JC. Sexual selection and the loss of laryngeal air sacs during the evolution of speech.  Anthropol. Sci. 2018, 126, 29–34. [Google Scholar]
108.
Alemseged Z, Spoor F, Kimbel WH, Bobe R, Geraads D, Reed D, et al. A juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia.  Nature 2006, 443, 296–301. [Google Scholar]
109.
de Boer B. Loss of air sacs improved hominin speech abilities.  J. Hum. Evol. 2012, 62, 1–6. [Google Scholar]
110.
Masters AV, Falk D, Gage TB. Effects of age and gender on the location and orientation of the foramen magnum in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulata).  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1991, 86, 75–80. [Google Scholar]
111.
Ashton EH, Zuckerman S. Age changes in the position of the occipital condyles in the gorilla.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1952, 10, 277–288. [Google Scholar]
112.
Kimbel WH, Rak Y.  The cranial base of Australopithecus afarensis: New insights from the female skull.  Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2010, 365, 3365–3376. [Google Scholar]
113.
Landi F, Profico A, Veneziano A, De Groote I, Manzi G. Locomotion, posture, and the foramen magnum in primates: Reliability of indices and insights into hominin bipedalism.  Am. J. Primatol. 2020, 82, e23170. [Google Scholar]
114.
Bastir M, Rosas A. Mosaic evolution of the basicranium in Homo and its relation to modular development.  Evol. Biol. 2009, 36, 57–70. [Google Scholar]
115.
Raia P, Boggioni M, Carotenuto F, Castiglione S, Di Febbraro M, Di Vincenzo F, et al. Unexpectedly rapid evolution of mandibular shape in hominins.  Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 7340. [Google Scholar]
116.
Wood B, Harrison T. The evolutionary context of the first hominins.  Nature 2011, 470, 347–352. [Google Scholar]
117.
Spoor F, Wood B, Zonneveld F. Implications of early hominid labyrinthine morphology for evolution of human bipedal locomotion.  Nature 1994, 369, 645–648. [Google Scholar]
118.
Bramble DM, Lieberman DE.  Endurance running and the evolution of Homo Nature 2004, 432, 345–352. [Google Scholar]
119.
Vaneechoutte M. The origin of articulate language revisited: The potential of a semi-aquatic past of human ancestors to explain the origin of human musicality and articulate language.  Hum. Evol. 2014, 29, 1–33. [Google Scholar]
120.
Kimbel WH, Villmoare B. From Australopithecus to Homo: the transition that wasn’t.  Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2016, 371, 20150248. [Google Scholar]
121.
Grabowski M, Voje KL, Hansen TF. Evolutionary modeling and correcting for observation error support a 3/5 brain-body allometry for primates.  J. Hum. Evol. 2016, 94, 106–116. [Google Scholar]
122.
Gunz P, Neubauer S, Falk D, Tafforeau P, Le Cabec A, Smith TM, et al. Australopithecus afarensis endocasts suggest ape-like brain organization and prolonged brain growth.  Sci. Adv. 2020, 6, eaaz4729. [Google Scholar]
123.
Cofran Z. Brain size growth in Australopithecus J. Hum. Evol. 2019, 130, 72–82. [Google Scholar]
124.
Falk D. Ape-like endocast of “ape-man” Taung.  Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1989, 80, 335–339. [Google Scholar]
125.
Kano T. The Last Ape: Pygmy Chimpanzee Behavior and Ecology; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1992.
126.
Zihlman A.  Pygmy chimps, people, and the pundits.  New Sci. 1984, 104, 39–40. [Google Scholar]
127.
Green DJ, Gordon AD, Richmond BG. Limb-size proportions in Australopithecus afarensis and Australopithecus africanus J. Hum. Evol. 2007, 52, 187–200. [Google Scholar]
128.
Reno PL, Meindl RS, McCollum MA, Lovejoy CO. Sexual dimorphism in Australopithecus afarensis was similar to that of modern humans.  Proc. Natl. Acad. USA 2003, 100, 9404–9409. [Google Scholar]
129.
Schultz AH. The Life of Primates; Universe Books: New York, NY, USA, 1969.
130.
Lee S-H. Patterns of size sexual dimorphism in Australopithecus afarensis: another look.  Homo 2005, 56, 219–232. [Google Scholar]
131.
Stern JT Jr, Susman RL. The locomotor anatomy of Australopithecus afarensis Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1983, 60, 279–317. [Google Scholar]
132.
Chirchir H, Kivell TL, Ruff CB, Hublin JJ, Carlson KJ, Zipfel B, et al. Recent origin of low trabecular bone density in modern humans.  Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 366–371. [Google Scholar]
133.
Lewin R. The earliest “humans” were more like apes.  Science 1987, 236, 1061–1063. [Google Scholar]
134.
Roffman I, Nevo E. Can chimpanzee biology highlight human origin and evolution?  Rambam Maimonides Med. J. 2010, 1, e0009. [Google Scholar]
135.
Rolian C, Gordon AD. Reassessing manual proportions in Australopithecus afarensis Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2013, 152, 393–406. [Google Scholar]
136.
McPherron SP, Alemseged Z, Marean CW, Wynn JG, Reed D, Geraads D, et al. Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia.  Nature 2010, 466, 857–860. [Google Scholar]
137.
Goodall J. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1986.
138.
Mercader J, Barton H, Gillespie J, Harris J, Kuhn S, Tyler R, et al. 4300-Year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology.  Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 3043–3048. [Google Scholar]
139.
Fernandes MEB. Tool use and predation of oysters by the tufted capuchin in brackish water mangrove swamp.  Primates 1991, 32, 529–531. [Google Scholar]
140.
Falótico T, Proffitt T, Ottoni EB, Staff RA, Haslam M. Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use.  Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2019, 3, 1034–1038. [Google Scholar]
141.
Motes-Rodrigo A, McPherron SP, Archer W, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Tennie C. Experimental investigation of orangutans’ lithic percussive and sharp stone tool behaviours.  PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0263343. [Google Scholar]
142.
Walker A, Leakey R. The Nariokotome Homo erectus Skeleton; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1993.
143.
Straus WL. Studies in primate ilia.  Am. J. Anat. 1929, 43, 403–460. [Google Scholar]
144.
Lovejoy CO. Natural history of human gait and posture. Part 1. Spine and pelvis.  Gait Posture 2005, 21, 95–112. [Google Scholar]
145.
Steudel KL. Locomotor energetics and hominid evolution.  Evol. Anthropol. 1994, 3, 42–48. [Google Scholar]
146.
Verhaegen M, Munro S. The continental shelf hypothesis.  Nutr. Health 2002, 16, 25–27. [Google Scholar]
147.
Harcourt-Smith WEH. Early hominin diversity and the emergence of the genus Homo J. Anthropol. Sci. 2016, 94, 19–27. [Google Scholar]
148.
Berillon G. In what manner did they walk on two legs? In From Biped to Strider; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2004, pp. 85–100.
149.
Leakey MD, Hay RL. Pliocene footprints in the Laetolil Beds at Laetoli, northern Tanzania. Nature 1979, 278, 317–323. [Google Scholar]
150.
Meldrum DJ, Lockley MG, Lucas SG, Musiba C. Ichnotaxonomy of the Laetoli trackways: The earliest hominin footprints.  J. Afr. Sci. 2011, 60, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
151.
Harcourt-Smith WEH. The origins of bipedal locomotion. In Handbook of Paleoanthropology; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2013, pp. 1–36.
152.
1Harper CM, Ruff CB, Sylvester AD. Calcaneal shape variation in humans, nonhuman primates, and early hominins.  J. Hum. Evol. 2021, 159, 103050. [Google Scholar]
153.
Anaya A, Patel BA, Orr CM, Ward CV, Almécija S. Evolutionary trends of the lateral foot in catarrhine primates: Contextualizing the fourth metatarsal of Australopithecus afarensis J. Hum. Evol. 2021, 161, 103078. [Google Scholar]
154.
Harcourt-Smith WE, Aiello LC. Fossils, feet and the evolution of human bipedal locomotion.  J. Anat. 2004, 204, 403–416. [Google Scholar]
155.
Rein TR, Harrison T, Carlson KJ, Harvati K. Adaptation to suspensory locomotion in Australopithecus sediba J. Hum. Evol. 2017, 104, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
156.
Wiseman ALA. Three-dimensional volumetric muscle reconstruction of the Australopithecus afarensis pelvis and limb, with estimations of limb leverage.  R. Soc. Open Sci. 2023, 10, 230356. [Google Scholar]
157.
Prado-Martinez J, Sudmant PH, Kidd JM, Li H, Kelley JL, Lorente-Galdos B, et al. Great ape genetic diversity and population history.  Nature 2013, 499, 471–475. [Google Scholar]
158.
Pilbeam D. Genetic and morphological records of the Hominoidea and hominid origins: A synthesis.  Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 1996, 5, 155–168. [Google Scholar]
159.
Zihlman AL, Bolter DR. Body composition in Pan paniscus compared with Homo sapiens has implications for changes during human evolution.  Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 7466–7471. [Google Scholar]
160.
Yohn CT, Jiang Z, McGrath SD, Hayden KE, Khaitovich P, Johnson ME, et al. Lineage-specific expansions of retroviral insertions within the genomes of African great apes but not humans and orangutans.  PLoS Biol. 2005, 3, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
161.
Kaiser SM, Malik HS, Emerman M. Restriction of an extinct retrovirus by the human TRIM5 antiviral protein.  Science 2007, 316, 1756–1758. [Google Scholar]
162.
Benveniste RE, Todaro GJ. Evolution of type C viral genes. Evidence for an Asian origin of man.  Nature 1976, 261, 101–108. [Google Scholar]
163.
Perez-Caballero D, Soll SJ, Bieniasz PD. Evidence for restriction of ancient primate gammaretroviruses by APOBEC3 but not TRIM5α proteins.  PLoS Pathog. 2008, 4, e1000181. [Google Scholar]
164.
Joordens JCA, Feibel CS, Vonhof HB, Schulp AS, Kroon D. Relevance of the eastern African coastal forest for early hominin biogeography.  J. Hum. Evol. 2019, 131, 176–202. [Google Scholar]
165.
Speth JD. Early hominid hunting and scavenging: The role of meat as an energy source.  J. Hum. Evol. 1989, 18, 329–343. [Google Scholar]
166.
Derricourt R. The enigma of Raymond Dart.  Int. J. Afr. Hist. Stud. 2009, 42, 257–282. [Google Scholar]
167.
Antón SC, Potts R, Aiello LC. Evolution of early Homo: An integrated biological perspective.  Science 2014, 344, 1236828. [Google Scholar]
168.
Dominguez-Rodrigo M. Is the “Savanna hypothesis” a dead concept for explaining the emergence of the earliest hominins?  Curr. Anthropol. 2014, 55, 59–69. [Google Scholar]
169.
Morgan E. The Aquatic Ape Hypothesis; Souvenir Press: London, UK, 1997.
170.
Schweitzer MH, Schroeter ER, Cleland TP, Zheng W. Paleoproteomics of Mesozoic dinosaurs and other Mesozoic fossils.  Proteomics 2019, 19, 1800251. [Google Scholar]
TOP