Next Article in Journal
The Difference in Nutrient Intakes between Chinese and Mediterranean, Japanese and American Diets
Previous Article in Journal
Adolescents’ Food Choice and the Place of Plant-Based Foods
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

I Eat Healthier Than You: Differences in Healthy and Unhealthy Food Choices for Oneself and for Others

1
Psychological Assessment and Health Psychology, University of Konstanz, P.O. Box 47, Konstanz 78457, Germany
2
General Psychology, University of Konstanz, P.O. Box 36, Konstanz 78457, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2015, 7(6), 4638-4660; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7064638
Submission received: 21 April 2015 / Revised: 11 May 2015 / Accepted: 1 June 2015 / Published: 9 June 2015

Abstract

:
The present study investigated self-other biases in actual eating behavior based on the observation of three different eating situations. To capture the complexity of real life food choices within a well-controlled setting, an ecologically valid fake food buffet with 72 different foods was employed. Sixty participants chose a healthy, a typical, and an unhealthy meal for themselves and for an average peer. We found that the typical meal for the self was more similar to the healthy than to the unhealthy meal in terms of energy content: The mean difference between the typical and healthy meals was MΔ = 1368 kJ (327 kcal) as compared to a mean difference between the typical and unhealthy meals of MΔ = 3075 kJ (735 kcal). Moreover, there was evidence that people apply asymmetrical standards for themselves and others: Participants chose more energy for a peer than for themselves (M = 4983 kJ or 1191 kcal on average for the peers’ meals vs. M = 3929 kJ or 939 kcal on average for the own meals) and more high-caloric food items for a typical meal, indicating a self-other bias. This comparatively positive self-view is in stark contrast to epidemiological data indicating overall unhealthy eating habits and demands further examination of its consequences for behavior change.

1. Introduction

Headlines stating that people have unhealthy eating habits such as “The way America eats is killing us” in September 2013 [1] are not surprising. Most people, experts as well as laypeople, seem to agree that eating habits in the general population are unhealthy, with too many calories and too many high-caloric foods.
However, when asking people how they gauge their own eating habits, a contrasting picture emerges. As in other life domains, many people tend to believe that they behave more positively than their average peers. For example, Sparks and colleagues [2] reported that respondents believed on average that they eat less fat and sweets in relation to others (see also [3]). In a similar vein, in a community sample, we found that only 9% believed that their own eating was less healthy than their peers’ eating, whereas 54% rated their eating as healthier than their peers’ eating [4]. This positive view of one’s own eating habits stands in contrast to epidemiological data which indicates that many people in western societies eat too much as well as too many high-caloric foods and do not eat enough fruits and vegetables. This raises the question of why we think that others eat more unhealthily than we do.

1.1. Self-Other Biases

The phenomenon that people think more positively of themselves than others, often to an unrealistic degree, has been demonstrated extensively (see [5,6,7,8] for reviews). This self-favoring difference in self- and others-perception has been named as “unrealistic optimism” [9], “false uniqueness” [10], or the “holier than thou” effect [11], depending on the judgment domain. Importantly, flattering self-assessments can stem from three different mechanisms. First, people might perceive themselves overly optimistically but others rather accurately (self-bias). Second, people might perceive themselves relatively accurately but others too pessimistically (peer-bias). Third, people might perceive both themselves and others inaccurately. Accordingly, to disentangle these three possible types of misjudgment, self- and others-views need to be assessed separately [5,7,8]. Whereas numerous studies have assessed self and peer related views in various judgment domains (e.g., [12]), to our knowledge, no study has yet assessed self- and peer-views separately in respect to eating. Hence, although people clearly demonstrate a self-flattering bias at the group level when they claim that they eat more healthily than their peers on average, evidence to identify whether the self-view, peer-view, or both is the source of this bias is lacking.
There are numerous psychological mechanisms underlying self-flattering biases (e.g., [5,8,13]). In particular, general or broad personality traits and behaviors typically elicit a stronger self-favoring bias than those which are more concrete and whose definition is more constrained [5]. In a similar vein, people are typically asked about hypothetical habits and behaviors, that is, how they and their peers generally behave or would behave in a given situation (see [11], see also [14]). As this represents a rather ambiguous judgment task, people might either lack the necessary information to provide accurate assessments or fail to integrate and weigh all relevant and available information [5].
Actual behavior observation reduces the ambiguity of the judgment task for participants and could, thereby, provide a more accurate picture of self-other biases than hypothetical or self-report assessment methods. Interestingly, in studies in which predicted and actual behavior was observed, people were typically more accurate in predicting others’ behaviors than in predicting their own behaviors, indicating a self-bias ([11,14], for review see [15]). For example, Epley and Dunning [11] showed that 83% of their sample predicted that they would buy a flower at a charity event, whereas they predicted that 56% of their peers would do so. After the charity event, 43% of the participants had actually bought a flower. Since the average prediction for the self was more discrepant from the observed behavior than the prediction for the peers, Epley and Dunning [11] concluded that participants showed a greater self- than peer-bias (see also [14]). However, one could argue that the observed self-bias is mainly due to poor prediction abilities for the self rather than biased perceptions of actual behavior. Recent studies suggest that people base their predictions on “ideal” behavior and, for example, attach too much weight to current intentions, whereas they fail to make realistic adjustments for situational constraints on intended actions [16].
In order to disentangle prediction errors from self-flattering perceptions of one’s own actual behavior as compared to others, we need to elicit separate behavioral probes for the self and for others. However, in current study designs this is not possible since, for a single observed behavior (e.g., likelihood of charity donation), average peer-behavior (e.g., that 43% of the sample made a donation) is an aggregate of self-behaviors and, therefore, equal to the average self-behavior. This raises the question, how can we measure self-other biases in actual behavior?

1.2. Measuring Self-Other Biases in Actual Eating Behaviors: Probing Behavioral Standards for the Self and an Average Peer

A first challenge in measuring self-other biases in actual behavior is to observe behaviors that are representative of what people do in their everyday lives [5,17]. Whereas natural environments have the advantage of ecological validity, a disadvantage is that food choice can be confounded by, for example, context variables such as food variety or lighting (e.g., [18]; see [19] for an overview). In contrast, laboratories offer well-controlled settings [20]. However, laboratory research faces the challenge of being representative of real-life situations; as Brunswik [21] put it, it has to have a representative design (see also [22]). Simulating the diversity of available food choices in real life in a standardized way is methodologically challenging and often conflicts with practical issues, for example, high costs, limited infrastructure, high preparation effort, and waste. To overcome these problems, an innovative method was recently developed: using replica food items [23]. With fake food items, real life, such as eating in buffet style restaurants, can be simulated under well-controlled conditions. This method has high ecological validity, has been shown to be reliable and a valid assessment of real food choices [23]. Furthermore, it allows capturing the complexity of food choice behavior that consumers face. On the one hand, how much people eat, that is the total energy content, is central for energy balance and weight maintenance (e.g., [24]). On the other hand, eating behavior comprises more than just consuming energy as an incredibly large variety of food items is available in the western world. With the fake food method, both variety and quantity can be assessed by food profiles and total energy content.
A second great challenge in measuring self-other biases in actual behavior is to separate behavior observations for the two different targets: the self and others. We would like to suggest a new method for assessing self-other biases in actual eating behavior based on the observation of three different eating situations: a typical, a healthy, and an unhealthy meal. Food choices in these three situations give information about a range of prototypical behaviors and, thus, give information which standards people apply, on average, for themselves and others in the different situations. Selected meals are a proxy for meals that are regarded as exemplary for the self and others in these situations. Hence, typical, healthy, and unhealthy behavioral standards for the self and others can be assessed.
Using a fake food buffet as standardized eating situation, it can be observed which food choices people make when they choose a typical, a healthy, or an unhealthy meal for themselves and for an average peer. These choices for the self versus for others enable a comparison between the average self-behavior, that is the “actual” average behavior profile (aggregated food choices for the self), and the average peer-behavior, that is the “assumed” average behavior profile (aggregated food choices for the average peer). The crucial question is whether people use different standards within and between the three different eating situations as a function of the target consumer (self vs. average peer). Hence, the selection of a different amount of food or other food items for the peer than for the self within one eating situation (e.g., typical meal) would indicate a self-other difference in eating standards. However, if people systematically choose more food (i.e., energy), or different food (e.g., more unhealthy food items) for the peer than for the self across the three different eating situations, this would provide clear evidence of asymmetrical behavioral standards for the self and others and thereby a self-other bias in actual behavior.

1.3. The Present Study

The goals of the present study were twofold: (a) to investigate behavioral standards within the self and within the peer as target consumers and (b) to examine whether asymmetrical behavioral standards are employed for the self as compared to an average peer. To this end, behavioral standards for three different eating situations were assessed: a typical, a healthy, and an unhealthy meal. In addition, participants were asked to compose these three different meals for two different target consumers: for themselves and for an average peer. The present study employed a fake food buffet to simulate the diversity of available food choices in real life within a well-controlled experimental setting [23]. It was observed both how much people chose (total energy content) and what they chose (food item profiles).

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Sixty participants from the University of Konstanz took part in the study. Of these, 49 (82%) were female. Participants had a mean age of 22.4 years (SD = 5.5, ranging from 19 to 58 years) and a mean body mass index (BMI) of 22.1 kg/m2 (SD = 2.6, ranging from 17.9 to 30.0 kg/m2). Participants were recruited via flyers and received €8 for their participation.
All participants gave written informed consent prior to the collection of data. The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The procedures were performed in compliance with relevant laws and institutional guidelines. We strictly followed the German Psychological Society’s (Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie) guidelines for conducting psychological studies [25] (see paragraph C.III). These are similar to those of the American Psychological Association. Moreover, the study was reviewed by the Ethics Committee (Institutional Review Board) of the University of Konstanz, Baden-Württemberg, Germany (project identification code: IRB15KN007ML; 28 May 2015). The study was judged to be exempt from full Institutional Review Board review as no threats to human health, well-being, or dignity were identified.

2.2. Material

In order to investigate participants’ food choices, a buffet with replica food items was prepared (see Figure 1). The composition of the fake food buffet aimed to represent a typical lunch buffet. An exploratory pilot study was conducted. Six participants were instructed to put together a lunch meal for themselves and for an average person of the same age and gender, that is their peer, from a fake food buffet with 55 different food items (see [26]). Participants were questioned how typical the buffet was and which items were missing or overrepresented. Overall, they found the buffet to be realistic, to include a large selection of foods, and found it easy to choose meals. However, they suggested adding sauces, dessert mousses (e.g., pudding, quark), and a larger selection of salad items. In line with their remarks, the composition of the buffet was adapted. The final fake food buffet contained a salad buffet, a selection of main courses and side dishes, a dessert buffet, and drinks. In total, it included 72 different food items (see Figure 1 and Table A1, Appendix).
For food selection, large plates (27 cm diameter), small plates (20 cm diameter), bowls (250 mL capacity), and trays (38 × 58 cm) were provided. All replica food items were manufactured by Döring GmbH, Munich, Germany. Small packets of real food condiments were also offered: Salt, pepper, mayonnaise, ketchup, and mustard. To estimate the energy content of the replica foods, the conversion factors for the weight of fake food items to corresponding real food items were calculated. Weight data was either derived from Bucher and colleagues [26] or gained by preparing corresponding real food items and taking weight measures (see Table A1, Appendix). The estimation of nutrients was based on the national database for nutritional information in Germany (Bundeslebensmittelschlüssel, [27]).
The amount of energy per meal was established by adding the caloric values of all food items selected for each meal. Additionally, to assess the quality of food choices, all food items were categorized based on a qualitative food hierarchy concerning the four food groups plant foods, animal foods, oils and fats, and drinks (German Nutrition Society, [28]; see also [29]). Within these four groups, foods were categorized as healthy, neutral, or unhealthy. The healthy food category included foods with low energy and high nutrient density as well as the recommendation to consume in high quantities. The neutral food category included foods with medium energy and medium nutrient density. The unhealthy food category included foods with high energy and low nutrient density as well as the recommendation to be consumed in small quantities. In total, 36 food items (51%) were assigned to the healthy food category, 12 food items (17%) to the neutral food category, and 22 food items (31%) to the unhealthy food category. Salt and pepper were excluded from the assignment as they were classified as spices.
Figure 1. Fake food buffet containing 72 different food items.
Figure 1. Fake food buffet containing 72 different food items.
Nutrients 07 04638 g001

2.3. Procedure

Participants were invited to the laboratory individually. In order to create standardized internal states of hunger, they were asked not to eat and to drink only water for two hours before participation. After giving informed consent, participants filled in a first questionnaire on demographic data. During this stage, the fake food buffet was hidden behind a movable wall to avoid potential distractions and influences.
Next, participants were instructed to compose different lunch meals from the fake food buffet. In total, participants composed six meals in two serving rounds. One serving round either consisted of successively choosing three meals for oneself (target: self) or three meals for an average peer of the same age and gender (target: peer). Within the serving rounds, participants were asked to compose a healthy meal (meal type: healthy), a meal they would typically compose (meal type: typical), and an unhealthy meal (meal type: unhealthy). Exact instructions for the typical meal type were as follows:
“Please choose a lunch meal as you would typically choose for yourself in such a buffet situation.” (target: self)
“Please choose a lunch meal as an average person of your age and gender would typically choose in such a buffet situation.” (target: peer)
For the healthy and unhealthy meal type, instructions were the same except for replacing “a lunch meal as … would typically choose in such a buffet situation” with “a healthy/unhealthy lunch meal”.
In order to control for possible carry-over and order effects, the order of choices was alternated starting either with the self- or the peer-condition. Within the self- and peer-condition, the order of meal types was alternated, too. However, the same order of meal types was applied in the two target conditions. Participants were randomly assigned to the order of choices.
After each meal was completed, the experimenter photographed the tray and weighed each food component or counted the single food items. Between the serving rounds, the buffet was refilled to initial levels, and the participant filled in a brief questionnaire on food habits. Afterwards, height and weight were measured following standardized procedures. Participants wore light indoor clothing. Height was measured without shoes to the nearest 0.1 cm using a wall-mounted stadiometer. Weight was measured using a digital scale (Omron Body Composition Monitor, BF511, Omron, Hoofddorp, The Netherland) to the nearest 0.1 kg. Finally, participants were carefully debriefed.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS statistics software, version 22.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). In order to test the research questions, ANOVAs including simple main effects and simple contrasts were computed. p-Values for non-orthogonal contrasts were Bonferroni-corrected. In case of violation of the assumption of sphericity, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates. All tests were based on a 0.05 significance level.
Concerning the total amount of energy, dependent variables were (1) the total amount of energy per meal and (2) the difference between the energy content of the typical meal minus the energy content of the healthy meal as well as the difference between the energy content of the unhealthy meal minus the energy content of the typical meal.
Concerning the quality of food choices, the data set was restructured, treating the 70 food items included in the healthy, neutral, and unhealthy categories as single observations and the number of participants who chose each food item within each of the six meals as continuous variable (see Figure 3). Dependent variables for these analyses were (1) the total number of participants who chose each food item and (2) the difference between the number of participants who chose each food item for the typical meal minus for the healthy meal and for the typical minus for the unhealthy meal. To ensure that the mean of each difference was positive, the difference score typical-healthy was multiplied by −1 for healthy food items and the difference score typical-unhealthy was multiplied by −1 for unhealthy food items.
Extreme values concerning the amount of energy chosen from the fake food buffet and the total number of participants who chose each food item were detected by interquartile range measures. These extreme values rendered some variables as severely skewed. After excluding extreme values, the skewness of dependent variables was acceptable (|skewness| < 1.73; |kurtosis| < 3.24; [30]). To secure our results, all analyses were repeated excluding extreme values. These analyses confirmed results reported below.

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral Standards for the Self and Others: How Much Did Participants Choose?

A 3 × 2 ANOVA with meal type (healthy vs. typical vs. unhealthy) and target (self vs. peer) as within-subject factors, and the total amount of energy as a dependent variable revealed significant main effects of meal type and target which were qualified by a significant meal type × target interaction (see Table 1). To follow up the interaction, additional analyses were conducted for the two factors.
Effects of meal type: Behavioral standards for the self. All simple contrasts of meal type within the self were significant (see Table 1). As shown in Figure 2, participants chose for themselves 3929 kJ (SE = 207; 939 kcal, SE = 49.5) for a typical meal, 2561 kJ (SE = 166; 612 kcal, SE = 39.6) for the healthy meal, and 7004 kJ (SE = 373; 1674 kcal, SE = 89.2) for the unhealthy meal. Thus, participants set different behavioral standards across the three different meal types when choosing for themselves.
Effects of meal type: Behavioral standards for the peer. As with the meals for the self, all simple contrasts of meal type were significant for the peer (see Table 1). For an average peer, participants chose 4983 kJ (SE = 269; 1191 kcal, SE = 64.2) for a typical meal, 2824 kJ (SE = 150; 675 kcal, SE = 35.8) for a healthy meal, and 8238 kJ (SE = 460; 1969 kcal, SE = 110.0) for an unhealthy meal. Thus, participants also set different behavioral standards across the three different meal types when choosing for an average peer.
Figure 2. Amount of energy chosen from the fake food buffet as a function of meal type and target. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01.
Figure 2. Amount of energy chosen from the fake food buffet as a function of meal type and target. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01.
Nutrients 07 04638 g002
Effects of the target person: Comparing behavioral standards for the self and peer. Interestingly, systematic differences were seen when comparing choices between the self and peer within the three eating situations. For both the typical and unhealthy meal, participants chose significantly more energy for an average peer than for themselves (see Figure 2). However, for the healthy meal type, the amount of energy chosen was similar.
The similarity of the typical to the healthy and unhealthy meal: A difference score analysis. For further comparison of the behavioral standards for the self and peer, a 2 × 2 ANOVA with the meal difference score (typical-healthy vs. unhealthy-typical) and target (self vs. peer) as within-subject factors was computed (see Table 1). This revealed significant main effects of meal discrepancy and target. Specifically, within the self, the discrepancy between the typical and healthy meal was significantly smaller than the discrepancy between the typical and unhealthy meal, MΔ = 1368 kJ; SE = 165 vs. MΔ = 3075 kJ; SE = 326 (MΔ = 327 kcal; SE = 39.5 vs. MΔ = 735 kcal; SE = 78.0). Hence, participants’ typical behavioral standard was more similar to their healthy behavioral standard than to their unhealthy behavioral standard. Simple main effect analyses within the peers’ meals showed a similar pattern to the own meals: The difference between the typical and the healthy meal was significantly smaller than the difference between the typical and the unhealthy meal, MΔ = 2155 kJ; SE = 229 vs. MΔ = 3255 kJ; SE = 396 (MΔ = 515 kcal; SE = 54.8 vs. MΔ = 778 kcal; SE = 94.7). However, comparing the two targets showed a significant self-other discrepancy: When participants chose for themselves, the difference between the typical and the healthy meal was significantly smaller than when they chose for their peer. The difference between the typical and the unhealthy meal did not differ when participants chose for themselves versus for their peer.
Table 1. Results of the ANOVAS with total amount of energy chosen from the buffet as dependent variable.
Table 1. Results of the ANOVAS with total amount of energy chosen from the buffet as dependent variable.
Effectdf Effectdf ErrorFpη2p
3 × 2 ANOVA with the factors meal type (healthy vs. typical vs. unhealthy) and target (self vs. peer)
Main effect meal type1.3881.34181.36< 0.0010.76
Main effect target15928.28< 0.0010.32
Meal type × target interaction1.5993.873.850.0340.06
Simple contrasts within self
Healthy vs. typical meal15968.63< 0.0010.54
Typical vs. unhealthy meal15988.69< 0.0010.60
Healthy vs. unhealthy meal159170.25< 0.0010.74
Simple contrasts within peer
Healthy vs. typical meal15988.53< 0.0010.60
Typical vs. unhealthy meal15967.49< 0.0010.53
Healthy vs. unhealthy meal159183.90< 0.0010.76
Simple main effects target
Healthy meal1592.350.131-
Typical meal15924.65< 0.0010.30
Unhealthy meal15910.950.0020.16
2 × 2 ANOVA with the factors meal discrepancy (typical-healthy vs. unhealthy-typical) and target (self vs. peer)
Main effect meal discrepancy15915.87< 0.0010.21
Main effect target1595.660.0210.09
Meal discrepancy × target interaction1591.120.294-
Simple main effects meal discrepancy
Self15919.14< 0.0010.25
Peer1594.640.0350.07
Simple main effects target
Typical-healthy meal1599.010.0040.13
Unhealthy-typical meal1590.180.672-

3.2. Behavioral Standards for the Self and Others: What Did Participants Choose?

A 3 × 2 × 3 ANOVA with the within-subject factors meal type (healthy vs. typical vs. unhealthy) and target (self vs. peer), the between-subject factor food category (healthy vs. neutral vs. unhealthy), and the number of participants who chose the food items as dependent variable revealed a significant meal type × food category interaction, target × food category interaction, and a significant three-way interaction (see Table 2). Effects of meal type and target were investigated in follow-up analyses.
Effects of meal type: Behavioral standards for the self. As shown in Figure 3, within the own meals, significantly more participants chose healthy food items for their healthy meal (M = 22; SE = 2.0) as compared to their typical meal (M = 19; SE = 2.0) and as compared to their unhealthy meal (M = 4; SE = 1.3). Moreover, significantly more participants chose healthy food items for their typical meal as compared to their unhealthy meal. For instance, 53 participants chose lettuce for their healthy meal, 48 for their typical meal, and 11 for their unhealthy meal. This pattern was reversed for the unhealthy food items: These were significantly less often chosen for participants’ healthy meal (M = 2; SE = 2.6) as compared to their typical meal (M = 7; SE = 2.6) and as compared to their unhealthy meal (M = 16; SE = 1.7). They were also significantly less often chosen for participants’ typical meals as compared to their unhealthy meals. In contrast, neutral food items were equally often chosen for participants’ healthy, typical, and unhealthy meals.
Figure 3. (A) Number of participants who chose the single food items for their own healthy, typical, and unhealthy meals (actual average behavior profile); (B) Number of participants who chose the single food items for the peers’ healthy, typical, and unhealthy meal (assumed average behavior profile).
Figure 3. (A) Number of participants who chose the single food items for their own healthy, typical, and unhealthy meals (actual average behavior profile); (B) Number of participants who chose the single food items for the peers’ healthy, typical, and unhealthy meal (assumed average behavior profile).
Nutrients 07 04638 g003
Effects of meal type: Behavioral standards for the peer. Inspecting simple contrasts revealed the same effects for the peers’ meals as for the own meals: Significantly more participants chose healthy food items for the peers’ healthy meal (M = 23; SE = 2.1) as compared to the peers’ typical meal (M = 15; SE = 2.2), and as compared to the peers’ unhealthy meal (M = 2; SE = 1.7). Moreover, significantly more participants chose healthy food items for the peers’ typical meal as compared to the peers’ unhealthy meal. This pattern was reversed for the unhealthy food items: These were chosen significantly less often for the peers’ healthy meal (M = 2; SE = 2.7) as compared to the peers’ typical meal (M = 9; SE = 2. 8) and as compared to the peers’ unhealthy meal (M = 20; SE = 2.2) as well as chosen less often for the peers’ typical meal as compared to the peers’ unhealthy meal. In contrast, neutral food items were chosen equally often for the peers’ healthy, typical, and unhealthy meals.
Effects of the target person: Comparing behavioral standards for the self and peer. Interestingly, comparing the meals chosen for the self versus for the peer revealed that even when choosing unhealthy meals, healthy food items were chosen more often for individuals’ own meals (M = 4, SE = 1.3) as compared to the peers’ meals (M = 2, SE = 1.7). Moreover, as shown in Figure 3, unhealthy food items were chosen less often for the own meal (M = 16, SE = 1.7) as compared to the peers’ meal (M = 20, SE = 2.2). For instance, only 12 participants chose Bratwurst for their own unhealthy meal, whereas 31 participants chose Bratwurst for their peers’ unhealthy meal. Moreover, when choosing typical meals, healthy food items were chosen more often for the own meal (M = 19, SE = 2.0) as compared to the peers’ meal (M = 15, SE = 2.2). This was the case, for example, for water, which 33 participants chose for their own typical meal, whereas only 10 participants chose water for their peers’ typical meals. The remaining simple main effects of target were not significant.
The similarity of the typical to the healthy and unhealthy meals: A difference score analysis. To further investigate these self-other discrepancies in behavioral standards, the similarity of the typical meal to the healthy and unhealthy meal was examined using difference scores. The 2 × 2 × 3 ANOVA with the within-subject factors meal discrepancy (typical-healthy vs. typical-unhealthy) and target (self vs. peer) and the between-subjects factor food category (healthy vs. neutral vs. unhealthy) yielded a significant main effect of meal discrepancy and target (see Table 2). Simple main effect analyses showed that participants’ typical meal was significantly more similar to their healthy meal (MΔ = 3; SE = 0.8) than to their unhealthy meal (MΔ = 15; SE = 2.1) concerning healthy food items. For instance, even more participants (n = 43) chose carrots for their typical meal than for their healthy meal (n = 38). In contrast, only nine participants chose carrots for their unhealthy meal. The simple main effect of meal discrepancy within the self was not significant for the neutral and unhealthy food category. Interestingly, simple main effect analyses for the peers’ meals revealed that the peers’ typical meals were not closer to the peers’ healthy meals than to the peers’ unhealthy meals concerning all three food categories. Even more participants (n = 32) chose crumbed schnitzel, for example, for the peers’ typical meal than for the peers’ unhealthy meal (n = 27). Moreover, the discrepancy between the typical and healthy meal was significantly smaller for the own meal (MΔ = 3; SE = 0.8) as compared to the peers’ meals (MΔ = 8; SE = 1.6) within the healthy food category. For instance, cauliflower was chosen 26 times for participants’ own typical meal and 33 times for their healthy meal. In contrast, cauliflower was chosen 14 times for the peers’ typical meals and 33 times for the peers’ healthy meals. Hence, the discrepancy between typical and healthy was only seven for own meals but as high as 19 for the peers’ meals. The remaining simple main effects of target were not significant.
Table 2. Results of the ANOVAS with the number of participants who chose the food items as dependent variables.
Table 2. Results of the ANOVAS with the number of participants who chose the food items as dependent variables.
Effectdf Effectdf ErrorFpη2p
3 × 2 × 3 ANOVA with the factors meal type (healthy vs. typical vs. unhealthy), target (self vs. peer), and food category (healthy vs. neutral vs. unhealthy)
Main effects and meal type × target interaction--< 2.90> 0.062-
Meal type × food category interaction2.6086.9330.48< 0.0010.48
Target × food category interaction2678.490.0010.20
Three-way interaction3.13104.994.350.0060.12
Simple contrasts within self
Healthy food category
Healthy vs. typical meal16713.54< 0.0010.17
Typical vs. unhealthy meal16749.02< 0.0010.42
Healthy vs. unhealthy meal16758.45< 0.0010.47
Neutral food category167< 5.22> 0.075-
Unhealthy food category
Healthy vs. typical meal16721.33< 0.0010.24
Typical vs. unhealthy meal16711.710.0030.15
Healthy vs. unhealthy meal16722.15< 0.0010.25
Simple contrasts within peer
Healthy food category
Healthy vs. typical meal16726.19< 0.0010.28
Typical vs. unhealthy meal16727.20< 0.0010.29
Healthy vs. unhealthy meal16757.61< 0.0010.46
Neutral food category167< 2.17> 0.146-
Unhealthy food category
Healthy vs. typical meal16712.160.0030.15
Typical vs. unhealthy meal16711.030.0030.14
Healthy vs. unhealthy meal16724.66< 0.0010.27
Simple main effects target
Healthy food category
Healthy meal1672.750.102-
Typical meal16711.010.0010.14
Unhealthy meal1675.580.0210.08
Neutral food category < 0.35> 0.555-
Unhealthy food category
Healthy meal1670.320.573-
Typical meal1672.610.111-
Unhealthy meal16711.670.0010.15
2 × 2 × 3 ANOVA with the factor meal discrepancy (typical-healthy vs. typical-unhealthy), target (self vs. peer), and food category (healthy vs. neutral vs. unhealthy)
Main effect meal discrepancy1676.420.0140.09
Main effect target1677.580.0080.10
Main effect food category and all interactions < 2.21> 0.118-
Simple main effects meal discrepancy within self
Healthy food category16730.04< 0.0010.31
Neutral and unhealthy food category167< 2.68> 0.106-
Simple main effects meal discrepancy within peer
Healthy, neutral and unhealthy food category167< 2.05> 0.157-
Simple main effects target
Healthy food category
Typical-healthy meal16717.91< 0.0010.21
Unhealthy-typical meal1672.540.115-
Neutral and unhealthy food category167< 2.23> 0.140-

4. Discussion

The present study investigated self-other biases in actual eating behavior based on the observation of behavioral standards for three different eating situations. To capture the diversity of food choices available in real life within a well-controlled setting, an ecologically valid fake food buffet was employed. The results showed that, when participants chose for themselves, a typical meal was more similar to a healthy meal than to an unhealthy meal. Hence, one’s own typical behavior was more similar to the healthy behavioral standard than to the unhealthy standard. Importantly, self-related meal choices differed systematically from choices made for an average peer, indicating asymmetrical behavioral standards for the peer and self. Specifically, participants chose less “healthy” meals for others than for themselves both regarding quantity and quality, indicating a self-other discrepancy. The results suggest that the observed self-peer discrepancy may be driven by a peer-bias which is displayed in the less favorable assumed average behavior profiles as compared to the actual average behavior profiles. In addition, the difference between the typical and the healthy behavioral standards was smaller for the self than for others. These results indicate a comparatively positive self-view which is in stark contrast to epidemiological data indicating that many people in the western world eat unhealthily.

4.1. Behavioral Standards for Different Eating Situations

Overall, we found different behavioral standards between the three eating situations both for the self and others. Interestingly, the typical behavioral standard for the self was relatively similar to the standard for the healthy eating situation. Some healthy food items (e.g., carrots, corn) were even chosen more often in the typical eating situation than in the healthy eating situation (see Figure 3). Our results further show relative accuracy for the healthy and unhealthy meals considering the selection of healthy and unhealthy foods. Consequently, the typical eating standard for the self was relatively healthy. This finding might appear to be in contrast to headlines and studies stating that children and adults have unhealthy eating habits [1,31,32]. However, there is evidence that overall unhealthy eating patterns result from unhealthy snacking between meals rather than main meals [33].
Another interesting result is that the similarity of the typical and healthy eating standard for the self was larger for healthy food items than for unhealthy food items. This might reflect that it is easier to consume healthy foods than to suppress consuming unhealthy foods. Specifically, Wegner’s [34] ironic process theory suggests that the suppression of a thought may ironically result in this thought becoming more prevalent. For instance, Adriaanse and colleagues [35] showed that planning not to eat unhealthy snacks ironically resulted in increased consumption.
The finding that the unhealthy and healthy eating standards were relatively accurate is in line with previous research showing that people’s perception of what is healthy, in general, agrees with official recommendations. For example, in several studies, people associated healthy eating with a high intake of fruits and vegetables [36,37,38,39]. It is important to note, however, that the number of participants who chose healthy food items for the healthy meal and unhealthy food items for the unhealthy meal varied across food items. For instance, fries and Coca-Cola were very often chosen for the unhealthy meal, whereas lettuce and tomatoes were very often chosen for the healthy meal (see Figure 3). In contrast, this effect was less clear for other food items such as rice. These variations might, in part, reflect that food items within the healthy and unhealthy category nevertheless differed in their nutritional quality and that the categorization used in the present study is only one of several existing categorizations of healthy and unhealthy foods. Moreover, stereotypes about unhealthy and healthy foods [40] might account for the clear effect for fries, Coca-Cola, lettuce, and tomatoes.

4.2. Behavioral Standards for the Self as Compared to for Others

We found evidence for asymmetrical behavioral standards for the self as compared to for others when it comes to a complex behavior, namely when choosing from 72 different food items. For instance, there was a self-other difference of 1054 kJ (252 kcal) for a typical meal. The found self-other bias is in line with previous findings of unrealistic optimism [9], false uniqueness [10], and the holier-than-thou effect [11]. Furthermore, our results complement and extend previous research on a self-favoring bias in self-reported healthy eating [2,3] and in the prediction of behaviors [11,14,16]. Specifically, our method allowed assessing separate behavior profiles for the self and others, which not only illustrated a self-other bias at the group level but also shed light on the sources of the self-other bias in actual behavior. Since the “assumed” average behavioral profile (aggregated food choices for the average peer) was more negative than the “actual” average behavior profile (aggregated food choices for the self), the observed self-other differences in our study can be attributed to a pessimistically biased peer-perception (peer-bias) which results in a flattering self-perception (see also [41]). As separate behavioral probes for the self and others were observed, prediction errors due to ignoring situational constraints [16] can be excluded as a contributing factor to the observed self-flattering bias. Moreover, observing actual behavior, rather than assessing self-reports of hypothetical or future behavior, rules out a lack or neglect of self-information as an underlying mechanism of the bias [5].
One potential mechanism for the observed peer-bias might be that others’ unhealthy eating behavior is more salient than others’ healthy eating behavior. When people observe others eating, these eating situations are often social. As research has shown that people eat more as well as more unhealthily [42] in social as compared to solo eating situations, people’s peer-perception might rely more often on social and thus, potentially more unhealthy eating situations. Moreover, people might engage in self-enhancement [7,43] and attribute a less favorable behavioral standard for their peers, if their own behavior is not in the desirable range. Conversely, if the own behavior is already in the desirable range (i.e., the healthy eating situation), there might be no need for a behavioral social comparison standard that is less favorable than the behavioral profile of the self. This motivational mechanism is in line with our finding that, although people used different behavioral standards for the self than for others within the typical and unhealthy eating situation, they did not within the healthy eating situation.
Interestingly, despite overall asymmetrical behavioral standards for the self and others in the typical and unhealthy eating situation, “zooming” in on single food items revealed that the size of self-other differences varied across food items. For instance, large differences in behavioral standards were found for water and crumbed schnitzel within the typical eating situation. Water was chosen by 33 participants for the own typical meal but only by 10 participants for the peers’ typical meal. Similarly, there was a large difference for Bratwurst (self: n = 12, peer: n = 31) within the unhealthy eating situation. In contrast, no difference occurred, for example, for chocolate marshmallow within the typical eating situation (self: n = 0, peer: n = 0). This differentiated pattern speaks in favor of differently salient consumption stereotypes. Specifically, research in the domain of risk perception has found larger unrealistic optimism when a salient stereotype is in effect. In other words, people, on average, rated their own risk for a hazard below that of an average person when they easily pictured a clear stereotype of a typical victim of the hazard [9,44]. In line with this, a larger self-other bias in our study might display that people find it easy to imagine a person who eats these specific unhealthy foods or avoids these particular healthy foods. One conclusion from this is that public health education campaigns might facilitate instead of reducing self-other biases in eating behavior (cf., [7]). Specifically, vivid presentations of eaters who avoid healthy and consume unhealthy foods may foster consumption stereotypes that are perceived as dissimilar. This might lead people to the conclusion that, although their behavior is not optimal, they eat healthily compared to others.
The observed self-other discrepancy might also result in part from the fact that our sample had a lower BMI than their average peer (M = 22.1 kg/m2 in our sample vs. M = 23.3 kg/m2 for an average female between 20 and 25 years [45]). Hence, our sample might consume less energy and less unhealthy foods than their average peers and further research is required to investigate self-other discrepancies in a sample which is representative in terms of body mass index (BMI).

4.3. Limitations

Some limitations need to be taken into account. First, our results need to be interpreted with regard to the sample of predominantly female, young adults with a normal BMI and high education from Germany. Second, the fake food buffet lacked the smell of real foods, and the quantity of leftovers could not be assessed. Third, our results provide information about the relative healthiness of meals but not about whether the meals were healthy at an absolute level. The latter requires, for example, investigation of the agreement of the meals with dietary guidelines such as MyPlate [46].

5. Conclusions

The present study uncovered behavioral standards for the self and others within three different eating situations by employing an ecologically valid method to capture the complexity of real life food choices. Participants’ behaviors indicate that they assume that others typically choose more energy and more unhealthy food items than they do. This “I eat more healthily than you” perception seems to be caused by a negative perception of the peer and, thus, needs to be extended to “I eat healthier than you because you are eating so unhealthily” perception. This could explain why most people agree that eating habits in the general population are unhealthy but think that they eat more healthily than others.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Rosemarie Brazdrum, Katrin Ziesemer, Bettina Ott, Tamara Bucher, and Isabella Mauro for their valuable support in setting up the fake food method and conducting the study.
This work was supported by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research within the projects EATMOTIVE (Grant 01EA1326, granted to Britta Renner & Harald Schupp) and SmartAct (Grant 01EL1420A, granted to Britta Renner & Harald Schupp) as well as by the Committee on Research (AFF) of the University of Konstanz (funding within the second funding period of the Excellence Initiative of the German Federal and State Governments, granted to Britta Renner). Publication costs are kindly covered by the DFG and open access publication funds of the University of Konstanz.

Author Contributions

Britta Renner and Harald Schupp conceived and designed the study as well as provided materials. Gudrun Sproesser and Verena Kohlbrenner participated in the generation of the study design. Verena Kohlbrenner performed the study under the supervision of Gudrun Sproesser and Britta Renner. Gudrun Sproesser and Verena Kohlbrenner analyzed the data. Britta Renner and Harald Schupp contributed significantly to the interpretation of results. Gudrun Sproesser and Britta Renner wrote the paper. Harald Schupp provided critical input. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Appendix

Table A1. Details of the food items presented on the fake food buffet.
Table A1. Details of the food items presented on the fake food buffet.
Food ItemReal Food (g)Fake FoodFake Food UnitEnergy ContentEstimation Method aFood Category b
kJ/100 gkcal/100 g
Tomato slice142100g751811
Lettuce69100g501211
Water2001200 mL Portion0011
Cucumber slice92100g501211
Pepper slice143100g1263011
Carrots130100g1383311
Strawberry181Piece1343211
Cauliflower82100g882111
Peas88100g3648711
Blackberry51Piece1674011
Beans76100g1423411
Corn108100g3187611
Olive72100g46411111
Grape101Piece2937021
Kiwi slice61Piece2265421
Fish1401Piece3317921
Italian dressing44150 ml Portion124729811
Banana slice111Piece3779021
Peach slice141Piece1724111
Chicken82100g48111511
Balsamic dressing44150 ml Portion3478311
Cherry131Pair2516011
Apple1401Piece2556121
Salmon321Slice72817421
Plum381Piece1804311
Wholegrain bread311Slice82819821
Banana1261Piece3779021
Apple juice2001200 ml Portion1974711
Orange1681Piece1804321
Orange juice2001200 mL Portion1804311
Wholegrain roll351Piece94122511
Tomato sauce1481150 mL Portion1844411
Onion ring21Piece1172821
Herring701Slice92922221
Mustard10110 mL Portion3608611
Lemon slice181Piece1513611
Plain schnitzel1251Piece84920322
Quark2501250 mL Portion3057312
Vanilla pudding2851250 mL Portion51912412
Coffee3001300 mL Portion8212
Espresso37135 mL Portion8212
Chocolate pudding2851250 mL Portion3979512
Cappuccino2201220 mL Portion1383312
Raspberry quark2501Piece54413012
Herb sauce1501150 mL Portion4069712
Potatoes113100g2977112
Yoghurt dressing50150 mL Portion60714512
Feta110100g118828412
Pasta87100g58213913
Rice110100g52712613
Bread roll221Piece113027013
Carbonara sauce1501150 mL Portion140233513
Chocolate marshmallow231Piece148535523
Apple tart1321Piece89121323
French dressing50150 mL Portion154837013
Fish stick321Piece84120113
Muffin371Piece118028223
Sugar41Piece169540523
Bratwurst1401Piece137732923
Cheese cake1001Piece117628123
Mayonnaise20120 mL Portion310974313
Gravy1371150 mL Portion2185213
Baguette slice241Piece118828413
Burger patty901Piece95022723
Crumbed schnitzel1201Piece95022723
Chocolate101Piece169540523
Chocolate cake1041Piece161138523
Ketchup20120 mL Portion46011013
Coca-Cola2001200 mL Portion1974713
Fries82100g122229213
Salt, Pepper-1Portion----
Note: a Estimation method of weight data: 1 indicates Comparison of fake food to real food, 2 indicates Data derived from [26]; b Food categories were based on the German food pyramid [28]: 1 indicates Healthy foods, 2 indicates Neutral foods, 3 indicates Unhealthy foods.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funding sources had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, and in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Filipovic, J. The Way America Eats Is Killing Us, Something Has to Change. Available online: http://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2013/sep/26/american-diet-report-card-unhealty (accessed on 15 April 2015).
  2. Sparks, P.; Shepherd, R.; Wieringa, N.; Zimmermanns, N. Perceived behavioral control, unrealistic optimism and dietary change: An exploratory study. Appetite 1995, 24, 243–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Paisley, C.M.; Sparks, P. Expectations of reducing fat intake: The role of perceived need within the theory of planned behaviour. Psychol. Health 1998, 13, 341–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Sproesser, G.; Klusmann, V.; Schupp, H.T.; Renner, B. Comparative optimism about healthy eating. Appetite 2015, 90, 212–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Dunning, D.; Heath, C.; Suls, J.M. Flawed self-assessment implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest. 2004, 5, 69–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Miles, S.; Scaife, V. Optimistic bias and food. Nutr. Res. Rev. 2003, 16, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Renner, B.; Schupp, H. The Perception of health risks. In Handbook of Health Psychology; Friedman, H.S., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 637–665. [Google Scholar]
  8. Shepperd, J.A.; Klein, W.M.P.; Waters, E.A.; Weinstein, N.D. Taking stock of unrealistic optimism. Perspect Psychol. Sci. 2013, 8, 395–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Weinstein, N.D. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 806–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Goethals, G.R. Constructing and ignoring social reality: positive self-appraisal consistent estimates of consensus. In Relative Deprivation and Social Comparison: The Ontario Symposium; Olson, J., Herman, C.P., Zanna, M.P., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1986; Volume 4, pp. 135–158. [Google Scholar]
  11. Epley, N.; Dunning, D. Feeling “holier than thou”: Are self-serving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 79, 861–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Perloff, L.S.; Fetzer, B.K. Self-other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. J. Pers Soc. Psychol 1986, 50, 502–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hevey, D.; French, D.P.; Marteau, T.M.; Sutton, S. Assessing unrealistic optimism impact of different approaches to measuring susceptibility to diabetes. J. Health Psychol. 2009, 14, 372–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Balcetis, E.; Dunning, D. Considering the situation: Why people are better social psychologists than self-psychologists. Self Identit. 2013, 12, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Dunning, D. Self-image motives and consumer behavior: How sacrosanct self-beliefs sway preferences in the marketplace. J. Consum. Psychol. 2007, 17, 237–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Poon, C.S.K.; Koehler, D.J.; Buehler, R. On the psychology of self-prediction: Consideration of situational barriers to intended actions. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2014, 9, 207–225. [Google Scholar]
  17. Gigerenzer, G. On narrow norms and vague heuristics: Reply. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 592–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Wansink, B.; van Ittersum, K. Fast food restaurant lighting and music can reduce calorie intake and increase satisfaction. Psychol. Rep. 2012, 111, 228–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Wansink, B. Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2004, 24, 455–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Hagger, M.S.; Panetta, G.; Leung, C.M.; Wong, G.G.; Wang, J.C.K.; Chan, D.K.C.; Keatley, D.A.; Chatzisarantis, N.L.D. Chronic inhibition, self-control and eating behavior: Test of a “resource depletion” model. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e76888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Brunswik, E. Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. Psychol. Rev. 1955, 62, 193–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Dhami, M.K.; Hertwig, R.; Hoffrage, U. The role of representative design in an ecological approach to cognition. Psychol. Bull. 2004, 130, 959–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Bucher, T.; van der Horst, K.; Siegrist, M. The fake food buffet—A new method in nutrition behaviour research. Br. J. Nutr. 2012, 107, 1553–1560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Kirkwood, L.; Aldujaili, E.; Drummond, S. Effects of advice on dietary intake and/or physical activity on body composition, blood lipids and insulin resistance following a low-fat, sucrose-containing, high-carbohydrate, energy-restricted diet. Int. J. Food Sci. Nutr. 2007, 58, 383–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Ethische Richtlinien der DGPs und des BDP. Available online: http://www.dgps.de/index.php?id=96422 (accessed on 9 May 2014).
  26. Bucher, T.; van der Horst, K.; Siegrist, M. Fruit for dessert. How people compose healthier meals. Appetite 2013, 60, 74–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Gesellschaft für optimierte Ernährung. Optidiet Basic. Available online: http://www.goe-software.de/WordPress/?page_id =1278 (accessed on 21 April 2015).
  28. Stehle, P.; Oberritter, H.; Büning-Fesel, M.; Heseker, H. Grafische Umsetzung von Ernährungsrichtlinien-traditionelle und neue Ansätze. Ernährungs-Umschau 2005, 52, 128–135. [Google Scholar]
  29. Oberritter, H.; Schäbethal, K.; von Ruesten, A.; Boeing, H. The DGE nutrition circle—Presentation and basis of the food-related recommendations from the German Nutrition Society (DGE). Ernährungs-Umschau Int. 2013, 2, 24–29. [Google Scholar]
  30. Curran, P.J.; West, S.G.; Finch, J.F. The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychol. Methods 1996, 1, 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Coppinger, T.; Jeanes, Y.M.; Dabinett, J.; Vogele, C.; Reeves, S. Physical activity and dietary intake of children aged 9–11 years and the influence of peers on these behaviours: A 1-year follow-up. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010, 64, 776–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Krebs-Smith, S.M.; Guenther, P.M.; Subar, A.F.; Kirkpatrick, S.I.; Dodd, K.W. Americans do not meet federal dietary recommendations. J. Nutr. 2010, 140, 1832–1838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. De Graaf, C. Effects of snacks on energy intake: An evolutionary perspective. Appetite 2006, 47, 18–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Wegner, D.M. Ironic processes of mental control. Psychol. Rev. 1994, 101, 34–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Adriaanse, M.A.; van Oosten, J.M.; de Ridder, D.T.; de Wit, J.B.; Evers, C. Planning what not to eat: Ironic effects of implementation intentions negating unhealthy habits. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 37, 69–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Lake, A.A.; Hyland, R.M.; Rugg-Gunn, A.J.; Wood, C.E.; Mathers, J.C.; Adamson, A.J. Healthy eating: Perceptions and practice (the ash 30 study). Appetite 2007, 48, 176–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Lynch, E.B.; Holmes, S.; Keim, K.; Koneman, S.A. Concepts of healthful food among low-income African American women. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2012, 44, 154–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Margetts, B.M.; Martinez, J.A.; Saba, A.; Holm, L.; Kearney, M. Definitions of “healthy” eating: A pan-EU survey of consumer attitudes to food, nutrition and health. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 1997, 51, 23–29. [Google Scholar]
  39. Paquette, M.C. Perceptions of healthy eating—State of knowledge and research gaps. Can. J. Public Health 2005, 96, 15–19. [Google Scholar]
  40. Oakes, M.E.; Slotterback, C.S. Manipulations that influence stereotypes of the weight-gain potential of foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2007, 18, 548–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rothman, A.J.; Klein, W.M.; Weinstein, N.D. Absolute and relative biases in estimations of personal risk. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 26, 1213–1236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Patel, K.A.; Schlundt, D. Impact of moods and social context on eating behavior. Appetite 2001, 36, 111–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Armor, D.A.; Taylor, S.E. Situated optimism: Specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 30, 309–379. [Google Scholar]
  44. Harris, P.R.; Griffin, D.W.; Murray, S. Testing the limits of optimistic bias: Event and person moderators in a multilevel framework. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 1225–1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Statistisches Bundesamt. Available online: https://www.gbe-bund.de/stichworte/BMI.html (accessed on 11 May 2015).
  46. USDA Choosemyplate. Available online: http://www.choosemyplate.gov/index.html (accessed on 15 April 2015).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Sproesser, G.; Kohlbrenner, V.; Schupp, H.; Renner, B. I Eat Healthier Than You: Differences in Healthy and Unhealthy Food Choices for Oneself and for Others. Nutrients 2015, 7, 4638-4660. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7064638

AMA Style

Sproesser G, Kohlbrenner V, Schupp H, Renner B. I Eat Healthier Than You: Differences in Healthy and Unhealthy Food Choices for Oneself and for Others. Nutrients. 2015; 7(6):4638-4660. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7064638

Chicago/Turabian Style

Sproesser, Gudrun, Verena Kohlbrenner, Harald Schupp, and Britta Renner. 2015. "I Eat Healthier Than You: Differences in Healthy and Unhealthy Food Choices for Oneself and for Others" Nutrients 7, no. 6: 4638-4660. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7064638

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop