Next Article in Journal
A Comparison of Combustion Properties in Biomass–Coal Blends Using Characteristic and Kinetic Analyses
Next Article in Special Issue
Pain Control: Normalization of the BPCQ Questionnaire on a Group of Patients Diagnosed with Malignant Cancer
Previous Article in Journal
Which Non-Pharmaceutical Primary Care Interventions Reduce Inequalities in Common Mental Health Disorders? A Protocol for a Systematic Review of Quantitative and Qualitative Studies
Previous Article in Special Issue
Factors Influencing Self-Care Behavior and Treatment Adherence in Hemodialysis Patients
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Regression-Based Normative Data for Independent and Cognitively Active Spanish Older Adults: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and Judgement of Line Orientation

by
Eva Calderón-Rubio
1,
Javier Oltra-Cucarella
1,2,*,
Beatriz Bonete-López
1,2,
Clara Iñesta
1 and
Esther Sitges-Maciá
1,2
1
SABIEX, Universidad Miguel Hernández de Elche, Avda. de la Universidad, 03207 Elche, Spain
2
Department of Health Psychology, Miguel Hernández University of Elche, 03202 Elche, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(24), 12977; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182412977
Submission received: 13 October 2021 / Revised: 2 December 2021 / Accepted: 7 December 2021 / Published: 9 December 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Diagnosis and Advances in Research on Human Behavior)

Abstract

:
The aim of this work was to develop normative data for neuropsychological tests for the assessment of independent and cognitively active Spanish older adults over 55 years of age. Methods: regression-based normative data were calculated from a sample of 103 nondepressed independent community-dwelling adults aged 55 or older (66% women). The raw data for the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT), the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF) and the Judgement of Line Orientation Test (JLO) were regressed on age, sex and education. The model predicting the FCSRT delayed-recall (FCSRT-Del) scores also included the FCSRT immediate-recall (FCSRT-Imm) scores. The model predicting the ROCF immediate-recall (ROCF-Imm) scores included the ROCF copy-trial (ROCF-C) scores, and the model predicting the ROCF delayed-recall (ROCF-Del) scores included both the ROCF-C and the ROCF-Imm scores. In order to identify low scores, z-scores were used to determine the discrepancy between the observed and the predicted scores. The base rates of the low scores for both the SABIEX normative data and the published normative data obtained from the general population were compared. Results: the effects of the different sociodemographic variables (age, sex and education) varied throughout the neuropsychological measures. Despite finding similar proportions of low scores between the normative data sets, the agreement was irrelevant or only fair-to-good. Conclusions: the normative data obtained from the general population might not be sensitive enough to identify low scores in cognitively active older adults, incorrectly classifying them as cognitively normal compared to the less active population.

1. Introduction

The United Nations [1] predicts that, by 2050, the global population of older people will be growing at a rate of 2.6% per year. It is expected that 30% and 6% of the population in developed countries will be aged 60 or older and 80 or older, respectively. In Spain, the number of people aged 55 years or older is around 15 million, which represents 33.08% of the overall population. Amongst these, 18.22% (2,856,102) are 80 years or older [2].
The increase in the population’s age span is related to a raise in the probability of pathological ageing, such as cognitive impairment or dementia [3,4]. Indeed, age is the main risk factor for cognitive impairment [5,6] and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), with the prevalence of AD increasing from age 60 to 89 years [7]. This situation has an important impact on both public health and sanitary and social services [8]. On the one hand, cognitive impairment is a frequent cause of consultation in primary health care, with an estimated prevalence of 15–20% in individuals older than 60 years [9]. In Spain in particular, the prevalence of cognitive impairment is 18.5% in people older than 65 years, and 45.3% in people older than 85 years [8]. On the other hand, the overall prevalence of dementia in the Spanish population is between 4% and 9% in groups older than 65 years [10]. Both cognitive impairment and dementia are related to higher comorbidity [11], susceptibility to infections [12] or depressive symptoms [13]. Furthermore, dementia has the highest total expenditure in neurological diseases [14] and requires about 3 h per day of care in both the basic and instrumental activities of daily life (ADL), becoming a huge burden for the caregivers of a family member with dementia [15].
A high percentage of individuals may not have a formal diagnosis of cognitive impairment or dementia: 55% in Spain [6], 58% in Europe [16] and 65% in the USA [17]. This situation could be due to the patients’ and relatives’ lack of consciousness about the presence of the disease [10], the lack of training among professionals in primary health care or the tight agendas in specialist settings [17]. For this reason, it is necessary to perform a thorough clinical study to overcome these problems. In order to detect subtle deficits or characterise cognitive strengths and weaknesses, an important part of this clinical study would be the neuropsychological assessment [18,19].
A neuropsychological assessment is based on the administration of standardised neurocognitive tests, in order to analyse the cognitive changes after brain damage [18,19]. It is essential to have reliable and suitable normative values, accounting for the effect of sociodemographic and cultural variables, in order to reduce the risk of misdiagnosis [20,21,22], determine the level of performance in the neuropsychological tests [23] and interpret the results obtained by a given subject by contrasting them with the performance of the reference group [18,24]. Normative data can be developed following three different strategies: mean and standard deviation scores, multiple regression and ROC curves. The first strategy is based on determining how the values are distributed and what their associated theoretical or empirical frequencies are [25,26,27]. The second is based on the generation of a prediction model accounting for age, sex and/or education. This model will be used to estimate a score, given certain variables (age, sex and education), that will be compared with the observed data [28,29,30]. ROC curves provide a cut-off value based on sensitivity and specificity [31].
Nevertheless, normative data obtained from the general population might not take into account the characteristics of active ageing. Active ageing is a process in which the opportunities for health, participation and security are optimised in order to enhance the quality of life during ageing [32]. This model encompasses six groups of determinants: behavioural styles, personal biological and psychological conditions, health and social services, physical engagement and social and economic factors. It has been previously reported that engaging in leisure activities is a relevant factor for a well-ageing process [33]. Indeed, most successfully ageing people, characterised by their health and independence, differ from the general population in the number of leisure activities they participate in [34]. Furthermore, cognitively stimulating activities may prevent cognitive decline during ageing [35].
Although educational level may play an important role in active ageing (for instance, [36]), the variable regarded as a determinant factor in the WHO model (WHO, 2002) is not early education, but lifelong learning [37]. In this sense, education is mostly taken into account as a long-term determinant [38]. Hijas-Gómez et al. [37] found that only the physical component was associated with survival and correlated with cognitive status, lifestyle and lifelong learning. For this reason, it is of great importance to ensure equal access opportunities for learning throughout the lifespan [39]. Thus, as active ageing is independent of the educational level, using normative data that account for educational level but do not comprehensively analyse the active ageing model could lead to a higher risk of misdiagnosis.
Two important cognitive domains assessed by neuropsychologists are memory and visuospatial perception [18,24]. In this work, memory was studied with the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT; [40,41,42]) and the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) [43,44]. Visuospatial perception was analysed through the ROCF and the Judgement of Line Orientation (JLO; [45,46,47]) tests.
The FCSRT was initially designed as a selective reminding memory test [40,41], and then a cued-recall trial was added [42]. The ROCF measures the visual perception and visuospatial construction ability by means of the copy trial, and visual memory through the immediate- and delayed-recall tasks [43,44]. For both the FCSRT and ROCF, normative data in a healthy Spanish population have been developed by the NEURONORMA project [48,49]. The JLO analyses spatial perception and spatial orientation [46]. Normative data in a healthy Spanish population for JLO have also been developed by the NEURONORMA project [50].
Since cognitively active people seem to outperform nonactive people in neuropsychological tests, it is likely that the use of normative data that only account for educational level may increase the number of misdiagnoses [51]. For this reason, the objective of this work is to develop normative data for a cognitively active elderly population. We hypothesise that, accounting for the characteristics of the active ageing population, the low scores obtained with the normative data based on cognitively active people (SABIEX) will differ from those of the normative data obtained from the general population (NEURONORMA). Our hypothesis is that the active ageing population will show a higher percentage of low scores in the SABIEX normative data.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This was a cross-sectional observational study with cognitively healthy individuals living independently in the community. One hundred and five (105) students from the Seniors’ University (SABIEX) of the Universidad Miguel Hernández de Elche (UMH, Spain) were recruited from October 2019 to July 2021. SABIEX is an academic university programme for people aged 55 years or older covering subjects such as economics, psychology, politics and the arts.
The features of the sample have been previously described [52]. Inclusion criteria were (a) being 55 years old or older, (b) being cognitively normal (CN) without subjective cognitive complaints and (c) living independently in the community. Cognitive normality was determined by (a) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; [53]) scores higher than 23, (b) Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR; [54]) scores equal to 0 and (c) Lawton–Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL; [55]) scores equal to 7 or higher. Exclusion criteria were (a) unwillingness to participate in the neuropsychological assessment and (b) presence of vision and/or hearing impairments that might have interfered with the administration of cognitive tests. Having past or current medical conditions (e.g., cancer, psychiatric disorders or metabolic disease) was not defined as an exclusion criterion, so that the representativeness of the sample was guaranteed. All participants were born and raised in Spain and had Spanish as their mother tongue.

2.2. Materials

The individual neuropsychological assessment spanned around 90 min and covered different cognitive domains. In this paper, data are reported for tests assessing memory and visuospatial perception: the FCSRT [40,41], the ROCF [43,44] and the JLO [45,46,47] tests. Descriptive statistics for the remaining tests can be found in Bonete-López et al. [52].

2.2.1. Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test

The FCSRT is a word-list learning test administered through three immediate free and cued recall tasks and a 30 min delayed free and cued recall task. The FCSRT was administered according to NEURONORMA instructions [49]. Participants were shown 4 different sheets, all of them containing 4 bold written words. Each word pertained to a different semantic category. The four sheets were presented individually and in sequence. Subjects were asked to read each of the four words aloud and match each of them with the semantic clue given by the examiner. After the 16 items were correctly identified, a nonsemantic interference task (counting backwards by three) was carried out for 20 s. After the interference task, the participants were asked to freely recall the 16 words in any order. Ninety seconds were given as the maximum time to complete the task. The semantic category was given (cued recall) for the words not recalled in the free-recall task. This task was followed by two more trials, each of them separated by a nonsemantic interference task (counting backwards by three). In the first two trials, participants were given the correct items for words not recalled with the semantic cue. After 30 min, the delayed-recall trial was performed. If any word was not remembered, the task was followed by the cued-recall trial. Lastly, 40 words were read out loud by the examiner [52]. Of them, 16 were words from the previous free- and cued-recall tasks (targets) and 24 were distractors. Subjects were asked to identify the targets by saying “yes” and the distractors by saying “no”. In this paper, the study variables were the total immediate- and total delayed-recall scores (FCSRT-Imm and FCSRT-Del, respectively) with a maximum score of 48 and 16, respectively.

2.2.2. Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure

The ROCF test consisted of 18 elements, each of which could be scored 0.5, 1 or 2 points depending on their accuracy and location [24]. The ROCF test included three trials: (a) a copy of the figure model (ROCF-C), (b) an immediate-recall task (ROCF-Imm) and (c) a delayed-recall task after 30 min (ROCF-Del). For each trial, a blank sheet of paper was placed horizontally in front of the participants. In the first trial, participants were asked to copy the figure, trying not to leave any element undrawn and to respect the details and dimensions of the model. They were allowed to rotate their piece of paper, but not the model, which was just shown in the copy trial. For both the immediate- and delayed-recall trials, subjects were asked to draw as many items as they could remember. If they could not remember the location of the element, they were requested to draw it wherever they desired.
The study variables were: (a) copy—overall sum of correctly drawn elements when copying the model; (b) immediate recall—total sum of correctly drawn items after 3 min; and (d) delayed recall—total sum of correctly drawn items after 30 min. The maximum score was 36 for each task.

2.2.3. Judgement of Line Orientation

The JLO test consisted of five practice items and 30 test items. Each element was formed by two unnumbered segments and 11 numbered lines forming a semicircle as a model. The task was based on visually matching both test lines with two of the semicircle’s segments in each trial. The five practice tests were initially presented, and then the task continued with the performance of the test items. The total score was given by the number of elements in which both lines were correctly identified.

2.3. Procedure

The participants were invited to voluntarily participate in the study. No credit for their courses were given. The participants were individually assessed by a board-certified neuropsychologist and trained undergraduate, master’s degree-level or PhD-level students. An informed consent form was signed by every subject prior to enrolment. All participants provided personal and familial medical information. This project was approved by the UMH Ethics Committee (DPS.ESM.01.19).

2.4. Statistical Analysis

2.4.1. Calculation of Normative Data

We used the methodology reported by Iñesta et al. [56]. Briefly, a linear regression model was built to predict neuropsychological test scores using age, sex and education. To test for possible nonlinear associations, we centered age and education using the lowest value in the distribution of data, which are referred to as AgeMin and EducationMin throughout the manuscript, and calculated the quadratic term. We used a two-step linear regression model with raw values introduced in the first step and the quadratic terms introduced in the second step.
To improve the interpretation of performance on neuropsychological tasks conditional on other, previous neuropsychological tasks, we calculated normative data for the ROCF-Imm and ROCF-Del tests with a regression model adding ROCF-C scores to demographic variables, which are referred to as ROCF-ImmSABIEX and ROCF-DelSABIEX. In the regression model for the FCSRT-Del test, we added FCSRT-Imm scores to the demographic variables. Each variable was centered using the lowest value in the distribution of data.

2.4.2. Comparing Normative Data Sets

The number of low scores shown by our sample when using either the SABIEX or the NEURONORMA normative data was analyzed with the McNemar test (corrected for continuity) for related proportions [57], and the Fleiss’ kappa [57] interrater correlation coefficient for categorical data was used to analyse the level of agreement between normative data sets. According to Fleiss et al. [57], agreement beyond chance can be interpreted as poor, fair to good and excellent for values of 0–0.40, 0.41–0.75 and >0.75, respectively. Low scores were defined as scaled scores equal to or lower than 6 using NEURONORMA, and as z-scores equal to or lower than −1.28 using SABIEX normative data [56]. Using z-scores equal to or lower than −1.28 guarantees that the true positive rate and true negative rate from a linear regression are close to the 95% for a sample of size n = 100 or larger [58].

3. Results

From a pool of 105 subjects (33% males), two subjects were not included because of MMSE scores <24. Descriptive statistics for the demographic variables and the MMSE, IADL and GSD scores of the 103 participants are reported in Iñesta et al. [56]. A descriptive analysis of the complete test battery can be found in Bonete-López et al. [52].
The differences in age between the sexes were statistically significant (t (df = 101) = 3.06; p = 0.004). Specifically, the men were, on average, four years older than the women (men: M = 68.47, SD = 6.51; women: M = 64.42, SD = 6.22). No statistically significant differences between the sexes were found in the years of education (t (df = 101) = 0.551; p = 0.583), MMSE (t (df = 101) = −1.59; p = 0.114), IADL (t (df = 101) = 0.70; p = 0.485) and GDS (t (df = 101) = −1.18; p = 0.240).
The descriptive statistics for the performance in the different tasks of each neuropsychological test is provided in Table 1.

3.1. Calculation of Normative Data

The demographic variables (age, sex and education) showed different effects on each neuropsychological test. However, as no relation was found between the demographic variables and any of the tasks of the ROCF test, normative data were calculated with the means and standard deviations.
The multiple linear regression models of the rest of the neuropsychological measures are shown in Table 2. Sex was significantly related to the FCSRT-Imm, FCSRT-Del and JLO scores. EducationMin was associated with the FCSRT-Imm, FCSRT-Del and FCSRT-DelSABIEX scores. EducationMin2 had an effect on the FCSRT-Del and JLO scores. No statistically significant effects of AgeMin and AgeMin2 were found. Particularly interesting for this work are the significant associations within the dependent tasks, as the FCSRT-Del scores were related to the FCSRT-ImmMin scores, the ROCF-Imm scores to the ROCF-CMin scores and the ROCF-Del scores to the ROCF-ImmMin scores.

3.2. Comparing Normative Data Sets

The frequency and cumulative percentage of the low scores for the three normative data sets (NEURONORMA, SABIEXINDEP and SABIEXDEP) are provided in Table 3 and Figure 1.
No statistically significant differences were found between the NEURONORMA and SABIEXINDEP data sets in the percentage of participants with one or more low scores (χ2 (N = 103) = 0.842; p = 0.358). The Fleiss’ kappa coefficient showed only a fair-to-good agreement for both data sets when identifying the participants showing one or more low scores (κ = 0.586; p < 0.001).
Conversely, statistically significant differences were found in the proportion of participants showing one or more low scores between the NEURONORMA and the SABIEXDEP data sets (χ2 (N = 103) = 6.50; p = 0.011). Furthermore, although the Fleiss’ kappa coefficient was statistically significant, the agreement between the normative data sets in identifying the low scores was only fair-to-good (κ = 0.464; p < 0.001).

3.3. Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test

Using the SABIEX normative data, 8.74% of the sample had at least one low score, whereas for the NEURONORMA data, 1.94% of the sample (two subjects) obtained one or more low scores. The McNemar test was statistically significant (χ2 (N = 103) = 5.143; p = 0.023), meaning that the proportions of low scores were not similar in both data sets. The Fleiss’ kappa coefficient showed a lack of agreement in identifying the low scores (κ = 0.328; p = 0.001) (see Table S1).
We also performed separate analyses of the proportion of low scores for the neuropsychological measures considered dependent from the others and compared them to the NEURONORMA data set. First, we compared both the FCSRT-Del and FCSRT-DelSABIEX scores with the data from NEURONORMA. In both the FCSRT-Del and FCSRT-DelSABIEX data, 11.65% of the sample (12 subjects) had at least one low score, in contrast to the 0.97% (one subject) from NEURONORMA. The McNemar test showed statistically significant differences in the proportion of both the SABIEX and NEURONORMA data sets (χ2 (N = 103) = 9.091; p = 0.002) and no agreement was found in identifying the low scores (κ = 0.097; p = 0.326) (see Tables S2 and S3).

3.4. Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure

The ROCF-C scores were not associated statistically with any of the variables analysed (see Table 2). Regarding the ROCF-Imm and ROCF-ImmSABIEX scores, at least one low score was obtained by 10.68% of the sample (11 subjects) in the SABIEXINDEP, 8.74% (9 subjects) in the SABIEXDEP and 6.80% (7 subjects) in the NEURONORMA data set. The McNemar test showed no statistically significant differences when comparing the NEURONORMA data with either the SABIEXINDEP (χ2 (N = 103) = 1.500; p = 0.221) or the SABIEXDEP (χ2 (N = 103) = 0.125; p = 0.7237) data. The Fleiss’ kappa coefficient showed fair-to-good agreement between the NEURONORMA data and both of the SABIEX data sets (SABIEXINDEP: κ = 0.635; p = 0.000; SABIEXDEP: κ = 0.458; p < 0.001) (see Tables S4 and S5).
Regarding the ROCF-Del and ROCF-DelSABIEX scores, at least one low score was obtained by 12.62% of the sample (13 subjects) in the SABIEXINDEP, 10.68% (11 subjects) in the SABIEXDEP and 9.71% (10 subjects) in the NEURONORMA data set. The McNemar test showed no statistically significant differences when comparing the NEURONORMA data with either the SABIEXINDEP (χ2 (N = 103) = 0.800; p = 0.371) or the SABIEXDEP (χ2 (N = 103) = 0.000; p > 0.999) data sets. However, the Fleiss’ kappa coefficient was only statistically significant when comparing the NEURONORMA data with the SABIEXINDEP data (κ = 0.755; p = 0.000), with a lack of agreement in identifying the participants as showing one or more low scores between the SABIEXDEP and NEURONORMA data sets (κ = −0.114; p = 0.249) (see Tables S6 and S7).

3.5. Judgement of Line Orientation

The JLO scores were negatively associated with the sex variable and positively associated with the EducationMIN2 variable, meaning that the effect of education was not linear and that better scores are expected when the level of education increases.

4. Discussion

The objective of this study was to develop normative data for Spanish cognitively active individuals aged 55 or older. For this aim, regression analyses were performed for every variable studied. Two novelties characterise this work: the use of a cognitively active sample and the analysis of every subtest as both dependent (SABIEXDEP) and independent (SABIEXINDEP) of other variables from the same test. Our results showed only a fair-to-good agreement in identifying the low scores between both the SABIEX normative data and the NEURONORMA normative data, with no agreement for the delayed-recall trial from the ROCF test.
No effect of age was found in any of the tests assessed, which differs from previous studies suggesting that neuropsychological performance declines with advancing age [49,59,60,61,62]. This result may represent a novelty in this area, as it may show that age becomes less relevant when individuals are cognitively active. This may also evidence the importance of creating a cognitive reserve [63] throughout the lifespan, independently of the level of education achieved during the early life stages.
Regarding the FCSRT, sex had a significant effect on both the FCSRT-Imm and FCSRT-Del trials, in line with previous studies suggesting that women outperform men on tests based on verbal material such as the FCSRT [64,65,66]. Other studies also showed a significant effect of sex in the free-recall tasks of the FCSRT [67,68]. However, its effect may be minor or irrelevant [49,69,70] and may be influenced by the different proportion of men–women in our study. The effect of sex became non-significant when we analysed the FCSRT-Del scores while controlling for the FCSRT-Imm scores (FCSRT-DelSABIEX). Education also showed a significant effect on the FCSRT-Imm, FCSRT-Del and FCSRT-DelSABIEX scores, in line with previous studies [49,66,67,68,69,70]. This result may illustrate the effect of education on neuropsychological performances and its association with cognitive reserve [61,69,70,71]. Quadratic education had an impact only on the FCSRT-Del scores, meaning that the effect of education was not linear and that the differences in memory performance were more significant amongst less educated subjects than between highly educated individuals, which is in line with previous works [72,73,74,75]. This effect disappeared when the FCSRT-Imm score was also controlled.
Regarding the ROCF test, no significant effect of any of the sociodemographic variables was found. In previous studies, minor or nonexistent effects have been reported [49,76,77,78], which is in line with our results. However, most of the scientific research shows that education has a positive impact when performing ROCF tasks, as higher education levels are usually associated with better scores [49,78,79,80]. Nevertheless, our results may indicate that, independently of the level of education, when a cognitively active lifestyle is carried out, cognitive strategies can compensate for these differences [81], as suggested by the cognitive reserve construct [63,82]. Lastly, regarding the JLO test, sex and quadratic education showed a significant effect, which is in line with previous studies [83,84,85].
Another relevant aspect of our research was the analysis of the different subtests as both independent of and dependent on the rest of the trials of the same test. In line with previous results [78], the ROCF-C scores had a positive effect on the ROCF-Imm scores and the ROCF-Imm on the ROCF-Del, and the FCSRT-Imm scores had a significant positive effect on the FCSRT-Del scores. When comparing the SABIEXINDEP and NEURONORMA data sets, we found similar proportions of low scores, but there was only a fair-to-good agreement in identifying the low scores. On the other hand, when comparing the SABIEXDEP and NEURONORMA data sets, we found a statistically different proportion of low scores and only a fair-to-good agreement, which was even lower than that of the SABIEXINDEP data set. This may have been due to the distinctive features characterising our sample: the NEURONORMA data was based on the general population, while the SABIEX sample included highly cognitively active participants. This may suggest that using normative data that do not take into account the specificities of the active ageing population may increase the number of diagnosis errors and, hence, the misclassification of subjects as cognitively impaired if low scores are to be used to diagnose cognitive impairment. Additionally, not controlling the effect of the related subtests when creating new normative data sets, as is the usual practice [49,79,80,84], may also be associated with an increased rate of false positives and/or negatives and with the misclassification of subjects. Further research is needed to confirm the clinical applicability of our results, analysing whether the use of SABIEX normative data is useful to identify with greater certainty individuals with a greater risk of cognitive decline. Lastly, when contrasting the neuropsychological measures separately, no agreement (FCSRT-Imm, FCSRT-Del, FCSRT-DelSABIEX, ROCF-DelSABIEX) or only a fair-to-good agreement (ROCF-Imm, ROCF-ImmSABIEX, ROCF-Del) was found in identifying low scores. This supports the idea that normative data accounting for the characteristics of cognitively active elders are needed to avoid the appearance of false negatives and/or false positives and, consequently, their misclassification.
These results may have important clinical implications. For example, objective cognitive impairment (i.e., one or more low scores) is necessary for the diagnosis of MCI [86,87]. Our results are important whether classic [86,88] or modified [89,90,91] diagnostic criteria are to be used to identify MCI. If only one test is used to identify memory impairments, as with the classic criteria, the selection of the appropriate normative sample against which the raw scores are to be compared may reduce the number of false positives. If several tests are used to identify MCI, as with the modified diagnostic criteria, the association between several tasks within the same test might identify with greater certainty individuals with true cognitive impairment. For example, if two low scores on the memory task are needed to meet the criterion for the comprehensive diagnostic criteria [89,92], using the delayed-recall scores as independent of the immediate-recall scores might be associated with a higher number of false positives, because the correlation between the tests is not taken into account when independent normative data are used. Relatedly, analysing the performance on some tests that are conditional on other tests might impact the number of low scores needed to define normal variability and, subsequently, true cognitive impairment [90,91]. Using the worst-performing 10% of the sample, two or more low scores would be needed to meet the criterion for objective cognitive impairment using the NEURONORMA normative data, compared to three or more low scores using the SABIEX normative data (Table 3). Additionally, since low scores on visual memory tasks have been associated with a similar risk of progression from MCI to AD compared to low scores on verbal memory tasks [93], using the normative data for the delayed-recall trial in the ROCF test conditional on the copy trial may help clinicians to better differentiate true impairments in visual memory from low scores on the immediate or delayed trials that are conditional on low scores on the copy trial. If low scores on the delayed-recall task are associated with low scores on the copy trial, the individual would be diagnosed as having nonamnestic MCI rather than as having amnestic MCI, with the former being associated with a lower risk of progression from MCI to AD than the latter [94]. We provide a friendly calculator for researchers and clinicians that is available at https://docs.google.com/spreadsheets/d/1p-RDT6F85EsXPxALV-l6R8Sq-E4qGEr0/edit?usp=sharing&ouid=109893127231470805500&rtpof=true&sd=true (accessed on 8 December 2021).
This work has certain limitations. First, we used an incidental sampling method, which was related to the overrepresentation of people aged 60–70 and the underrepresentation of people aged over 75 years. It was also associated with the overrepresentation of women. As gender differences in cognitive domains have been previously suggested [95,96], these results should be interpreted with caution. The number of younger and older subjects, as well as the number of men, may be further investigated to improve the reliability and accuracy of the data set. Secondly, with our methodology, we cannot determine the clinical applicability of the SABIEX normative data. We can only suggest that individuals showing low scores might differ between the SABIEX and population-based normative data sets, but further research into the clinical population is needed to find out which is more accurate in identifying cognitive impairment in cognitively active older adults if low scores are to be used to identify cognitive impairment. Lastly, this work was only based on the Spanish population, so, due to the cultural differences in neuropsychological functions suggested by other authors [21,22,97], interpretations and application in other cultures or languages should be taken with caution.

5. Conclusions

Our results suggest that normative data obtained from the general population might not be sensitive enough to identify low scores in cognitively active older adults. Normative data accounting for the characteristics of cognitively active older adults might be necessary to reduce the number of diagnostic errors and, eventually, their misclassification as cognitively impaired compared to normative data obtained from the general and less cognitively active population.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph182412977/s1, Table S1: Comparing number of low scores on FCSRT-Imm between normative data sets (NEURONORMA and SABIEX), Table S2: Comparing number of low scores on FCSRT-Del between normative data sets (NEURONORMA and SABIEXINDEP), Table S3: Comparing number of low scores on FCSRT-Del independent (NEURONORMA) with FCSRT-Del conditional on FCSRT-Imm (FCSRT-DelSABIEX), Table S4: Comparing number of low scores on ROCF-Imm between normative data sets (NEURONORMA and SABIEXINDEP), Table S5: Comparing number of low scores on ROCF-Imm independent (NEURONORMA) with ROCF-Imm conditional on ROCF-C (ROCF-ImmSABIEX), Table S6: Comparing number of low scores on ROCF-Del between normative data sets (NEURONORMA and SABIEXINDEP), Table S7: Comparing number of low scores on ROCF-Del independent (NEURONORMA) with ROCF-Del conditional on ROCF-Imm (ROCF-DelSABIEX).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.O.-C., E.S.-M. and B.B.-L.; methodology, J.O.-C., E.C.-R. and C.I.; formal analysis, E.C.-R. and J.O.-C.; investigation, E.C.-R., J.O.-C., B.B.-L., C.I. and E.S.-M.; data curation, E.C.-R. and J.O.-C.; writing—original draft preparation, E.C.-R. and J.O.-C.; writing—review and editing, E.C.-R., J.O.-C., B.B.-L., C.I. and E.S.-M.; supervision, J.O.-C., B.B.-L. and E.S.-M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was partially funded by the Conselleria d’Innovació, Universitats, Ciència i Societat Digital, Generalitat Valenciana (NEUROPREVENT project, GV/2021/139).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Miguel Hernández University (protocol code DPS.ESM.01.19).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. United Nations. World Population Ageing 2009; United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  2. Instituto Nacional de Estadística Proporción de Personas Mayores de Cierta Edad Por Comunidad Autónoma. Available online: https://www.ine.es/jaxiT3/Datos.htm?t=1451#!tabs-tabla (accessed on 13 March 2021).
  3. De Pedro-Cuesta, J.; Virués-Ortega, J.; Vega, S.; Seijo-Martínez, M.; Saz, P.; Rodríguez, F.; Rodríguez-Laso, A.; Reñé, R.; de las Heras, S.P.; Mateos, R.; et al. Prevalence of Dementia and Major Dementia Subtypes in Spanish Populations: A Reanalysis of Dementia Prevalence Surveys, 1990–2008. BMC Neurol. 2009, 9, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. De Ronchi, D.; Berardi, D.; Menchetti, M.; Ferrari, G.; Serretti, A.; Dalmonte, E.; Fratiglioni, L. Occurrence of Cognitive Impairment and Dementia after the Age of 60: A Population-Based Study from Northern Italy. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2005, 19, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Rodríguez-Sánchez, E.; Mora-Simón, S.; Patino-Alonso, M.C.; García-García, R.; Escribano-Hernández, A.; García-Ortiz, L.; Perea- Bartolomé, M.V.; Gómez-Marcos, M.A. Prevalence of Cognitive Impairment in Individuals Aged over 65 in an Urban Area: DERIVA Study. BMC Neurol. 2011, 11, 147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Tola-Arribas, M.A.; Yugueros, M.I.; Garea, M.J.; Ortega-Valín, F.; Cerón-Fernández, A.; Fernández-Malvido, B.; San José-Gallegos, A.; González-Touya, M.; Botrán-Velicia, A.; Iglesias-Rodríguez, V.; et al. Prevalence of Dementia and Subtypes in Valladolid, Northwestern Spain: The DEMINVALL Study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e77688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Jellinger, K.A.; Attems, J. Prevalence of Dementia Disorders in the Oldest-Old: An Autopsy Study. Acta Neuropathol. 2010, 119, 421–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Vega Alonso, T.; Miralles Espí, M.; Mangas Reina, J.M.; Castrillejo Pérez, D.; Rivas Pérez, A.I.; Gil Costa, M.; López Maside, A.; Arrieta Antón, E.; Lozano Alonso, J.E.; Fragua Gil, M. Prevalencia de deterioro cognitivo en España. Estudio Gómez de Caso en redes centinelas sanitarias. Neurología 2018, 33, 491–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Petersen, R.C. Mild Cognitive Impairment. Contin. Minneap. Minn. 2016, 22, 404–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Villarejo Galende, A.; Eimil Ortiz, M.; Llamas Velasco, S.; Llanero Luque, M.; López de Silanes de Miguel, C.; Prieto Jurczynska, C. Informe de la Fundación del Cerebro. Impacto social de la enfermedad de Alzheimer y otras demencias. Neurología 2021, 36, 39–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Poblador-Plou, B.; Calderón-Larrañaga, A.; Marta-Moreno, J.; Hancco-Saavedra, J.; Sicras-Mainar, A.; Soljak, M.; Prados-Torres, A. Comorbidity of Dementia: A Cross-Sectional Study of Primary Care Older Patients. BMC Psychiatry 2014, 14, 84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Matusik, P.; Tomaszewski, K.; Chmielowska, K.; Nowak, J.; Nowak, W.; Parnicka, A.; Dubiel, M.; Gąsowski, J.; Grodzicki, T. Severe Frailty and Cognitive Impairment Are Related to Higher Mortality in 12-Month Follow-up of Nursing Home Residents. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2012, 55, 22–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Leyhe, T.; Reynolds, C.F.; Melcher, T.; Linnemann, C.; Klöppel, S.; Blennow, K.; Zetterberg, H.; Dubois, B.; Lista, S.; Hampel, H. A Common Challenge in Older Adults: Classification, Overlap, and Therapy of Depression and Dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2017, 13, 59–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Gustavsson, A.; Svensson, M.; Jacobi, F.; Allgulander, C.; Alonso, J.; Beghi, E.; Dodel, R.; Ekman, M.; Faravelli, C.; Fratiglioni, L.; et al. Cost of Disorders of the Brain in Europe 2010. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011, 21, 718–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Lin, C.-Y.; Shih, P.-Y.; Ku, L.-J.E. Activities of Daily Living Function and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of People with Dementia and Caregiver Burden: The Mediating Role of Caregiving Hours. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2019, 81, 25–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Eefsting, J.A.; Boersma, F.; Brink, W.V.D.; Tilburg, W.V. Differences in Prevalence of Dementia Based on Community Survey and General Practitioner Recognition. Psychol. Med. 1996, 26, 1223–1230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Valcour, V.G.; Masaki, K.H.; Curb, J.D.; Blanchette, P.L. The Detection of Dementia in the Primary Care Setting. Arch. Intern. Med. 2000, 160, 2964–2968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Lezak, M.D.; Howieson, D.B.; Bigler, E.D.; Tranel, D. Neuropsychological Assessment, 5th ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2012; ISBN 978-0-19-539552-5. [Google Scholar]
  19. Munro Collum, C.; Lacritz, L. Neuropsychological Assessment in Dementia. In The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Alzheimer Disease and Other Dementias; American Psychiatric Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  20. Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Ramos-Usuga, D.; Vergara-Moragues, E.; Montero-López, E.; Adana Díaz, L.A.; Aguayo Arelis, A.; García-Guerrero, C.E.; García De La Cadena, C.; Llerena Espezúa, X.; et al. Trail Making Test: Normative Data for the Latin American Spanish-Speaking Pediatric Population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 627–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Ardila, A. Cultural Values Underlying Psychometric Cognitive Testing. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2005, 15, 185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Ardila, A. The Impact of Culture on Neuropsychological Test Performance. In International Handbook of Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 23–44. [Google Scholar]
  23. Horton, A.M.; Roberts, C. Demographic Effects on the Trail Making Test in a Drug Abuse Treatment Sample. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. Off. J. Natl. Acad. Neuropsychol. 2003, 18, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.M.S.; Spreen, O.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2006; ISBN 978-0-19-515957-8. [Google Scholar]
  25. Hall, J.R.; Balldin, V.H.; Gamboa, A.; Edwards, M.L.; Johnson, L.A.; O’Bryant, S.E. Texas Mexican American Adult Normative Studies: Normative Data for the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS). Dev. Neuropsychol. 2018, 43, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. O’Bryant, S.E.; Edwards, M.; Johnson, L.; Hall, J.; Gamboa, A.; O’jile, J. Texas Mexican American Adult Normative Studies: Normative Data for Commonly Used Clinical Neuropsychological Measures for English- and Spanish-Speakers. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2018, 43, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Silvestre, G.; Iglesias, R.M.; Silvestre, E. Boston Naming Test Norms for the Dominican Population. Aphasiology 2018, 32, 340–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Heaton, A.; Gooding, A.; Cherner, M.; Umlauf, A.; Franklin, D.R.; Rivera Mindt, M.; Suárez, P.; Artiola i Fortuni, L.; Heaton, R.K.; Marquine, M.J. Demographically-Adjusted Norms for the Grooved Pegboard and Finger Tapping Tests in Spanish-Speaking Adults: Results from the Neuropsychological Norms for the U.S.-Mexico Border Region in Spanish (NP-NUMBRS) Project. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2021, 35, 419–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Peña-Casanova, J.; Blesa, R.; Aguilar, M.; Gramunt-Fombuena, N.; Gómez-Ansón, B.; Oliva, R.; Molinuevo, J.L.; Robles, A.; Barquero, M.S.; Antúnez, C.; et al. Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Methods and Sample Characteristics. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2009, 24, 307–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Rivera, D.; Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Van Der Elst, W.; Gonzalez, I.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Arelis, A.A.; Rodriguez-Irizarry, W.; De La Cadena, C.G.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C. Normative Data for Verbal Fluency in Healthy Latin American Adults: Letter M, and Fruits and Occupations Categories. Neuropsychology 2019, 33, 287–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Mandrekar, J.N. Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve in Diagnostic Test Assessment. J. Thorac. Oncol. 2010, 5, 1315–1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. WHO—World Health Organization. Active Ageing: A Policy Framework; WHO: Madrid, Spain, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  33. Lee, P.-L.; Lan, W.; Yen, T.-W. Aging Successfully: A Four-Factor Model. Educ. Gerontol. 2011, 37, 210–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Chaves, M.L.; Camozzato, A.L.; Eizirik, C.L.; Kaye, J. Predictors of Normal and Successful Aging Among Urban-Dwelling Elderly Brazilians. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2009, 64B, 597–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. George, C.; Ummar, S.A.; Shaji, K.S. Prevention of Cognitive Decline: Lifestyle and Other Issues. J. Geriatr. Ment. Health 2016, 3, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Arias-Merino, E.D.; Mendoza-Ruvalcaba, N.M.; Arias-Merino, M.J.; Cueva-Contreras, J.; Vazquez Arias, C. Prevalence of Successful Aging in the Elderly in Western Mexico. Curr. Gerontol. Geriatr. Res. 2012, 2012, 460249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Hijas-Gómez, A.I.; Ayala, A.; Rodríguez-García, M.P.; Rodríguez-Blázquez, C.; Rodríguez-Rodríguez, V.; Rojo-Pérez, F.; Fernández-Mayoralas, G.; Rodríguez-Laso, A.; Calderón-Larrañaga, A.; Forjaz, M.J. The WHO Active Ageing Pillars and Its Association with Survival: Findings from a Population-Based Study in Spain. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2020, 90, 104114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Fernández-Ballesteros, R.; Robine, J.M.; Walker, A.; Kalache, A. Active Aging: A Global Goal. Curr. Gerontol. Geriatr. Res. 2013, 2013, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Olivera, J.; Andreoli, F.; Leist, A.K.; Chauvel, L. Inequality in Old Age Cognition across the World. Econ. Hum. Biol. 2018, 29, 179–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Buschke, H.; Fuld, P.A. Evaluating Storage, Retention, and Retrieval in Disordered Memory and Learning. Neurology 1974, 24, 1019–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Buschke, H. Selective Reminding for Analysis of Memory and Learning. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1973, 12, 543–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Buschke, H. Cued Recall in Amnesia. J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1984, 6, 433–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Osterrieth, P.A. Le Test de Copie d’une Figure Complexe; Contribution à l’étude de La Perception et de La Mémoire. Arch. Psychol. 1944, 30, 206–356. [Google Scholar]
  44. Rey, A. L’examen Psychologique Dans Les Cas d’encéphalopathie Traumatique. (Les Problems.). Arch. Psychol. 1941, 28, 215–285. [Google Scholar]
  45. Benton, A.; Hannay, H.J.; Varney, N.R. Visual Perception of Line Direction in Patients with Unilateral Brain Disease. Neurology 1975, 25, 907–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Benton, A.; Sivan, A.B.; Hamsher, K.; Varney, N.R.; Spreen, O. Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  47. Benton, A.L.; Varney, N.R.; Hamsher, K.D. Visuospatial Judgment. A Clinical Test. Arch. Neurol. 1978, 35, 364–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Palomo, R.; Casals-Coll, M.; Sanchez-Benavides, G.; Quintana, M.; Manero, R.M.; Rognoni, T.; Calvo, L.; Aranciva, F.; Tamayo, F.; Pena-Casanova, J. Spanish Normative Studies in Young Adults (NEURONORMA Young Adults Project): Norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (Copy and Memory) and Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test. Neurol. Barc. Spain 2013, 28, 226–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Peña-Casanova, J.; Gramunt-Fombuena, N.; Quiñones-Ubeda, S.; Sanchez-Benavides, G.; Aguilar, M.; Badenes, D.; Molinuevo, J.L.; Robles, A.; Barquero, M.S.; Payno, M.; et al. Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (Copy and Memory), and Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2009, 24, 371–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Calvo, L.; Casals-Coll, M.; Sánchez-Benavides, G.; Quintana, M.; Manero, R.M.; Rognoni, T.; Palomo, R.; Aranciva, F.; Tamayo, F.; Peña-Casanova, J. Spanish Normative Studies in Young Adults (NEURONORMA Young Adults Project): Norms for the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery and Judgment of Line Orientation Tests. Neurol. Engl. Ed. 2013, 28, 153–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Narushima, M.; Liu, J.; Diestelkamp, N. Lifelong Learning in Active Ageing Discourse: Its Conserving Effect on Wellbeing, Health and Vulnerability. Ageing Soc. 2018, 38, 651–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Bonete López, B.; Oltra-Cucarella, J.; Marín, M.; Antón, C.; Balao, N.; López, E.; Sitges Maciá, E. Validation and Norms for a Recognition Task for the Spanish Version of the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2021, 36, 954–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. “Mini-Mental State”. A Practical Method for Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hughes, C.P.; Berg, L.; Danziger, W.; Coben, L.A.; Martin, R.L. A New Clinical Scale for the Staging of Dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 1982, 140, 566–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Lawton, M.; Brody, E. Assessment of Older People: Self-Maintaining and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living. Gerontologist 1969, 9, 179–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Iñesta, C.; Oltra-Cucarella, J.; Bonete-López, B.; Calderón-Rubio, E.; Sitges-Maciá, E. Regression-Based Normative Data for Independent and Cognitively Active Spanish Older Adults: Digit Span, Letters and Numbers, Trail Making Test and Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2021, 18, 9958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Fleiss, J.L.; Levin, B.; Paik, M.C. Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions, 3rd ed.; Wiley Series in Probability and Statistics; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2003; ISBN 978-0-471-52629-2. [Google Scholar]
  58. de Andrade Moral, R.; Diaz-Orueta, U.; Oltra-Cucarella, J. Logistic versus Linear Regression-Based Reliable Change Index: Implications for Clinical Studies with Diverse Sample Sizes. PsyArXiv 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Campo, P.; Morales, M. Normative Data and Reliability for a Spanish Version of the Verbal Selective Reminding Test. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2004, 19, 421–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Ivnik, R.J.; Smith, G.E.; Lucas, J.A.; Tangalos, E.G.; Kokmen, E.; Petersen, R.C. Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test: MOANS Norms. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1997, 19, 676–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Koss, E.; Haxby, J.V.; DeCarli, C.; Schapiro, M.B.; Friedland, R.P.; Rapoport, S.I. Patterns of Performance Preservation and Loss in Healthy Aging. Dev. Neuropsychol. 1991, 7, 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Tombaugh, T.N.; Faulkner, P.; Hubley, A.M. Effects of Age on the Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures: Test-Retest Data Using an Intentional Learning Paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1992, 14, 647–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Stern, Y. What Is Cognitive Reserve? Theory and Research Application of the Reserve Concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002, 8, 448–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Amieva, H.; Carcaillon, L.; Rouze L’Alzit-Schuermans, P.; Millet, X.; Dartigues, J.F.; Fabrigoule, C. Cued and uncued memory tests: Norms in elderly adults from the 3 Cities epidemiological study. Rev. Neurol. 2007, 163, 205–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Grober, E.; Buschke, H.; Crystal, H.; Bang, S.; Dresner, R. Screening for Dementia by Memory Testing. Neurology 1988, 38, 900–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Ouvrard, C.; Berr, C.; Meillon, C.; Ribet, C.; Goldberg, M.; Zins, M.; Amieva, H. Norms for Standard Neuropsychological Tests from the French CONSTANCES Cohort. Eur. J. Neurol. 2019, 26, 786–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Frasson, P.; Ghiretti, R.; Catricalà, E.; Pomati, S.; Marcone, A.; Parisi, L.; Rossini, P.M.; Cappa, S.F.; Mariani, C.; Vanacore, N.; et al. Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test: An Italian Normative Study. Neurol. Sci. 2011, 32, 1057–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Vogel, A.; Stokholm, J.; Andreasen, R.; Henriksen, B.D.; Brønniche, V.; Madsen, G.J.; Gustafsson, M.; Overgaard, S.; Guldberg, A.-M.; Jørgensen, K. Psychometric Properties and Reference Data for Danish Versions of Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, Category Cued Memory Test and Logical Memory. Scand. J. Psychol. 2018, 59, 496–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Girtler, N.; De Carli, F.; Amore, M.; Arnaldi, D.; Bosia, L.E.; Bruzzaniti, C.; Cappa, S.F.; Cocito, L.; Colazzo, G.; Ghio, L.; et al. A Normative Study of the Italian Printed Word Version of the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test. Neurol. Sci. 2015, 36, 1127–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Mokri, H.; Ávila-Funes, J.A.; Meillon, C.; Gutiérrez Robledo, L.M.; Amieva, H. Normative Data for the Mini-Mental State Examination, the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test and the Isaacs Set Test for an Older Adult Mexican Population: The Coyoacán Cohort Study. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2013, 27, 1004–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Hall, C.B.; Derby, C.; LeValley, A.; Katz, M.J.; Verghese, J.; Lipton, R.B. Education Delays Accelerated Decline on a Memory Test in Persons Who Develop Dementia. Neurology 2007, 69, 1657–1664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Le Carret, N.; Lafont, S.; Mayo, W.; Fabrigoule, C. The Effect of Education on Cognitive Performances and Its Implication for the Constitution of the Cognitive Reserve. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2003, 23, 317–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Stern, Y.; Gurland, B.; Tatemichi, T.K.; Tang, M.X.; Wilder, D.; Mayeux, R. Influence of Education and Occupation on the Incidence of Alzheimer’s Disease. JAMA 1994, 271, 1004–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. De Souza-Talarico, J.N.; Caramelli, P.; Nitrini, R.; Chaves, E.C. The Influence of Schooling on Working Memory Performance in Elderly Individuals without Cognitive Decline. Dement. Neuropsychol. 2007, 1, 276–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Ostrosky-Solis, F.; Ardila, A.; Rosselli, M.; Lopez-Arango, G.; Uriel-Mendoza, V. Neuropsychological Test Performance in Illiterate Subjects. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. Off. J. Natl. Acad. Neuropsychol. 1998, 13, 645–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Berry, D.T.R.; Allen, R.S.; Schmitt, F.A. Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Psychometric Characteristics in a Geriatric Sample. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1991, 5, 143–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Lu, P.; Boone, K.; Cozolino, L.; Mitchell, C. Effectiveness of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and the Meyers and Meyers Recognition Trial in the Detection of Suspect Effort. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2003, 17, 426–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Tremblay, M.-P.; Potvin, O.; Callahan, B.L.; Belleville, S.; Gagnon, J.-F.; Caza, N.; Ferland, G.; Hudon, C.; Macoir, J. Normative Data for the Rey-Osterrieth and the Taylor Complex Figure Tests in Quebec-French People. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2015, 30, 78–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Rivera, D.; Perrin, P.B.; Morlett-Paredes, A.; Galarza-del-Angel, J.; Martínez, C.; Garza, M.T.; Saracho, C.P.; Rodríguez, W.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Rábago, B.; et al. Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure—Copy and Immediate Recall: Normative Data for the Latin American Spanish Speaking Adult Population. NeuroRehabilitation 2015, 37, 677–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Rodríguez-Lorenzana, A.; Núñez-Fernández, S.; Adana-Díaz, L.; Mascialino, G.; Ponce, T.Y.; Rivera, D.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C. Normative Data for Test of Learning and Memory in an Ecuadorian Adult Population. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2020, 34, 54–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Roberts, K.L.; Allen, H.A. Perception and Cognition in the Ageing Brain: A Brief Review of the Short- and Long-Term Links between Perceptual and Cognitive Decline. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 8, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Stern, Y. Cognitive Reserve. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 2015–2028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Lucas, J.A.; Ivnik, R.J.; Smith, G.E.; Ferman, T.J.; Willis, F.B.; Petersen, R.C.; Graff-Radford, N.R. Mayo’s Older African Americans Normative Studies: Norms for Boston Naming Test, Controlled Oral Word Association, Category Fluency, Animal Naming, Token Test, WRAT-3 Reading, Trail Making Test, Stroop Test, and Judgment of Line Orientation. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2005, 19, 243–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Peña-Casanova, J.; Quintana-Aparicio, M.; Quiñones-Ubeda, S.; Aguilar, M.; Molinuevo, J.L.; Serradell, M.; Robles, A.; Barquero, M.S.; Villanueva, C.; Antunez, C.; et al. Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery-Abbreviated, and Judgment of Line Orientation. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2009, 24, 355–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Steinberg, B.A.; Bieliauskas, L.A.; Smith, G.E.; Langellotti, C.; Ivnik, R.J. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: Age- and IQ-Adjusted Norms for the Boston Naming Test, the MAE Token Test, and the Judgment of Line Orientation Test. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2005, 19, 280–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Winblad, B.; Palmer, K.; Kivipelto, M.; Jelic, V.; Fratiglioni, L.; Wahlund, L.-O.; Nordberg, A.; Backman, L.; Albert, M.; Almkvist, O.; et al. Mild Cognitive Impairment—Beyond Controversies, towards a Consensus: Report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Intern. Med. 2004, 256, 240–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Albert, M.S.; DeKosky, S.T.; Dickson, D.; Dubois, B.; Feldman, H.H.; Fox, N.C.; Gamst, A.; Holtzman, D.M.; Jagust, W.J.; Petersen, R.C.; et al. The Diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment Due to Alzheimer’s Disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association Workgroups on Diagnostic Guidelines for Alzheimer’s Disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011, 7, 270–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Petersen, R.C.; Smith, G.E.; Waring, S.C.; Ivnik, R.J.; Tangalos, E.G.; Kokmen, E. Mild Cognitive Impairment: Clinical Characterization and Outcome. Arch. Neurol. 1999, 56, 303–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Jak, A.J.; Preis, S.R.; Beiser, A.S.; Seshadri, S.; Wolf, P.A.; Bondi, M.W.; Au, R. Neuropsychological Criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Risk in the Framingham Heart Study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2016, 22, 937–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  90. Oltra-Cucarella, J.; Sánchez-SanSegundo, M.; Lipnicki, D.M.; Sachdev, P.S.; Crawford, J.D.; Pérez-Vicente, J.A.; Cabello-Rodríguez, L.; Ferrer-Cascales, R. Using Base Rate of Low Scores to Identify Progression from Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment to Alzheimer’s Disease: Base Rate of Low Scores for MCI Diagnosis. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2018, 66, 1360–1366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Oltra-Cucarella, J.; Sánchez-SanSegundo, M.; Rubio-Aparicio, M.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Ferrer-Cascales, R. The Association Between the Number of Neuropsychological Measures and the Base Rate of Low Scores. Assessment 2019, 28, 955–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Jak, A.J.; Bondi, M.W.; Delano-Wood, L.; Wierenga, C.; Corey-Bloom, J.; Salmon, D.P.; Delis, D.C. Quantification of Five Neuropsychological Approaches to Defining Mild Cognitive Impairment. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2009, 17, 368–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Oltra-Cucarella, J.; Sánchez-SanSegundo, M.; Lipnicki, D.M.; Crawford, J.D.; Lipton, R.B.; Katz, M.J.; Zammit, A.R.; Scarmeas, N.; Dardiotis, E.; Kosmidis, M.H.; et al. Visual Memory Tests Enhance the Identification of Amnestic MCI Cases at Greater Risk of Alzheimer’s Disease. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2019, 31, 997–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Oltra-Cucarella, J.; Ferrer-Cascales, R.; Alegret, M.; Gasparini, R.; Díaz-Ortiz, L.M.; Ríos, R.; Martínez-Nogueras, Á.L.; Onandia, I.; Pérez-Vicente, J.A.; Cabello-Rodríguez, L.; et al. Risk of Progression to Alzheimer’s Disease for Different Neuropsychological Mild Cognitive Impairment Subtypes: A Hierarchical Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Psychol. Aging 2018, 33, 1007–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Polunina, A.; Bryun, E.; Sydniaeva, E.; Golukhova, E. Gender Differences in Cognitive Functions: Retrospective Analysis of the Data of 5 Neuropsychological Studies. Sch. Rep. 2018, 3, 61–75. [Google Scholar]
  96. Zhang, J.; Zhou, W.; Wang, L.; Zhang, X. Gender Differences of Neuropsychological Profiles in Cognitively Normal Older People without Amyloid Pathology. Compr. Psychiatry 2017, 75, 22–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Rosselli, M.; Ardila, A. The Impact of Culture and Education on Non-Verbal Neuropsychological Measurements: A Critical Review. Brain Cogn. 2003, 52, 326–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Percentage of participants with one or more low scores per normative data set.
Figure 1. Percentage of participants with one or more low scores per normative data set.
Ijerph 18 12977 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for neuropsychological performance.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for neuropsychological performance.
Neuropsychological TestMSDRange
FCSRT-Imm44.014.0426–48
FCSRT-Del14.861.489–16
ROCF-C28.182.2414–36
ROCF-Imm14.974.902–26.5
ROCF-Del15.044.802–25
JLO20.554.949–30
M: mean; SD: standard deviation; FCSRT-Imm: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, total immediate recall; FCSRT-Del: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, total delayed recall; ROCF-C: Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, copy; ROCF-Imm: Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, immediate recall; ROCF-Del: Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, delayed recall; JLO: Judgement of Lines Orientation.
Table 2. Multiple linear regression models.
Table 2. Multiple linear regression models.
BStd. ErrorSig.R2SEE
FCSRT-ImmIntercept40.311.170.0000.1043.865
EducationMin0.2880.1110.011
Sex1.8860.8110.022
FSCRT-DelIntercept11.5670.8360.0000.1951.350
EducationMin0.5470.1850.004
Sex0.6670.2830.021
EducationMin2−0.0210.0100.030
FSCRT-DelSABIEXIntercept9.3860.4300.0000.6320.908
FCSRT-ImmMin0.2720.0230.000
EducationMin0.0690.0270.011
ROCF-ImmSABIEXIntercept8.7711.570.0000.1444.554
ROCF-CMin0.8720.0440.000
ROCF-DelSABIEXIntercept3.7270.6160.0000.7922.199
ROCF-ImmMin0.8720.0440.000
JLOIntercept22.2520.9580.0000.2084.446
Sex−4.2630.9330.000
EducationMin20.0140.0070.038
FCSRT-Imm: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, total immediate recall; FCSRT-Del: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, total delayed recall, independently calculated from FCSRT-Imm; FCSRT_DelSABIEX: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, total delayed recall, conditional on FCSRT-Imm; ROCF-ImmSABIEX: Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, immediate recall, conditional on ROCF-C; ROCF-DelSABIEX: Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, delayed recall, conditional on ROCF-C and ROCF-Imm; JLO: Judgement of Line Orientation.
Table 3. Frequency and accumulated percentage of NEURONORMA, SABIEXINDEP and SABIEXDEP low scores.
Table 3. Frequency and accumulated percentage of NEURONORMA, SABIEXINDEP and SABIEXDEP low scores.
Low ScoresNEURONORMASABIEXINDEPSABIEXDEP
FreqCum%FreqCum%Freq.Cum%
0711006610057100
12331.11735.93144.7
238.81219.51214.6
345.957.822.9
41222.911
5111100
Total103 103 103
Freq.: frequency of subjects showing the number of low scores in the Low Scores column. Cum%: cumulative percentage.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Calderón-Rubio, E.; Oltra-Cucarella, J.; Bonete-López, B.; Iñesta, C.; Sitges-Maciá, E. Regression-Based Normative Data for Independent and Cognitively Active Spanish Older Adults: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and Judgement of Line Orientation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 12977. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182412977

AMA Style

Calderón-Rubio E, Oltra-Cucarella J, Bonete-López B, Iñesta C, Sitges-Maciá E. Regression-Based Normative Data for Independent and Cognitively Active Spanish Older Adults: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and Judgement of Line Orientation. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(24):12977. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182412977

Chicago/Turabian Style

Calderón-Rubio, Eva, Javier Oltra-Cucarella, Beatriz Bonete-López, Clara Iñesta, and Esther Sitges-Maciá. 2021. "Regression-Based Normative Data for Independent and Cognitively Active Spanish Older Adults: Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and Judgement of Line Orientation" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 24: 12977. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182412977

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop