Next Article in Journal
Features of Action Potentials from Identified Thalamic Nuclei in Anesthetized Patients
Previous Article in Journal
Toward a Computational Neuropsychology of Cognitive Flexibility
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample

1
Department of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo 315211, China
2
Department of Psychological Science, Kennesaw State University, Kennesaw, GA 30144, USA
3
Lab of Light and Physiopsychological Health, National Center for International Research on Green Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China
4
Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Optical Information Materials and Technology & Institute of Electronic Paper Displays, South China Academy of Advanced Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China
5
School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2020, 10(12), 1001; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001
Submission received: 26 October 2020 / Revised: 11 December 2020 / Accepted: 15 December 2020 / Published: 17 December 2020

Abstract

:
The stereotype content model (SCM; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu, 2002) identifies four basic categories of stereotyped social groups: high warmth-high competence (HW-HC), high warmth-low competence (HW-LC), low warmth-high competence (LW-HC), and low warmth-low competence (LW-LC). However, many of these groups have not been directly examined in stereotype activation research. The purpose of the present research was to extend stereotype activation research to groups that more fully represent those identified under the SCM. Employing explicit sequential priming task, participants responded to prime-target stimulus pairs that were either congruent or incongruent with stereotypes of social groups from all four SCM quadrants in two studies in the current investigation. Study 1 was to determine the behavioral pattern of explicit stereotype activation among four quadrants (the sample included 60 Chinese undergraduate students, 51%—female). Study 2 further employed event-related brain potentials (ERPs) technique to track the time course and electrophysiological underpinnings of explicit stereotype activation (the sample included 22 right-handed Chinese undergraduate students, 76%—female). In Study 1, participants responded more quickly and accurately on stereotype congruent trials than incongruent trials for all social groups except LW-LC groups. This reverse priming effect on LW-LC social groups in RTs was also replicated in Study 2. ERPs findings further showed that incongruent targets elicited larger N400 amplitudes than congruent targets for all four SCM quadrants. Moreover, congruent targets elicited larger P2 than incongruent targets, but only found for the LW-LC social groups. In addition, congruent targets elicited larger amplitudes of late positive component than incongruent targets for the low warmth (LW-LC and LW-HC) groups. Together, these results highlight the unique processing that LW-LC groups receive throughout the cognitive stream, ultimately manifesting in distinctive behavioral responses. Unconscious activation of egalitarian goals, disgust, and distrust accounts are discussed.

1. Introduction

In an effort to process and simplify a complex social world, people often rely on mental shortcuts and heuristics as they navigate their daily lives [1,2]. Stereotypes represent one of these mental shortcuts, and while their use can provide a sense of order and coherence, they can just as easily lead to misunderstandings and friction in one’s social environment. The activation of stereotypes and their resulting impact on human behavior are seen as a ubiquitous part of being human, thus they have been the subject of intense research scrutiny for over half a century. Starting in the 1980s, researchers adapted response time paradigms to study stereotype activation (e.g., [3,4,5,6]). More recently, various neuroscience techniques have been used to further elucidate the cognitive processes involved in stereotype activation (see reviews [7,8,9]). This line of research has revealed a great deal about stereotype activation and the conditions under which it occurs (for review, see [10]), but the vast majority has focused on only one or two social groups (e.g., racial groups, gender groups). According to the stereotype content model (SCM [11,12]), there are many other groups in society that are consistently stereotyped, and these groups also require empirical attention. The primary purpose of the present research was therefore to investigate patterns of stereotype activation in both response times and neural activity for a wide range of social groups identified under the SCM.

1.1. The Stereotype Content Model (SCM) and BIAS Map

The SCM [11,13] organizes social and cultural groups along two universal and fundamental dimensions: perceived warmth and perceived competence. Warmth refers to the intent to help or harm others, and warmth judgments are dictated primarily by perceptions of competition (competing groups are perceived as less warm). Competence refers more specifically to the ability to help or cause harm, and judgments are based more on the perceived status of the group in society (lower status groups are perceived as less competent). Social and cultural groups in society are perceived as low or high on each of these dimensions, resulting in their placement in one of four different quadrants of the model. Some groups are perceived as high on both warmth and competence (HW-HC). These are typically groups that participants perceive as part of their in-group or are held in relatively universal high regard within society. On the other end of the spectrum, some groups are perceived as low on both warmth and competence (LW-LC). These groups are held in relatively universal low regard and include groups such as the homeless and drug addicts. Finally, there are groups in society that have mixed stereotype content, meaning they are perceived as high on one dimension, but low on the other. For example, some groups are perceived as high on warmth but low on competence (HW-LC) (e.g., elderly people, the disabled). Other groups are perceived as low on warmth but high on competence (LW-HC) (e.g., rich people). Research has confirmed that SCM patterns generalize across time, level of analysis, and measures (see reviews [14,15]). Additionally, the SCM has been validated in multiple countries [16,17,18,19]. To further develop the SCM, BIAS (behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotype) map [12,20] was proposed to capture the affect and action tendencies associated with stereotype. Besides the application in intergroup situations, SCM and BIAS map could also be employed in interpersonal perception [21,22,23].

1.2. Low Warmth—Low Competence (LW-LC) Social Groups

Among the four quadrants of the SCM, groups in the LW-LC quadrant might be processed differently from groups in the other three quadrants. According to the SCM and BIAS map, LW-LC social groups elicit contempt and disgust along with active harm behavioral tendencies (e.g., attacking or harassing) [11,12,13,20]. Social groups in the LW-LC (vs. HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC) quadrant fail to elicit brain activity associated with human social processing (medial pre-frontal cortex, mPFC) and instead elicit activity associated with disgust (insula and amygdala) [24,25] but these findings have not been replicated by others. In other words, it has been argued that members of social groups in the LW-LC quadrant are more likely to be dehumanized relative to social groups in the other three quadrants (see [26,27] for a more complete discussion). Other research demonstrated that brain regions associated with the emotion of disgust (the left anterior insula) are more active when thinking about the homeless (LW-LC) than the elderly (HW-LC) [28]. Compared with other three quadrants, participants were more willing to sacrifice LW-LC group members in moral dilemmas [29]. Moreover, desirable events were anticipated to occur significantly more frequently than undesirable events for HW-HC and HW-LC group members while this social optimism biases were reversed for LW-LC groups [30,31,32]. Compared to HW-HC and HW-LC groups, the temporoparietal junction and right anterior temporal lobe were recruited when evaluating LW-HC and LW-LC groups, while thinking about LW-LC groups additionally involved the dorsomedial frontal cortex (stereotypical thinking and disgust), inferior frontal cortex (stereotypical thinking), and anterior insula (disgust and repulsion) [31].

1.3. Response Pattern of Stereotype Activation

Behavioral research on stereotype activation has relied heavily on priming and response time techniques (e.g., [33,34,35,36]). A common paradigm used in stereotype research is the sequential priming paradigm [37,38]. In this paradigm, participants view trials that each contains two stimuli (prime and target) with the meaning congruency between the stimuli varied across trials. For example, in stereotype priming, a congruent stimulus pair would be the prime “men” followed by the stereotypically congruent target “strong”. An incongruent stimulus pair would be the prime “women” followed by the stereotypically incongruent target “strong”. If participants respond more quickly to a target preceded by a stereotypically congruent prime than those preceded by a stereotypically incongruent prime, this is interpreted as evidence of stereotype priming/activation. The sequential priming paradigm has been used to examine stereotypes for a variety of different social groups [37,38] (such as gender [3,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54], and race [52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67]) and the priming effect was repeatedly reported [1,68,69], but we are not aware of any research that examined groups from all SCM quadrants simultaneously and compared observed priming effects among them. Thus, the purpose of Study 1 in the present research was to investigate the pattern of stereotype activation in behavioral response for social groups that fall into the four different quadrants of the SCM. In line with previous research, we expected to observe lower accuracy and slower response times on stereotype incongruent trials than stereotype congruent trials, and that this pattern would be observed for social groups from all four SCM quadrants.

1.4. Electrophysiological Pattern of Stereotype Activation

Stereotype activation occurs rapidly and involves multiple cognitive processes [8,9,43,70,71]. Due to high temporal resolution (milliseconds), event-related potential (ERP) is optimal for studying the progress of stereotype activation as it unfolds in real-time [72,73]. The various ERP components are also sensitive to different types of cognitive processes, enabling researchers to disentangle the presence and impact of these processes. Thus, ERPs are better able to examine the time course of stereotype activation, and the cognitive processes that contribute to this activation, than either response times or fMRI data. Similar to response times research, however, ERP research on stereotypes and stereotype activation has focused on a limited number of social groups (e.g., men/women, black/white) [40,41,67,74,75]. The utility of ERPs to study all types of SCM groups therefore remains untested. Study 2 in the present research had two primary goals: (1) extend neuropsychological investigations of the SCM by using ERPs to elucidate the cognitive processing time-course for multiple social groups, and (2) extend previous ERP research on stereotype activation by identifying potential differences among groups in the four SCM quadrants. These goals were accomplished by examining ERP components at different time points in the information-processing stream.

1.4.1. Early ERP Component: P2

The P2 component has previously been connected with stereotype processing. While the P2 component appears to be modulated by a variety of cognitive tasks and conditions, it is generally thought to also reflect mechanisms of selective attention [76]. Previous research has found the P2 component to be particularly sensitive to race and stereotypes. Specifically, larger P2 amplitudes were elicited by pictures of black faces (than white faces) among white participants (e.g., [77,78,79,80]), and larger P2 amplitudes have been observed on race stereotype congruent trials than incongruent trials in a sequential priming paradigm [67]. These results still need to be replicated for other stereotyped social groups in society. Given the uniquely stigmatized status of LW-LC groups, we expected these groups to attract greater attention early in the processing stream, which would be reflected in larger P2 amplitudes on stereotype-congruent trials for these groups.

1.4.2. Later ERP Components: N400 and LPC

Two later ERP components have been examined in research on stereotypes and stereotype activation—the N400 and the late positive component (LPC). The N400 component is a negative deflection that peaks around 400 milliseconds following stimulus exposure. It is thought to reflect the influence of both bottom-up and top-down neural processes in efforts to construct meaning, drawing on information from the multimodal long-term memory system [81]. The amplitude of the N400 is particularly responsive to the ease of integrating information into the preceding context, with larger amplitudes elicited when integration is more difficult. For example, larger N400 amplitudes would be elicited by the word “jelly” if preceded by the word “shoe” than if preceded by the word “grape” [82]. One potential explanation for these results is that exposure to the first word (“grape”) results in the activation of long-term memory information related to this concept as the system attempts to reconstruct the meaning of this concept. If the second word is related to the concept of “grape” (as would be the case for the word “jelly”), then the preactivation of this information would facilitate processing of the second word. Thus, differences in N400 amplitudes may reflect differences in the activation of multimodal information in long-term memory during the meaning-construction process [81].
Recent research extended earlier N400 findings to stereotypes, and consistently found the N400 to be sensitive to stereotype incongruities (e.g., [40,41,67,75,83]). This was first demonstrated by White and colleagues (2009) [40], who presented participants with prime-target stimulus pairs which were congruent or incongruent with gender stereotypes. In this study, stereotype incongruent trials (e.g., MEN: NURSE) elicited significantly larger N400 amplitudes than stereotype congruent trials (e.g., WOMEN: NURSE). Later studies replicated these results for gender stereotypes in a non-Western culture (i.e., Chinese culture [39,41,42,43]) and for other stereotype dimensions, such as race [67] and migrant workers [83]. This suggests that the N400 ERP component is sensitive to stereotype incongruities and may reflect differences in stereotype activation. However, whether these results could be replicated and generalized in social groups that represent all four quadrants of the SCM still remains unclear. Aligned with previous research, we expected that stereotype-incongruent targets would elicit larger N400 amplitudes than congruent targets for social groups in all four quadrants of the SCM.
Evidence for the sensitivity of the LPC to stereotypes is more mixed. The LPC is a positive deflection with typical onset around 300 milliseconds, extending for several hundred additional milliseconds. In one early study, participants read sentences that violated definitional (e.g., the mailman took a shower after she got home) or stereotypical (e.g., our aerobics instructor gave himself a break) noun-pronoun agreement [84]. Relative to sentences without a noun-pronoun violation, LPC amplitudes were enhanced for both definitionally and stereotypically incongruent sentences, suggesting that the LPC might be sensitive to stereotype incongruities. Other studies also found the LPC to be sensitive to pronoun incongruities within the context of social and nonsocial sentences [85,86]. In contrast to these results, the LPC was not modulated by stereotype incongruities in studies that employed a sequential priming paradigm (i.e., presented participants with word pairs rather than sentences) (e.g., [40,41,67]). Given these latter results, we did not expect stereotype congruity to modulate LPC amplitudes in Study 2.

1.5. Current Research

In the current research, we conducted two studies to explore the similarities and differences on explicit stereotype activation for HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC and LW-LC social groups based on the framework of SCM. Employing sequential priming paradigm, we investigated the behavioral pattern of explicit stereotype activation in Study 1 and further examined the electrophysiological pattern of explicit stereotype activation among four quadrants by using ERPs in Study 2.

2. Study 1: The Behavioral Pattern of Explicit Stereotype Activation for HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC and LW-LC Social Groups

2.1. Methods

2.1.1. Participants

Sixty Chinese undergraduate students (31 female, M = 21.5 years, range 19–25 years) participated in the study in exchange for 10 RMB. All participants signed an informed consent form approved by the Academic Ethical Committee of Ningbo University. Data from two participants (1 female and 1 male) were excluded due to excessive incorrect trials (greater than 30%), leaving a final sample of 58 participants.

2.1.2. Design

Group (HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC, LW-LC) × congruency (congruent, incongruent) within-subject design was applied in the current study.

2.1.3. Stimuli

Stimuli were Chinese word pairs consisting of a prime word followed by a target word. Prime words were 24 social group labels, which were retrieved from Yang et al. (2019) [87], which were validated in Ji et al. (2020) [88] (see Appendix A). These social groups were divided into four categories, according to the two SCM dimensions of warmth and competence. Specifically, the six high warmth-high competence (HW-HC) groups were soldiers, firemen, professors, psychotherapists, air hostesses, and yoga instructors. The six high warmth-low competence (HW-LC) groups included elderly, farmers, housewives, migrant workers, left-behind children, and cleaning workers. The six low warmth-high competence (LW-HC) groups were businessmen, overseas returnees, government workers, government officials, stars in showbiz, and rich. Finally, the six low warmth-low competence (LW-LC) groups included criminals, unemployed, beggars, drug addicts, terrorists, and urban management officers.
Target stimuli were words stereotypically associated with one of the social groups. Target words were also retrieved from Yang et al. (2019) [87], which were selected through the classical method by Katz and Braly (1933) [89] (see [88] for details). For each quadrant of SCM, the 30 most stereotypic words were selected, totaling 120 target words (see Appendix A).
These 120 words were used to form both congruent and incongruent trials. When paired with their matching social group, the words formed congruent trials. Incongruent trials were then created by pairing each target word with a social group that was as different in stereotypes as possible. This resulted in 30 congruent and 30 incongruent target words for each social group category (see Appendix A). Thus, eight experimental conditions were established: stereotype congruent vs. incongruent for the HW-HC social group category, stereotype congruent vs. incongruent for the HW-LC social group category, stereotype congruent vs. incongruent for the LW-HC social group category, and stereotype congruent vs. incongruent for the LW-LC social group category. In total, there were 120 congruent prime-target trials and 120 incongruent prime-target trials. Each prime-target word pair was presented twice, thus there were 480 trials in the experiment.
All primes and targets (Chinese characters) were presented centrally on a white background in 60-point, black, boldface font. All stimuli were presented in the center of a 17 in. LCD (Liquid Crystal Display) monitor with a silver-gray background (resolution: 1024 × 768, refresh rate: 60 Hz) using E-Prime 2.0 (see Figure 1). The stimuli, seen from a distance of 120 cm, occupied a visual angle of 6.06° × 6.06°.

2.1.4. Procedure

Upon arriving at the lab, participants read and signed an informed consent form. Each participant was escorted to a dimly lit, sound attenuated testing room and seated in a comfortable chair approximately 120 cm in front of a computer monitor. Participants were instructed to watch the center of the screen where a series of word pairs would be presented. It was explained that each trial would begin with a fixation point, then they would see a social group label (e.g., bagger), and the second word would sometimes be associated with the preceding social group (i.e., sloppy) and other times not associated with the preceding social group (i.e., pretty). Participants were instructed to verify as quickly and as accurately as possible whether or not the second word matched the priming word according to cultural stereotypes by pressing the “CONGRUENT” button on the response keyboard for matching words and the “INCONGRUENT” button on the response keyboard for non-matching words. The “CONGRUENT” and “INCONGRUENT” labels were pasted on the “Q” and “P” keys of the keyboard before the experiment and were counterbalanced across participants.
As presented in Figure 1, each trial began with the appearance of a fixation point for 500 ms. The fixation point was then replaced with a prime stimulus, which remained on the screen for 700 ms. After an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 500 ms (SOA (Stimulus Onset Asynchrony) = 1200 ms), the target stimulus was presented for 300 ms. The screen then went blank and remained blank until the participant responded. The inter-trial interval (ITI) was randomized between 600 and 800 ms.
Participants completed a total of 528 trials, 480 of which constituted the experimental trials (presented in 6 blocks of 80 trials). The first 48 trials were presented as practice trials to familiarize participants with the procedure. Six social groups (policemen, otaku, Tibetan girls, football players, models, and the mentally disabled) were used as practice primes. The 480 experimental trials were presented after these practice trials. Stimuli used in practice trials did not appear in the experimental trials and only data from the experimental trials was analyzed. Participants took a break (about 3 to 4 min in length) after each block of 80 trials.

2.1.5. Data Analyses

Accuracy rates and response times (RTs) were analyzed using two-way repeated measures ANOVAs, with both groups (HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC, LW-LC) and congruency (congruent, incongruent) as within-subject factors. For each participant, incorrect responses and response times that deviated ±2 SD from each condition were removed before formal analysis [90]. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 13.0. When the assumption of sphericity was violated, the Greenhouse–Geisser (G–G) adjustment, with the corrected significant levels and partial eta-squared (η2) effect-size statistic, are reported. For simplicity, the uncorrected degrees of freedom are presented along with the G–G epsilon (ε). Post hoc comparisons were conducted using a Bonferroni correction to control for inflated family-wise error.

2.2. Results

2.2.1. Accuracy

The analysis revealed a significant main effect for group, F (3, 171) = 93.58, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.62, but not for congruency, F (1, 57) = 1.24, p = 0.270, partial η2 = 0.02. The main effect for group was qualified by a significant interaction with congruency, F (3, 171) = 7.22, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.11. As presented in Figure 2A, simple effect analyses for the HW-HC group showed that accuracy on trials with congruent targets (M = 0.972, SE = 0.005) was significantly higher than for trials with incongruent targets (M = 0.955, SE = 0.006), p = 0.002. The difference in accuracy between congruent and incongruent trials was also significant for the LW-LC group, but in the opposite direction; accuracy on trials with congruent targets (M = 0.864, SE = 0.012) was significantly lower than trials with incongruent targets (M = 0.901, SE = 0.008), p = 0.009. There was no significant difference in accuracy between trials with congruent and incongruent targets for the HW-LC (for congruent targets: M = 0.922, SE = 0.008; for incongruent targets: M = 0.934, SE = 0.007; p = 0.154) or the LW-HC groups (for congruent targets: M = 0.896, SE = 0.009; for incongruent targets: M = 0.900, SE = 0.009; p = 0.781).

2.2.2. Response Times

Analysis of RTs revealed significant main effects for both Group, F (3, 171) = 66.67, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.54, and congruency, F (1, 57) = 13.34, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.19. The interaction between group and congruency was also significant, F (3, 171) = 30.41, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.35. As presented in Figure 2B, participants responded significantly faster to congruent targets than incongruent targets for the HW-HC (for congruent targets: M = 722 ms, SE =18; for incongruent targets: M = 813 ms, SE = 23; p < 0.001), HW-LC (for congruent targets: M = 817 ms, SE = 23; for incongruent targets: M = 848 ms, SE = 27; p = 0.031), and LW-HC groups (for congruent targets: M = 791 ms, SE = 20; for incongruent targets: M = 860 ms, SE = 26; p < 0.001). Conversely, participants responded significantly slower to congruent targets (M = 898 ms, SE = 28) than to incongruent targets for the LW-LC group (M = 850 ms, SE = 24), p = 0.003.

2.3. Discussion

Using a sequential priming paradigm, Study 1 examined the behavioral pattern of stereotype priming on accuracy rates and response times for the four quadrants (HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-LC, and LW-HC) identified under the framework of SCM. The typical stereotype priming pattern was observed (especially in RTs) for the HW-HC, HW-LC and LW-HC social groups. That is, congruent targets elicited significantly faster responses than incongruent targets for these social groups, replicating previous studies [3,6,40,42,47]. More importantly, a reverse stereotype priming pattern for LW-LC groups was revealed in Study 1. That is, congruent targets elicited significantly slower responses than incongruent targets, which indicated the robustness of reverse priming effect for LW-LC social groups.
Overall, the results from Study 1 replicate the typical stereotype priming pattern for HW-HC, HW-LC and LW-HC social groups, but reveal a decidedly reverse stereotype priming pattern for LW-LC social groups. Accuracy rates and response times may not be sensitive enough to adequately probe stereotype priming results, though. Behavioral RTs represent the outcome of a set of cognitive processes, and thus are limited to measuring a single conscious outcome of these processes [91]. For example, response times in sequential priming paradigms may reflect the influence of memory activation and response competition [92]. Distinguishing among these processes in behavioral results can be done using specific analyses and models (e.g., [93,94,95]), but an alternative approach is to measure stereotype activation more directly with event-related potentials (ERPs). This approach was adopted in Study 2, which used both behavioral and ERP measures to further examine patterns of stereotype activation.

3. Study 2: The Electrophysiological Pattern of Explicit Stereotype Activation for HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC and LW-LC Social Groups

The purpose of Study 2 was to explore potential neuropsychological differences in stereotype activation patterns among the four SCM clusters. Both early (P2) and later (N400, LPC) ERP components were examined to determine if stereotype activation patterns differ among four SCM quadrants, both in magnitude of activation and timing. The N400 ERP component was of primary importance, given that previous research has found it to be a reliable measure of stereotype activation [39,40,41,42,67,83], and it reflects a post-perceptual processing stage that involves the dynamic reconstruction of information from long-term memory [81]. In this way, Study 2 aimed to demonstrate the utility of the N400 ERP component as a measure of stereotype activation for a wide range of social groups identified under the framework of SCM. In addition to the N400, we also examined early ERP component (P2) at fronto-central regions to capture early perceptual processing stages and the late positive component (LPC), which reflects a later, decision processing stage.
Overall, we expected to replicate the results of Study 1, that is, a typical stereotype priming effect in response times for the HW-HC, HW-LC, and LW-HC groups, and reverse priming effect for the LW-LC groups. For the N400, we expected stereotype incongruent trials to elicit larger N400 amplitudes than stereotype congruent trials for all four SCM group clusters, but that this effect would be especially pronounced for the LW-LC social group cluster. Due to the attention-grabbing nature of the LW-LC groups, we also expected these social groups to elicit larger P2 amplitudes than the other SCM clusters. No differences were expected for any of the remaining ERP components.

3.1. Methods

3.1.1. Participants

Twenty-two right-handed, Chinese undergraduate students (16 female, M = 21.9 years, range 19–25 years) participated in the study in exchange for 30 RMB. All participants reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision, had no history of current or past neurological or psychiatric illness, and took no medications known to affect the central nervous system. All signed an informed consent form approved by the Academic Ethical Committee of Ningbo University. Data from three participants (1 female and 2 male) were excluded due to excessive artifacts, leaving a final sample of 19 participants.

3.1.2. Stimuli and Procedure

The stimuli used in Study 2 were the same as those used in Study 1. The procedures used were also identical, except that participants were prepared for electroencephalogram (EEG) recording before the formal experiment.

3.1.3. EEG Recording

EEG data was recorded continuously using an electrode cap with 64 Ag/AgCl electrodes mounted according to the extended international 10–20 system. Electro-oculargram (EOG) data was recorded via two pairs of electrodes placed on the bilateral external canthi and left supraorbital and infraorbital areas to monitor eye movements and blinks. Both EEG and EOG were sampled at 500 Hz, with a bandpass of 0.05–100 Hz using NeuroScan Synamps2 Amplifier. The left mastoid served as a reference during recording and the right mastoid served as a recording electrode. Electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ.
NeuroScan 4.3 software (Compumedics Neuroscan, Charlotte, NC, USA) was used to analyze the data offline. The data was re-referenced offline to the algebraic average of the left and right mastoids. The raw EEG data were manually previewed to remove artifacts after merging with the behavioral data. Then an automatic eye-movement correction program corrected vertical eye movements and blinks [96]. Afterwards, the EEG was segmented in epochs of 1200 ms beginning 200 ms prior to the target stimulus onset and each epoch in all electrodes was then corrected to a 200 ms baseline. Segments contaminated with artifacts exceeding an amplitude of ±80 μV were automatically rejected from the averaging (9.7% artifacts were rejected). EEG data from correct trials were averaged separately for each of the eight conditions. After this procedure, averaged ERPs included at least 48 trials for each trial type. Finally, the averaged ERPs were low-pass filtered at 30 Hz (24 dB/Oct ave).

3.1.4. Data Analyses

Behavioral data were analyzed using the same method in Study 1. For ERPs data, mean amplitude of each ERP component (P2, N400, LPC) was subjected to two-way repeated measures ANOVA with both group (HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC, LW-LC) and congruency (congruent, incongruent) as within-subject factors. The analyses were conducted on ERP mean amplitudes of clusters of electrodes (regions of interest, ROI). The measurement windows and electrodes to be included in the ROIs were determined by visual inspection of the ERP waveforms and literature-based hypotheses (for exact time-windows and ROIs of each component please see the corresponding section in the results) [97]. All post-hoc tests were Bonferroni corrected for multiple testing. If necessary, the degrees of freedom were corrected using the Greenhouse–Geisser epsilon.

3.2. Results

3.2.1. Behavioral Results

Accuracy. Accuracy results mirrored those from Study 1. That is, the main effect was significant for group, F (3, 54) = 23.55, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.57, but not for congruency, F (1, 18) = 0.06, p = 0.809, partial η2 = 0.003. Additionally, the main effect for group was qualified by a significant interaction with congruency, F (3, 54) = 4.88, p = 0.005, partial η2 = 0.21. As presented in Figure 2C, simple effect analyses for the HW-HC quadrant revealed that accuracy on congruent trials (M = 0.984, SE = 0.005) was significantly higher than incongruent trials (M = 0.958, SE = 0.008), p = 0.002. The difference in accuracy between congruent and incongruent trials was also significant for the LW-LC quadrant, but in the opposite direction; accuracy on congruent trials (M = 0.874, SE = 0.019) was significantly lower than incongruent trials (M = 0.920, SE = 0.013), p = 0.012. There was no significant difference in accuracy between congruent and incongruent trials for the HW-LC (for congruent targets: M = 0.945, SE = 0.013; for incongruent targets: M = 0.940, SE = 0.011; p = 0.762) or LW-HC groups (for congruent targets: M = 0.924, SE = 0.012; for incongruent targets: M = 0.918, SE = 0.015; p = 0.819).
Response times. Similar to Study 1, for HW-HC, LW-LC and LW-HC groups, analysis of RTs revealed significant main effects for both group, F (3, 54) = 49.44, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.73, and congruency, F (1, 18) = 9.45, p = 0.007, partial η2 = 0.34. The interaction between group and congruency was also significant, F (3, 54) = 25.38, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.59. Simple effect analyses showed that, as presented in Figure 2D, participants responded significantly faster to congruent targets than incongruent targets for the HW-HC (for congruent targets: M = 698 ms, SE = 25; for incongruent targets: M = 797 ms, SE = 31; p < 0.001) and LW-HC groups (for congruent targets: M = 757 ms, SE = 28; for incongruent targets: M = 823 ms, SE = 28; p < 0.001). Although participants also responded faster to congruent targets (M = 793 ms, SE = 28) than incongruent targets (M = 822 ms, SE = 30) for the HW-LC groups, the difference did not reach a statistically significant level (p = 0.122). Conversely, participants responded significantly slower to congruent targets (M = 896 ms, SE = 34) than to incongruent targets (M = 834 ms, SE = 32) for the LW-LC group (p = 0.001).

3.2.2. ERPs Results

P2. For the P2 component, the ROI included 3 central electrodes (Fz, FCz, Cz) in the 150–250 ms time-window. Amplitudes were averaged across all electrodes within the ROI before submitting them to statistical analysis. Analysis of P2 mean amplitudes revealed significant main effect of group, F (3, 54) = 10.14, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.36, but the main effect of congruency was not, F (1, 18) = 1.47, p = 0.241, partial η2 = 0.08. The interaction between group and congruency was significant, F (3, 54) = 2.78, p = 0.050, partial η2 = 0.13. Simple effect analyses revealed that the P2 amplitudes elicited by congruent targets (M = 2.56 μV) were significantly more positive than those elicited by incongruent targets (M = 1.20 μV; p = 0.002), but only for the LW-LC social groups (see Figure 3).
N400. Similar to the statistical analyses of the P2 component, analyses of the N400 component were conducted on mean ERP amplitudes of the given ROI (FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2) in the 300–500 ms time-window. Analysis of N400 mean amplitudes revealed a significant main effect of group, F (3, 54) = 9.23, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.34, as well as a significant main effect of congruency, F (1, 18) = 164.93, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.90. Specifically, the N400 amplitude elicited by incongruent targets (M = −0.69 μV) was significantly more negative than that elicited by congruent targets (M = 1.74 μV). Moreover, the interaction between group and congruency was also significant, F (3, 54) = 2.79, p = 0.049, partial η2 = 0.13.
Simple effect analyses for the group × congruency interaction confirmed the main effect of congruency. As illustrated in Figure 4, the N400 amplitudes elicited by incongruent targets was significantly more negative than those elicited by congruent targets, for all four quadrants (for HW-HC quadrant: M incongruent = −0.06 μV, M congruent = 2.36 μV, MD incongruent-congruent = −2.42 μV, SE = 0.37, p < 0.001; for HW-LC quadrant: M incongruent = −1.43 μV, M congruent = 0.32 μV, MD incongruent-congruent = −1.75 μV, SE = 0.37, p < 0.001; for LW-HC quadrant: M incongruent = 0.20 μV, M congruent = 2.28 μV; MD incongruent-congruent = −2.08 μV, SE = 0.59, p = 0.002; for LW-LC quadrant: M incongruent = −1.48 μV, M congruent = 2.02 μV, MD incongruent-congruent = −3.49 μV, SE = 0.36, p < 0.001). However, as presented in the Figure 4, this tendency seems more obvious for the LW-LC quadrant.
To further test the discrepancies on N400 component induced by different warmth-competence social groups, difference waves were calculated (subtracted the N400 amplitude elicited by congruent trials from the N400 amplitude elicited by incongruent trials within each social group) and analyzed using a one-way repeated measure ANOVA with group (HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-LC, LW-HC) as a within-subject factor. Results revealed a significant main effect for group, F (3, 54) = 2.79, p = 0.049, partial η2 = 0.13. Multiple comparisons showed that the N400 difference waves elicited by LW-LC social groups were the largest (M = −3.49 μV) and were significantly more negative than those elicited by the HW-LC social groups (M = −1.75 μV; p = 0.031). The LW-LC N400 difference waves were also more negative than those elicited by HW-HC social groups (M = −2.42 μV; p = 0.363) and LW-HC social groups (M = −2.08 μV; p = 0.233), but differences did not reach significance. No other comparisons between social groups approached significance (ps > 0.775).
LPC. Similar to the statistical analyses of the P2 and N400 component, analyses of the LPC were conducted on mean ERP amplitudes of the given ROI (F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2) in the 500-700 ms time-window. The analysis of LPC amplitudes revealed a significant main effect of group [F (3, 54) = 8.34, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.32] and congruency [F (1, 18) = 19.24, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.52]. Moreover, the interaction between group and congruency was significant as well, F (3, 54) = 2.88, p = 0.044, partial η2 = 0.14.
As presented in Figure 5, simple effect analyses for the interaction showed that LPC amplitudes elicited by congruent targets were significantly more positive than those elicited by incongruent targets for the LW-LC groups (M congruent = 5.32 μV, M incongruent = 2.45 μV, MD congruent-incongruent = 2.87 μV, SE = 0.59, p < 0.001) and LW-HC groups (M congruent = 6.45 μV, M incongruent = 4.51 μV, MD congruent-incongruent = 2.04 μV, SE = 0.65, p = 0.006). While the LPC amplitudes elicited by congruent targets were also more positive than those elicited by incongruent targets for the HW-HC (M congruent = 5.73 μV, M incongruent = 4.92 μV, MD congruent-incongruent = 0.81 μV, SE = 0.63, p = 0.216) and HW-LC (M congruent = 4.14 μV, M incongruent = 3.37 μV, MD congruent-incongruent = 0.77 μV, SE = 0.69, p = 0.280) social groups, these differences failed to reach significant.

3.3. Discussion

Predictions for Study 2 were largely supported. Participants responded more quickly on stereotype congruent trials than incongruent trials for HW-HC, HW-LC, and LW-HC social groups. Replicating the results from Study 1, an opposite RT pattern was observed for LW-LC groups. Specifically, participants responded more quickly on incongruent than congruent trials for these groups. This highlights that LW-LC groups are perceived and processed differently than social groups from the other three SCM clusters. Results from the ERP data further support this conclusion. Replicating previous research [39,40,41,42,67,83], stereotype incongruent trials elicited more negative N400 amplitudes than congruent trials for all SCM quadrants. Extending this research and confirming predictions, the N400 effect was most pronounced for the LW-LC groups. Additionally, larger P2 amplitudes were elicited by stereotype congruent trials than incongruent trials, but only for the LW-LC groups, and LPC amplitudes were larger on congruent trials than incongruent trials, but only for the LW-LC and LW-HC groups.

4. General Discussion

The present research had two primary goals: (1) expand the investigation of stereotype activation/priming to social groups that represent all clusters under the framework of SCM, and (2) use event-related potentials (ERPs) to elucidate the timing and nature of stereotypical processing for these groups. A sequential priming paradigm was used to investigate stereotype activation in response times (Studies 1 and 2) and ERPs (Study 2) for a variety of social groups under the framework of SCM. Across both studies, response times revealed typical stereotype priming effects (faster RTs on stereotype congruent than incongruent trials) for HW-HC, HW-LC, and LW-HC social groups while a reverse priming pattern (slower RTs on congruent than incongruent trials) was observed for LW-LC social groups. ERP results from Study 2 further highlighted the unique processing of LW-LC groups. Amplitudes were larger on stereotype congruent than incongruent trials for P2 components, but only for the LW-LC groups. N400 amplitudes were larger on stereotype incongruent than congruent trials for all four SCM quadrants, while this difference on N400 was largest for LW-LC groups. Finally, LPC amplitudes were larger on stereotype congruent than incongruent trials, but only for the low warmth (LW-LC and LW-HC) groups. These results reveal a great deal about the way various SCM groups are processed, and provide further evidence that LW-LC quadrant are processed differently than other three SCM quadrants.
The unique status of LW-LC groups is evident very early in the stream of cognitive processing. P2 amplitudes elicited by stereotype congruent trials were greater than those elicited by stereotype incongruent trials, but only on trials that used a LW-LC group as the prime. Previous research demonstrates that larger P2 amplitudes are associated with perceptions of threatening and/or negative stimuli [78,98,99]. The heightened P2 amplitudes elicited on congruent trials for LW-LC groups thus indicates that these groups attract greater attention because they are perceived as more negative and/or threatening than other groups.
Additional evidence that LW-LC groups are processed uniquely was observed further down in the cognitive stream, in both N400 and LPC amplitudes. The N400 component is thought to reflect the dynamic activation of associations in long-term memory, in the sense that N400 amplitudes elicited by a given stimulus will be smaller when the associations typically activated by that stimulus are already partially or fully activated by other stimuli or experiences [81]. Applied to the current studies, exposure to a social category (prime stimulus) activates information about this group in long-term memory. When the category is followed by a stereotype congruent target, this pre-activation of information facilitates processing of the target stimulus, resulting in a smaller N400 amplitude than if the target stimulus is stereotypically incongruent with the primed social group. This was the case for all SCM groups in the present research, but the effect was particularly pronounced for the LW-LC groups. More specifically, the amplitudes of N400 difference waves for LW-LC groups are larger than those for the other SCM groups. This suggests that the semantic integration for LW-LC groups is harder than for the other groups. As a whole, these results replicate previous N400 stereotype research [39,40,41,67,83] and extend it in two important ways—(1) they demonstrate that the N400 can be used to examine stereotypes for a wide range of social groups under the framework of SCM, and (2) they add to existing evidence for the unique status of LW-LC groups.
While the N400 results were expected, it was somewhat surprising that congruency also modulated LPC amplitudes for LW-LC and LW-HC groups. Specifically, congruent trials elicited larger LPC amplitudes than incongruent trials for low warmth (LW) groups. A potential explanation for these results comes from research that has connected the LPC to avoidance tendencies. In previous research, the LPC has been linked to the regulation of approach-avoidance tendencies, with larger amplitudes accompanying avoidance tendencies than approach tendencies [100]. Heightened LPC amplitudes on congruent trials for LW-LC and LW-HC groups may therefore reflect a general tendency to avoid LW groups. This aligns with previous research where approach–avoidance tendencies have been connected to warmth judgments (e.g., [101]). It also makes sense within the theoretical framework of the SCM. According to the SCM, the warmth dimension is prioritized over the competence dimension (receiving both earlier attention and greater weight) due to the evolutionary importance of discerning good or ill intentions in others [20]. This could explain why congruency modulated the LPC for both LW-LC and LW-HC groups—both groups are perceived primarily in terms of their warmth, and avoidance tendencies are correspondingly activated. The activation of avoidance tendencies might also explain the behavioral results observed in the present research.
The stream of unique processing for LW-LC groups culminate in a very distinct pattern for response times (RTs). Specifically, we observed typical RT priming effects for HW-HC, HW-LC, and LW-HC groups, but an opposite pattern (longer RTs on congruent trials than incongruent trials) for LW-LC groups. This reverse priming effect for LW-LC groups has been replicated in two additional experiments in Yang et al. [87], and reverse priming effects, in general, have been observed in a variety of conditions in past research. For example, reverse priming effects have been reported when a long SOA is used, when each trial contains two primes, when instructions emphasize accuracy over speed, and when primes are extreme (for review, see [102]). Of these conditions, only the condition employing extreme primes applies to the present research. Specifically, the LW-LC groups may be perceived as extreme with regard to their low warmth and low competence, relative to the other groups used as primes. Their extremity would cause these groups to stand out and subtly alert participants to their potentially biasing influence on responses [103]. This may, in turn, activate an unconscious goal to correct for this bias [103] (for review, see [104]), resulting in slowed responses on congruent trials. Considering the difference on P2 found on LW-LC groups, P2 might also be an indicator of unconscious activation of egalitarian goal or unconscious activation of motivation to correct bias [104,105,106,107]. However, this possibility needs further empirical support.
Alternatively, the reverse priming effect may reflect an avoidance behavioral tendency toward these groups, particularly since these groups elicit the emotion of disgust [12]. Previous research using disgust facial cues suggests that disgust strongly elicits avoidance tendencies, which ultimately diverts, rather than attracts, attention to stimuli [108,109]. In this line of research, dot-probe judgments were faster on validly cued trials than invalidly cued trials when fearful or angry facial cues were used. However, when disgust facial cues were used, judgments were slower on validly cued trials. In other words, the RT pattern was reversed on disgust facial cue trials [108,109]. Applied to the present research, LW-LC congruent trials might elicit disgust and attendant avoidance tendencies, significantly slowing responses on congruent trials relative to incongruent trials. Because disgust is not the dominant emotion elicited by other SCM groups [12], it would not interfere with responses on congruent trials for these groups. If the activation of disgust and avoidance explains the reversed RT pattern, we would see a significant slow-down on congruent trials for LW-LC groups, but much less or no difference in RTs between LW-LC and other SCM groups for incongruent trials. The difference in RTs between LW-LC and other SCM groups for incongruent trials was non-significant (Study 1) or relatively small (Study 2), meaning that participants responded to incongruent trials fairly similarly for all SCM groups. However, RTs were significantly longer for LW-LC groups for congruent trials in both studies, meaning that participants responded much more slowly on congruent trials for LW-LC groups than for the other SCM groups.
One final possible explanation for the reverse priming effect involves distrust. According to the SCM and BIAS map, LW-LC groups can easily become a target of distrust [31,110,111], and previous research has found that distrust can facilitate processing of inconsistent information while inhibiting the processing of congruent information (e.g., [112]; for review, see [113]). Additionally, other research has found that distrust primarily affects stereotyping by reducing the application of stereotypes, as opposed to reducing the activation of stereotypes [114]. This could explain why a reverse-priming effect was observed in response times, but the anticipated pattern was observed for LW-LC groups for the N400 component. All these potential explanations, however, are speculative and require direct testing in future studies.
Taken as a whole, the results from the present research suggest that LW-LC groups may attract additional attention early in processing (as evidenced in P2 activity), but ultimately attention is repelled away from them as processing culminates in avoidance behavioral tendencies. Although share some common neural underpinnings (temporoparietal junction and right anterior temporal lobe [31]), results from the present research also highlight some important differences between LW-LC groups and LW-HC groups. LW-LC groups were immediately perceived as more negative or threatening (P2), activated avoidance predispositions (LPC), and ultimate elicited avoidance behavioral responses (RTs). LW-HC groups also appeared to activate avoidance tendencies (LPC), but they did not attract early attention, were not immediately perceived negatively, and did not elicit avoidance behavioral responses. According to the BIAS map [12], LW-LC groups elicit the emotion of disgust and both active and passive forms of harmful behavior (directed toward members of these groups). The BIAS map does not identify any general conditions when these groups would elicit facilitating (helpful/friendly) behavior from others. These groups may therefore be automatically and unconditionally perceived negatively, activating avoidance predispositions that are translated into action. This would explain their differential processing from perception to behavior in the present research.
Because of their mixed stereotype content (low warmth but high competence), LW-HC groups elicit the more ambiguous emotion of envy and either passive facilitation behavior or active harm behaviors. The BIAS map proposes that the type of behavior LW-HC groups elicit (facilitation or harm) depends on the situation and circumstances. In times of stability and success, people will passively facilitate (cooperate) with these groups because their high status makes it advantageous to do so. In times of instability and misfortune, however, people can turn on these groups as scapegoats and may actively try to harm them [12]. LW-HC groups would therefore not be automatically and unconditionally perceived negatively, hence the absence of differential processing of these groups in P2. Due to the primacy of the warmth dimension, it is reasonable to assume that LW-HC groups would elicit avoidance tendency, hence differential processing in the LPC occurred. However, absent any clear signals of instability or misfortune, it would be more advantageous to cooperate with, rather than avoid, these groups in LW-HC quadrant. Thus, these avoidance tendencies were not translated into avoidance behavioral tendencies in the present research. It is possible that an opposite RT pattern would be observed for these groups if signals of instability/misfortune were introduced into the experimental context. This is an interesting question to pursue in future research.
Some limitations and future directions should be mentioned in the current study. First of all, researchers should seek to replicate these results in other populations. Both of the current samples were composed of Chinese undergraduate students. In one sense, this helps improve the overall diversity of samples in psychological research, which tends to skew heavily toward Western cultures. On the other hand, the results may apply specifically to Chinese individuals. We consider this unlikely, though, given that the basic response time and N400 effects observed in the present research replicate past research conducted with Western samples. Even so, replications with other populations and non-student samples would increase confidence in the generalizability of the results. Moreover, the current findings were obtained at the intergroup level. Considered that SCM was also applied in interpersonal perception [21,22,23], it would be interesting to explore the reverse priming effect and its mechanism at the inter-individual level. Recently, a framework [115] was proposed to integrate five different but connected models (including SCM [11], dimensional compensation model [116], dual perspective model [117], behavioral regulation model [118] and agency beliefs-communion model [119]). Four types of moderators (type of target, number of targets, perceiver-target relationship and context) were raised to explain contradicts among these models. The present results were obtained only under the framework of SCM, whether these results could be replicated under the newly integrated framework or other four models still remains unknown and the potential moderating role of these four kinds of moderators on reverse priming effect and traditional priming effect would be a fruitful field in the future. Lastly, the perceived warmth and competence for all four SCM quadrants fit SCM map well when checked individually, however, when validating the groups across SCM categories, the competence dimension did not fit perfectly while warmth dimension was balanced among four SCM quadrants in the current study. Specifically, the difference on competence ratings between LW-HC and HW-LC did not reach significant (p = 0.225) and HW-LC was perceived more competent than LW-LC (p = 0.006), even though they are both labeled as LC quadrants. Moreover, the SCM quadrants identified from pretest were the results of relative placement rather than the ideal absolute placement (see [88] for details). This limitation would be a potential confound and need addressed in future studies.

5. Conclusions

The present research makes several meaningful contributions to the literature. First, as far as we know, this is the one of the first studies to investigate the neural mechanism underlying reverse priming effect. Secondly, it verifies that the N400 ERP component can be used as a measure of stereotype activation for groups within all quadrants of the SCM. Thirdly, it adds to previous research evidence that suggests LW-LC social groups are processed differently than other SCM groups, and extends this research by elucidating the various time points when these groups are uniquely processed. Finally, it highlights the utility of ERPs for differentiating between the various SCM groups with regard to the sequence and timing of their processing. When combined with fMRI data, ERP data can provide a more complete and thorough understanding of the neural processes that give rise to biased/stereotypical processing of, as well as potentially discriminatory behaviors toward, these groups.
The present results demonstrate that the reverse priming effect is robust and exist among various LW-LC social groups. The underlying mechanism might involve early attention attraction (P2), followed by difficulty on semantic integration (N400) and avoidance behavioral tendency (LPC). Future studies are urgently needed to elucidate the moderators of the reverse priming effect and the mechanism underlying it.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.Y. and Q.-W.C.; data curation, Y.Y. and Q.X.; formal analysis, Y.Y. and Q.X.; funding acquisition, Y.Y. and Q.X.; investigation, Y.Y. and Q.X.; methodology, Y.Y. and Q.-W.C.; project administration, Y.Y. and Q.-W.C.; resources, K.R.G.W. and Q.-W.C.; software, Y.Y. and Q.X.; supervision, Y.Y.; validation, Q.X. and Q.-W.C.; visualization, Y.Y.; writing—original draft, Y.Y. and Q.-W.C.; writing—review and editing, Y.Y., K.R.G.W., X.F. and Q.-W.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Science Foundation of Ministry of Education of China (grant number 15YJC190024, 18YJC190027).

Acknowledgments

We thank Werner Sommer from Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin for advice in revising this paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. The prime words and target words for four SCM quadrants.
Table A1. The prime words and target words for four SCM quadrants.
SCM QuadrantsPrime Words
in Chinese
Prime Words
in English
Stereotype Congruent Target Words
in Chinese
Stereotype Congruent Target Words
in English
Stereotype Incongruent Target Words
in Chinese
Stereotype Incongruent Target Words
in English
HW-HC军人soldiers保家卫国protect our homes and defend our country重利益regard interests as highly important
威严dignified欺软怕硬bully the weak and fear the strong
坚毅fortitude瘦弱thin and weak
正义righteous可恶hateful
正直upstanding爱唠叨nagging
消防员firemen工作危险dangerous work喊口号shout slogans
行动迅速act quickly行动缓慢slow movement
灭火救人put out the fire and rescue people不办实事don’t get things done
令人尊敬respectable唯利是图seek nothing but profits
敬业dedicated凶残merciless
勇敢brave无责任心without the sense of responsibility
大学教师professors有学问scholarly狡猾crafty
知识渊博knowledgeable凶残cruel
教书育人imparting knowledge and educating people游手好闲idle
心理咨询师psychotherapists善于观察good at observing没人性inhuman
懂人心empathetic缺少关爱lack of love and care
有耐心have much patience极端go to extremes
温和gentle没有同情心without compassion
帮助他人helping others凶狠fierce
空姐air hostesses礼仪好good manners威严dignified
微笑服务service with a smile残暴brute
有气质graceful颓废decadence
漂亮pretty白发苍苍grey-haired
美丽大方beautiful and gracious心理扭曲psychological distortion
举止优雅elegant manner蛮横rude and unreasonable
瑜伽教练yoga instructors柔韧性强great flexibility精神涣散spiritless
体形优美beautiful shape大腹便便big-bellied
身材好good figure没有上进心no desire to advance
健美well-built邋遢slovenly
健康athleticdirty
HW-LC老年人elderly需要照顾need to be looked after体形优美beautiful shape
需要关爱need love and care柔韧性强great flexibility
爱唠叨nagging身材好good figure
白发苍苍grey-haired残暴brutal
行动缓慢slow movement行动迅速act quickly
慈祥kindly挥霍spend extravagantly
农民farmers辛苦laborious work知识渊博knowledgeable
种田farming私生活混乱promiscuous
勤劳industrious奢侈spend extravagantly
憨厚老实simple and honest 懒惰lazy
收入低low-income官腔官调bureaucratic
家庭主妇housewives做饭带孩子cooking and taking care of children留学study abroad
顾家value family保家卫国protect our homes and defend our country
做家务keep house贪污腐败corrupt
贤惠virtuous傲慢arrogant
勤劳industrious形式主义formalism
农民工migrant workers 辛苦laborious work有知识educated
劳累overworked工作清闲work at leisure
生活艰辛life is tough工作稳定stable job
弱势群体vulnerable group官腔official jargon
留守儿童left-behind children渴望陪伴eager to be accompanied by parents见识广well-informed
缺少关爱lack of love and care有钱wealthy
孤独lonely美丽大方beautiful and gracious
可怜pitiful外语好good foreign language
清洁工cleaning workers辛苦laborious work有气质graceful
早起early rising有学问scholarly
朴实simple 收入高high income
可敬worthy of respect铁饭碗has a stable lifelong job
勤劳industrious漂亮pretty
工资低low salary待遇好well-paid
LW-HC商人businessmen重利益regard interests as highly important渴望陪伴eager to accompany
有钱wealthy贫穷poor
精明shrewd憨厚老实simple and honest
唯利是图seek nothing but profits做家务keep house
海归overseas returnees留学study abroad监狱prison
外语好good foreign language萎靡flattened
见识广well-informed种田farming
有知识educated工资低low salary
公务员 government workers工作稳定stable job绯闻多need more concern
铁饭碗has a stable lifelong job需要照顾need to be looked after
工作清闲work at leisure堕落decadent
待遇好well-paid工作危险dangerous work
政府官员government officials官腔official jargon有耐心have much patience
形式主义formalism敬业dedicated
官腔官调bureaucratic孤独lonely
喊口号shout slogans弱势群体vulnerable group
不办实事don’t get things done朴实simple
贪污腐败corrupt做饭带孩子cooking and taking care of children
大腹便便big-bellied骨瘦如柴skinny
演艺明星stars in showbiz收入高high income无所事事slothful
绯闻多more scandal啃老depend on parents for living
光鲜亮丽glamorous衣衫褴褛shabbily dressed
漂亮pretty收入低low-income
私生活混乱promiscuous灭火救人put out the fire and rescue people
好看good-looking形容枯槁haggard
富人rich有钱wealthy生活艰辛life is tough
奢侈extravagant需要关爱need love and care
傲慢arrogant劳累overworked
狡猾crafty可怜pitiful
挥霍spend extravagantly辛苦laborious work
LW-LC罪犯criminals监狱prison慈祥kindly
可恶hateful令人尊敬respectable
凶狠fierce正义righteous
残暴violent and cruel 教书育人imparting knowledge and educating people
无业游民unemployed游手好闲idle辛苦laborious work
无所事事slothful早起early bird
无责任心irresponsible好看good-looking
啃老depend on parents for living勤劳industrious
没有上进心no desire to advance勇敢courageous
乞丐beggarsdirty健美well-built
衣衫褴褛shabbily dressed光鲜亮丽glamorous
邋遢slovenly有钱wealthy
懒惰lazy漂亮pretty
贫穷poor勤劳industrious
吸毒者drug addicts颓废decadence健康fitness
堕落decadent辛苦laborious work
萎靡flattened勤劳industrious
精神涣散spiritless善于观察good at observing
骨瘦如柴skinny坚毅fortitude
形容枯槁haggard精明shrewd
瘦弱thin and weak顾家value family
恐怖分子terrorists极端go to extremes帮助他人helping others
残暴brutal微笑服务service with a smile
凶残cruel正直upstanding
心理扭曲psychological distortion举止优雅elegant manner
没人性inhuman礼仪好good manners
城管urban management officers欺软怕硬bully the weak and fear the strong温和gentle
蛮横rude and unreasonable懂人心empathetic
凶残merciless 贤惠virtuous
没有同情心without compassion可敬worthy of respect

References

  1. Macrae, C.N.; Bodenhausen, G.V. Social cognition: Thinking categorically about others. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2000, 51, 93–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Ellemers, N. Gender stereotypes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018, 69, 275–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Blair, I.V.; Banaji, M.R. Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 1142–1163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Devine, P.G. Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 56, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Macrae, C.N.; Milne, A.B.; Bodenhausen, G.V. Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 66, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Oakhill, J.; Garnham, A.; Reynolds, D. Immediate activation of stereotypical gender information. Mem. Cognit. 2005, 33, 972–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Amodio, D.M.; Cikara, M. The Social Neuroscience of Prejudice. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Quadflieg, S.; Macrae, C.N. Stereotypes and stereotyping: What’s the brain got to do with it? Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 22, 215–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Amodio, D.M. The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 670–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Blair, I.V. The Malleability of Automatic Stereotypes and Prejudice. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2002, 6, 242–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Fiske, S.T.; Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P.; Xu, J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 82, 878–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cuddy, A.J.C.; Fiske, S.T.; Glick, P. The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 631–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Fiske, S.T.; Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P. Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Fiske, S.T. Intergroup biases: A focus on stereotype content. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 3, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Fiske, S.T. Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 27, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Cuddy, A.J.C.; Fiske, S.T.; Kwan, V.S.Y.; Glick, P.; Demoulin, S.; Leyens, J.P.; Bond, M.H.; Croizet, J.C.; Ellemers, N.; Sleebos, E. Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 48, 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Wu, S.J.; Bai, X.; Fiske, S.T. Admired rich or resented rich? How two cultures vary in envy. J. Cross Cultur. Psychol. 2018, 49, 1114–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bye, H.H.; Herrebrøden, H.; Hjetland, G.J.; Røyset, G.Ø.; Westby, L.L. Stereotypes of Norwegian social groups. Scand. J. Psychol. 2014, 55, 469–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Grigoryev, D.; Fiske, S.T.; Batkhina, A. Mapping ethnic stereotypes and their antecedents in Russia: The stereotype content model. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Cuddy, A.J.C.; Fiske, S.T.; Glick, P. Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 40, 61–149. [Google Scholar]
  21. Russell, A.M.T.; Fiske, S.T. It’s all relative: Competition and status drive interpersonal perception. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 38, 1193–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Simon, J.C.; Styczynski, N.; Gutsell, J.N. Social perceptions of warmth and competence influence behavioral intentions and neural processing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 20, 265–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Abele, A.E.; Yzerbyt, V. Body posture and interpersonal perception in a dyadic interaction: A Big Two Analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Dehumanizing the lowest of the low neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 847–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Social groups that elicit disgust are differentially processed in mPFC. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Social neuroscience evidence for dehumanised perception. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 20, 192–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Haslam, N.; Loughnan, S. Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014, 65, 399–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Krendl, A.C. An fMRI investigation of the effects of culture on evaluations of stigmatized individuals. NeuroImage 2016, 124, 336–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cikara, M.; Farnsworth, R.A.; Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. On the wrong side of the trolley track: Neural correlates of relative social valuation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2010, 5, 404–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dricu, M.; Bührer, S.; Hesse, F.; Eder, C.; Posada, A.; Aue, T. Warmth and competence predict overoptimistic beliefs for out-group but not in-group members. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e207670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Dricu, M.; Schüpbach, L.; Bristle, M.; Wiest, R.; Moser, D.A.; Aue, T. Group membership dictates the neural correlates of social optimism biases. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Moser, D.A.; Dricu, M.; Wiest, R.; Schüpbach, L.; Aue, T. Social optimism biases are associated with cortical thickness. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2020, 15, 745–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Kawakami, K.; Young, H.; Dovidio, J.F. Automatic stereotyping: Category, trait, and behavioral activations. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 28, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Macrae, C.N.; Bodenhausen, G.V.; Milne, A.B. The dissection of selection in person perception: Inhibitory processes in social stereotyping. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 397–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Pendry, L.F.; Macrae, C.N. What the disinterested perceiver overlooks: Goal-directed social categorization. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 22, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sinclair, L.; Kunda, Z. Reactions to a black professional: Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 885–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kidder, C.K.; White, K.R.; Hinojos, M.R.; Sandoval, M.; Crites, S.L. Sequential stereotype priming: A meta-analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 22, 199–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. White, K.R.; Danek, R.H.; Herring, D.R.; Taylor, J.H.; Crites, S.L. Taking priming to task: Variations in stereotype priming effects across participant task. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 49, 29–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Wang, P.; Yang, Y.; Zhao, L. The activation of stereotypes: Behavioral and ERPs evidence. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2010, 42, 607–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. White, K.R.; Crites, S.L.; Taylor, J.H.; Corral, G. Wait, what? Assessing stereotype incongruities using the N400 ERP component. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2009, 4, 191–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Wang, P.; Yang, Y.; Tan, C.; Chen, Q.; Van Cantfort, T.E. Gender stereotype activation versus lexical semantic activation: An ERP study. J. Gen. Psychol. 2017, 144, 283–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Yang, Y.; Wang, P.; Yin, Z.; Chen, Q.; Feng, X. The pattern and neural correlates of unintentional stereotype activation. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2015, 47, 488–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Zhang, X.; Li, Q.; Sun, S.; Zuo, B. The time course from gender categorization to gender stereotype activation. Soc. Neurosci. 2018, 13, 52–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Plaza, M.; Boiché, J.; Brunel, L.; Ruchaud, F. Sport = male but not all sports: Investigating the gender stereotypes of sport activities at the explicit and implicit levels. Sex Roles 2017, 76, 202–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tsamadi, D.; Falbén, J.K.; Persson, L.M.; Golubickis, M.; Caughey, S.; Sahin, B.; Macrae, C.N. Stereotype-based priming without stereotype activation: A tale of two priming tasks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2020, 73, 1939–1948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Müller, F.; Rothermund, K. What does it take to activate stereotypes? Simple primes don’t seem enough: A replication of stereotype activation (Banaji & Hardin, 1996; Blair & Banaji, 1996). Soc. Psychol. 2014, 45, 187–193. [Google Scholar]
  47. Banaji, M.R.; Hardin, C.D. Automatic stereotyping. Psychol. Sci. 1996, 7, 136–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Kimura, A.; Wada, Y.; Goto, S.; Tsuzuki, D.; Cai, D.; Oka, T.; Dan, I. Implicit gender-based food stereotypes. Semantic priming experiments on young Japanese. Appetite 2009, 52, 521–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Macrae, C.N.; Cloutier, J. A matter of design: Priming context and person perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 1012–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Macrae, C.N.; Martin, D. A boy primed Sue: Feature-based processing and person construal. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 37, 793–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Macrae, C.N.; Mitchell, J.P.; Pendry, L.F. What’s in a forename? Cue familiarity and stereotypical thinking. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 38, 186–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Gawronski, B.; Deutsch, R.; Mbirkou, S.; Seibt, B.; Strack, F. When “just say no” is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 370–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kawakami, K.; Dovidio, J.F. The reliability of implicit stereotyping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 212–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Todd, A.R.; Thiem, K.C.; Neel, R. Does seeing faces of young black boys facilitate the identification of threatening stimuli? Psychol. Sci. 2016, 27, 384–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Ito, T.A.; Tomelleri, S. Seeing is not stereotyping: The functional independence of categorization and stereotype activation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017, 12, 758–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Wittenbrink, B.; Judd, C.M.; Park, B. Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 72, 262–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Wittenbrink, B.; Judd, C.M.; Park, B. Evaluative versus conceptual judgments in automatic stereotyping and prejudice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 37, 244–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Amodio, D.M.; Harmon-Jones, E.; Devine, P.G.; Curtin, J.J.; Hartley, S.L.; Covert, A.E. Neural signals for the detection of unintentional race bias. Psychol. Sci. 2004, 15, 88–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Dickter, C.L.; Kittel, J.A. The effect of stereotypical primes on the neural processing of racially ambiguous faces. Soc. Neurosci. 2012, 7, 622–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Govorun, O.; Payne, B.K. Ego—Depletion and prejudice: Separating automatic and controlled components. Soc. Cogn. 2006, 24, 111–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Jones, C.R.; Fazio, R.H. Person categorization and automatic racial stereotyping effects on weapon identification. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 36, 1073–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Kawakami, K.; Dion, K.L.; Dovidio, J.F. Implicit stereotyping and prejudice and the primed Stroop task. Swiss J. Psychol. 1999, 58, 241–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Kubota, J.T.; Ito, T.A. The role of expression and race in weapons identification. Emotion 2014, 14, 1115–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  64. Payne, B.K. Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 81, 181–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Payne, B.K. Conceptualizing control in social cognition: How executive functioning modulates the expression of automatic stereotyping. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 89, 488–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Stewart, B.D.; Payne, B.K. Bringing automatic stereotyping under control: Implementation intentions as efficient means of thought control. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 34, 1332–1345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Hehman, E.; Volpert, H.I.; Simons, R.F. The N400 as an index of racial stereotype accessibility. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 544–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Freeman, J.B.; Ambady, N. A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 118, 247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Kawakami, K.; Amodio, D.M.; Hugenberg, K. Intergroup perception and cognition: An integrative framework for understanding the causes and consequences of social categorization. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2017; Volume 55, pp. 1–80. [Google Scholar]
  70. Krieglmeyer, R.; Sherman, J.W. Disentangling stereotype activation and stereotype application in the stereotype misperception task. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 103, 205–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Jia, L.; Dickter, C.L.; Luo, J.; Xiao, X.; Yang, Q.; Lei, M.; Qiu, J.; Zhang, Q. Different brain mechanisms between stereotype activation and application: Evidence from an ERP study. Int. J. Psychol. 2012, 47, 58–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Amodio, D.M.; Bartholow, B.D.; Ito, T.A. Tracking the dynamics of the social brain: ERP approaches for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 385–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Ito, T.A.; Kubota, J.T. Bioelectrical echoes from a career at the cutting edge: John Cacioppo’s legacy and the use of ERPs in social psychology. Soc. Neurosci. 2019, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Wang, P.; Yang, Y.; Tan, C.; Zhao, X.; Liu, Y.; Lin, C. Stereotype activation is unintentional: Behavioural and event-related potenials evidence. Int. J. Psychol. 2016, 51, 156–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Pesciarelli, F.; Scorolli, C.; Cacciari, C. Neural correlates of the implicit processing of grammatical and stereotypical gender violations: A masked and unmasked priming study. Biol. Psychol. 2019, 146, 107714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Luck, S.J.; Hillyard, S.A. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology 1994, 31, 291–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Dickter, C.L.; Bartholow, B.D. Racial ingroup and outgroup attention biases revealed by event-related brain potentials. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Ito, T.A.; Urland, G.R. Race and gender on the brain: Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 616–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Ito, T.A.; Urland, G.R. The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces: An ERP study of race and gender perception. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2005, 5, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Willadsen-Jensen, E.C.; Ito, T.A. Ambiguity and the timecourse of racial perception. Soc. Cogn. 2006, 24, 580–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Kutas, M.; Federmeier, K.D. Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 621–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Bentin, S.; McCarthy, G.; Wood, C.C. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1985, 60, 343–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Wang, L.; Ma, Q.; Song, Z.; Shi, Y.; Wang, Y.; Pfotenhauer, L. N400 and the activation of prejudice against rural migrant workers in China. Brain Res. 2011, 1375, 103–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Osterhout, L.; Bersick, M.; McLaughlin, J. Brain potentials reflect violations of gender stereotypes. Mem. Cognit. 1997, 25, 273–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  85. Hammer, A.; Jansma, B.M.; Lamers, M.; Münte, T.F. Pronominal reference in sentences about persons or things: An electrophysiological approach. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 227–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Xu, X.; Jiang, X.; Zhou, X. Processing biological gender and number information during Chinese pronoun resolution: ERP evidence for functional differentiation. Brain Cogn. 2013, 81, 223–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Yang, Y.; Xu, Q.; Zhu, T.; Zheng, X.; Dong, X.; Chen, Q. The behavioral patterns of stereotype activation among four different warmth-competence social groups based on Stereotype Content Model. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2019, 51, 1143–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Ji, Z.; Chen, Q.; Fan, X.; Xu, Q.; Yang, Y. Stereotypes of Social Groups in Mainland China in Terms of Warmth and Competence: Evidence from a Large Undergraduate Sample. 2020. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/dbh3v/ (accessed on 26 October 2020).
  89. Katz, D.; Braly, K. Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1933, 28, 280–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Ratcliff, R. Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 114, 510–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Bartholow, B.D. On the role of conflict and control in social cognition: Event-related brain potential investigations. Psychophysiology 2010, 47, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Wentura, D.; Degner, J. A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 95–116. [Google Scholar]
  93. Conrey, F.R.; Sherman, J.W.; Gawronski, B.; Hugenberg, K.; Groom, C.J. Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 89, 469–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Hütter, M.; Klauer, K.C. Applying processing trees in social psychology. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 27, 116–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Johnson, D.J.; Hopwood, C.J.; Cesario, J.; Pleskac, T.J. Advancing research on cognitive processes in social and personality psychology: A hierarchical drift diffusion model primer. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2017, 8, 413–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Gratton, G.; Coles, M.G.; Donchin, E. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1983, 55, 468–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Keil, A.; Debener, S.; Gratton, G.; Junghöfer, M.; Kappenman, E.S.; Luck, S.J.; Luu, P.; Miller, G.A.; Yee, C.M. Committee report: Publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. Psychophysiology 2014, 51, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Carretié, L.; Martín-Loeches, M.; Hinojosa, J.A.; Mercado, F. Emotion and attention interaction studied through event-related potentials. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2001, 13, 1109–1128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Eimer, M.; Holmes, A.; McGlone, F.P. The role of spatial attention in the processing of facial expression: An ERP study of rapid brain responses to six basic emotions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2003, 3, 97–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Bamford, S.; Broyd, S.J.; Benikos, N.; Ward, R.; Wiersema, J.R.; Sonuga-Barke, E. The late positive potential: A neural marker of the regulation of emotion-based approach-avoidance actions? Biol. Psychol. 2015, 105, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Peeters, G. From good and bad to can and must: Subjective necessity of acts associated with positively and negatively valued stimuli. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 32, 125–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Klauer, K.C.; Teige-Mocigemba, S.; Spruyt, A. Contrast effects in spontaneous evaluations: A psychophysical account. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 96, 265–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Glaser, J.; Banaji, M.R. When fair is foul and foul is fair: Reverse priming in automatic evaluation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 669–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Moskowitz, G.B.; Ignarri, C. Implicit volition and stereotype control. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 20, 97–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Moskowitz, G.B.; Gollwitzer, P.M.; Wasel, W.; Schaal, B. Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egalitarian goals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 167–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Moskowitz, G.B. On the Control over Stereotype Activation and Stereotype Inhibition. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2010, 4, 140–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Moskowitz, G.B.; Salomon, A.R.; Taylor, C.M. Preconsciously Controlling Stereotyping: Implicitly Activated Egalitarian Goals Prevent the Activation of Stereotypes. Soc. Cogn. 2000, 18, 151–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Liu, Y.; Zhang, D.; Luo, Y. How disgust facilitates avoidance: An ERP study on attention modulation by threats. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015, 10, 598–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Zhang, D.; Liu, Y.; Wang, L.; Ai, H.; Luo, Y. Mechanisms for attentional modulation by threatening emotions of fear, anger, and disgust. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2017, 17, 198–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Fiske, S.T. Warmth and competence: Stereotype content issues for clinicians and researchers. Can. Psychol. 2012, 53, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  111. Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P.; Beninger, A. The dynamics of warmth and competence judgments, and their outcomes in organizations. Res. Organ. Behav. 2011, 31, 73–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Schul, Y.; Mayo, R.; Burnstein, E. Encoding under trust and distrust: The spontaneous activation of incongruent cognitions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 668–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  113. Mayo, R. Cognition is a matter of trust: Distrust tunes cognitive processes. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 26, 283–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Posten, A.; Mussweiler, T. When distrust frees your mind: The stereotype-reducing effects of distrust. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 567–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  115. Abele, A.E.; Ellemers, N.; Fiske, S.T.; Koch, A.; Yzerbyt, V. Navigating the social world: Toward an integrated framework for evaluating self, individuals, and groups. Psychol. Rev. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Yzerbyt, V.; Corneille, O. Cognitive Process: Reality Constraints and Integrity Concerns in Social Perception. In On the Nature of Prejudice: Fifty Years after Allport; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 173–191. [Google Scholar]
  117. Abele, A.E.; Wojciszke, B. Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 751–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  118. Leach, C.W.; Ellemers, N.; Barreto, M. Group virtue: The importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 234–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  119. Koch, A.; Imhoff, R.; Dotsch, R.; Unkelbach, C.; Alves, H. The ABC of stereotypes about groups: Agency/socioeconomic success, conservative–progressive beliefs, and communion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 110, 675–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Example of the sequences of one trial. ITI: inter-trial interval; ISI: inter-stimulus interval.
Figure 1. Example of the sequences of one trial. ITI: inter-trial interval; ISI: inter-stimulus interval.
Brainsci 10 01001 g001
Figure 2. Behavioral results for Study 1 and Study 2. (A) Accuracy in Study 1; (B) mean reaction times in Study 1 (n = 58); (C) accuracy in Study 2; (D) mean reaction times in Study 2 (n = 19). Error bars indicate ± 1 standard error of the mean. RT: reaction time. SE: standard error. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Figure 2. Behavioral results for Study 1 and Study 2. (A) Accuracy in Study 1; (B) mean reaction times in Study 1 (n = 58); (C) accuracy in Study 2; (D) mean reaction times in Study 2 (n = 19). Error bars indicate ± 1 standard error of the mean. RT: reaction time. SE: standard error. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Brainsci 10 01001 g002
Figure 3. The grand-average event-related brain potentials (ERPs) at selected regions of interest (ROI) electrodes (Fz, FCz, Cz); the shaded areas mark P2 components (150–250 ms). Topographies of P2 difference waves (incongruent minus congruent trials) for four stereotype content module (SCM) quadrants.
Figure 3. The grand-average event-related brain potentials (ERPs) at selected regions of interest (ROI) electrodes (Fz, FCz, Cz); the shaded areas mark P2 components (150–250 ms). Topographies of P2 difference waves (incongruent minus congruent trials) for four stereotype content module (SCM) quadrants.
Brainsci 10 01001 g003
Figure 4. The grand-average ERPs at selected ROI electrodes (FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2); the shaded areas mark N400 components (300–500 ms). Topographies of N400 difference waves (incongruent minus congruent trials) for four SCM quadrants.
Figure 4. The grand-average ERPs at selected ROI electrodes (FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2); the shaded areas mark N400 components (300–500 ms). Topographies of N400 difference waves (incongruent minus congruent trials) for four SCM quadrants.
Brainsci 10 01001 g004
Figure 5. The grand-average ERPs at selected ROI electrodes (F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2); the shaded areas mark LPC components (500–700 ms). Topographies of late positive component (LPC) difference waves (incongruent minus congruent trials) for four SCM quadrants.
Figure 5. The grand-average ERPs at selected ROI electrodes (F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2); the shaded areas mark LPC components (500–700 ms). Topographies of late positive component (LPC) difference waves (incongruent minus congruent trials) for four SCM quadrants.
Brainsci 10 01001 g005
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yang, Y.; White, K.R.G.; Fan, X.; Xu, Q.; Chen, Q.-W. Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 1001. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001

AMA Style

Yang Y, White KRG, Fan X, Xu Q, Chen Q-W. Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(12):1001. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yang, Yaping, Katherine R. G. White, Xinfang Fan, Qiang Xu, and Qing-Wei Chen. 2020. "Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample" Brain Sciences 10, no. 12: 1001. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop