Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 12 March 2024
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Developmental Trajectories of Early Life Trauma View all 3 articles

A network analysis of ICD-11 Complex PTSD, emotional processing, and dissociative experiences in the context of psychological trauma at different developmental stages

Zahra MohammadiZahra Mohammadi1Mahmood Dehghani*Mahmood Dehghani1*Fahimeh Fathali LavasaniFahimeh Fathali Lavasani1Hojjatollah FarahaniHojjatollah Farahani2Ahmad AshouriAhmad Ashouri1
  • 1Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Behavioral Sciences and Mental Health (Tehran Institute of Psychiatry), Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
  • 2Department of Psychology, Tarbiat Modares University, Tehran, Iran

Objective: Traumatic experiences are a significant risk factor for psychological disturbances, including disorders such as complex posttraumatic stress disorder, emotion-processing problems, and trauma-related dissociative experiences. The present investigation examined the coexistence of these symptoms using a network analysis model.

Method: This study included a sample of 406 people referred to comprehensive health centers in Tehran from September to December 2023 with psychopathological syndromes. Variables were assessed using The International Trauma Questionnaire, International Measurement of Exposure to Traumatic Event checklist, Baker Emotional Processing Questionnaire, and Dissociative Experiences. A regularized partial correlation network and Glasso algorithm, in combination with Extended Bayesian information criteria, were applied to estimate the network structure.

Results: Signs of unprocessed emotions and disturbance in self-organization symptoms were the most important symptoms in the symptom network, forming strong connections with other nodes. Thereby, these two symptoms can be regarded as the most important clinical manifestations in the symptom network following traumatic experiences. Three distinct symptom communities were identified: the community of traumatic experiences (childhood, adolescence, adulthood), the community of dissociative experiences (amnesia, depersonalization/derealization, and absorption), and the community of emotional processing (suppression, unpleasant emotional experience, Signs of unprocessed emotions, avoidance, and emotional control, posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and disturbance in self-organization symptoms). The strongest edges observed were between childhood trauma-adolescence trauma (0.473) in the community of traumatic experiences, between amnesia and depersonalization/derealization (0.644) in the community of dissociative experiences, and between disturbance in self-organization symptoms and unprocessed emotions (0.324) in the community of emotional processing, indicating the recurrent occurrence of these symptoms.

Conclusion: In this study, disturbance in self-organization symptoms was identified as the central psychopathologic symptom in individuals experiencing traumas at different developmental stages. It seems that adolescent trauma and not childhood trauma plays a more decisive role in the symptoms that a person manifests after traumatic experiences. Also, posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and disturbance in self-organization symptoms were recognized in the cluster of emotional processing symptoms and can have substantial roles in prioritizing therapeutic measures.

1 Introduction

Prolonged, repetitive, interpersonal trauma in children (1, 2), mainly inflicted by caregivers (3, 4), with no possibility for escape due to physical, psychological, maturational, environmental, or social constraints, usually show more complex symptoms than usual PTSD injuries (1, 2). According to the 11th edition of the World Health Organization’s International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11), complex PTSD comprises six symptom clusters: the three PTSD criteria of avoidance of the trauma reminders, disturbing experiences, feelings of current threat, in addition to three disturbances of self-organization (DSO) symptoms defined as affect dysregulation (heightened emotional reactivity to minor stressors or emotional numbing), negative self-concept (such as feelings of failure or worthlessness, inadequacy, shame, and loss of self-awareness), and disturbances in relationships (such as difficulty in establishing and maintaining interpersonal relationships or avoiding relationships, isolation or exclusion) (5, 6). This disorder, which was first proposed by Judith Herman (7) and then conceptualized as complex developmental trauma by Van Der Kolk (8), refers to the unique consequences of complex traumas and maltreatment in children. Diagnostic modalities that fail to consider the history of psychological traumas and victimization in a person often lead to unsuccessful treatments (9).

Maltreatment is conceptualized as “Failure of the Average Expectable Environment.” For children, the Expectable environment includes a supportive and loving caregiver within a larger community where the child socializes. For older children, the Expectable environment consists of a supportive family, peer groups, and continuous opportunities, allowing them to be energetic (10). Early life traumas substantially hurdle the nurturing of security and trust in interpersonal relationships with attachment figures and effectively invoke coping strategies to manage and reanalyze the trauma. This phenomenon can predispose the child to the formation of insecure attachments, as a result of this diminishing the internal sense of security or external support in the face of traumas and subsequent undesirable sequences in life, paving the ground for future traumas and the development of psychological disorders (11). Abusive parents compromise their children’s ability to perceive and communicate their abstract emotional experiences. Unsuitable communication with abusive parents can lead the child to internalize these early insecure attachments, which adversely affects the development of children’s mentalizing capacities. It is believed that the relationship between insecure attachment and lack of mentalizing is maintained during adulthood, and grownups with insecure attachment are identified with insecure attachment representations to the accessibility of attachment figures. Fizke et al. found that activating insecure attachment representations could significantly reduce mentalizing capacities, causing an individual to fail to perceive others’ emotions (12). Studies on defects in the emotional processing of people with developmental trauma indicate that the severity of the trauma strongly and positively correlates with emotional awareness (the ability to perceive and pay attention to others’ emotional experiences), emotional acceptance (the ability to experience negative emotions without invoking secondary negative emotions), emotional transparency (the ability to understand others’ emotional experiences) and impulse control (the degree of behavioral control during emotional distress) (1316).

Moreover, adults with a history of traumatic events experience intrusive memories related to the event in various forms, such as emotional numbing and attempts to avoid reminders of the trauma. In these conditions, the dissociation mechanism can act as a psychological defense mechanism for an abused child whose defense capacity has been broken by traumatic events (17, 18). Dissociation helps the person abandon such events or at least keep them far away to become emotionally unavailable (18). Although dissociation has not been defined as a core criterion of CPTSD, symptoms such as reliving the trauma in the here and now (part of the re-experiencing cluster) and emotional numbing (part of the affective dysregulation cluster) fall into this category (4, 19, 20). The relationship between dissociation and trauma is clinically significant (21). Trauma-related dissociation is a biological response to a stressful event in which the victim finds himself in a completely helpless situation, to which the body responds by stopping processing perceptual, cognitive, and emotional information (22). From an evolutionary perspective, dissociation-induced silencing may be considered an adaptive response to life-threatening conditions during which fight-and-flight responses are inadequate for survival. In such a situation, as self-defense or escape is impossible, the person can only minimize the damage by remaining silent (23). Deactivation of sensory and functional systems may occur again in the future in response to cues related to trauma and minor stressors (22). Dissociation is a cognitive avoidance mechanism that reduces intense negative emotions, such as fear, and elicits autonomic arousal (24). Dissociation also involves a significant distortion of attention by shutting down perceptual information processing, which top-down cortical-subcortical processes can mediate at the level of functional neural activity (25).

In psychopathology, clinicians are more interested in categorical rather than dimensional approaches where symptoms and signs do not occur randomly, and some change more than others. However, there are disagreements on its mechanism. The neo-Kraepelin categorical perspective states that signs and symptoms are clustered because they share a common cause. This highlights the importance of underlying and hidden pathologies. The opposite approach states that latent dimensions cause syndromic clustering. In this model, mental disorders are the main origins of disease symptoms. The network perspective is significantly different from both traditional approaches (26). The network approach to psychopathology started a decade ago with a simple hypothesis: symptoms may present as a syndrome nurtured by causal relationships between themselves (27, 28). From this perspective, symptoms are not merely the passive indicators of a “common hidden cause” but the elements of a causal system (27, 29, 30). This hypothesis has led to several theoretical, methodological, and experimental works based on the idea that mental disorders can be described as complex systems in which symptoms play an active causal role (31, 32). Therefore, latent factor models in psychopathology have been widely criticized in the last decade. In latent factor models, symptoms are considered passive receptors for common background factors independent of their environment (26). So far, atomistic approaches have been influential in psychology, in which actors are usually pictured regardless of the behaviors of other players. Therefore, such individualistic descriptions disregard context and other components (33).

The network approach is an attractive alternative for causal models in psychopathology, proposing that psychological disorders can be conceptualized as a network system consisting of interconnected elements (nodes) where changes in one node can lead to changes in other nodes or the whole network (34) In this approach, the unique/conditional connections between nodes are called edges (35). The structure of relationships between nodes and the position of nodes in the network are essential attitudinal, perceptual, and behavioral consequences for individual units and the system as a whole (33). The density of the network or the ratio of existing nodes reflects the degree to which the activation of a symptom may activate other components of the symptom network (36). A method employing centrality measures was used to examine the relative effects of these unique symptoms on the entire network, which shows that a symptom with high centrality plays an important role in the etiology and maintenance of a psychiatric disorder. This can have important therapeutic implications (37, 38). Network analysis studies conducted in this field so far have addressed the pattern of mutual relationships between CPTSD symptoms and emotional processing problems (39), anxiety and depression symptoms (40), borderline personality disorder symptoms (41), traumas in different developmental stages (42, 43) and PTSD and CPTSD symptoms (4346). Another study investigated network relationships between cumulative trauma of childhood, CPTSD, dissociation, depression, and emotional regulation (39). The recent study only assessed the role of childhood trauma; however, we found no study on network relationships between CPTSD, emotional processing, dissociative experiences, and traumas in different developmental stages. Based on the existing research gap, our research had three main objectives: 1- Examining the role of childhood trauma vs. adolescence and adulthood traumas in the formation of the symptom network as a risk factor, 2- Evaluating symptom clustering in the network, and 3- Exploring relationships between symptoms, seeking the central symptom that interconnects various clusters, and describing the main symptoms in the network and their interrelationships.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants and procedures

The present research was a descriptive-cross-sectional study conducted in comprehensive health centers (CHC) in Tehran affiliated with the Iran University of Medical Sciences from September to December 2023. The physician referred patients with psychiatric symptoms to the researcher who attended the CHC. Inclusion criteria were (1) Age between 20 and 50 years, (2) the ability to read and write in Persian, (3) the absence of psychotic symptoms, (4) the absence of bipolar disorder, and (5) the absence of brain injury and substance use disorder. People who did not meet these criteria were excluded. The individuals who expressed their willingness to participate in the research were offered to sign an informed consent form enclosing information about the study’s objectives, voluntary participation, and data confidentiality assurance. After obtaining written informed consent, the screening questions (which had already been checked by the physician in the comprehensive health centers) were again administered to ensure that participants met the study’s inclusion criteria.

Regarding inclusion criteria, 406 patients were evaluated, and their data were analyzed. This research was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Iran University of Medical Sciences (ethics code: IR.IUMS.REC.1402.454). The procedures used in this study adhere to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

2.2 Measure

2.2.1 International trauma questionnaire

The International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ) is an 18-item self-reported measure to assess ICD-11 PTSD and CPTSD. This questionnaire is scored on a 5-point Likert scale from 0(not at all) to 4(extremely) with a categorical approach for diagnosis and a dimensional approach for rating the severity of symptoms. For PTSD diagnosis, the individual responses to items 1 to 9 are evaluated. For CPTSD, beyond the requirement for PTSD diagnosis (i.e., items 1 to 9), the required scores related to disturbed self-organization (i.e., items 10 to 18) should also be obtained. To rate the severity of symptoms on a score range from 0 to 24, scores from questions 1 to 6 are used to assess the severity of PTSD symptoms, and those related to questions 10 to 15 are utilized to evaluate the severity of disturbed self-organization (DSO) (47). Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for internal consistency revealed values >0.77 for all subscales, except for the avoidance dimension, with a value of 0.67, indicating the acceptable validity of almost all subscales (>0.79) (48). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha of the ITQ score was 0.93.

2.2.2 International trauma exposure measure

The International Measurement of Exposure to a Traumatic Event (ITEM) checklist includes 21 life-threatening events that a person has experienced at different stages of development (childhood, adolescence, adulthood). Of these, 16 events match the definition of trauma exposure by DSM-5 (i.e., direct and indirect threats to life or sexual and physical safety), and the other five events encompass psychological traumas according to the ICD-11 trauma exposure criteria. The scoring varies depending on the objective. The total score of childhood traumas can be calculated by adding up all events before the age of 12 years, and this can be accomplished for adolescence and adulthood traumas by gathering together the events happening between 13 and 18 years old and beyond 18 years old, respectively. Finally, the overall score for traumatic events in a lifetime can be calculated by summing the scores of all events in each developmental stage. In addition, this checklist can designate the most disturbing traumatic event (i.e., the trauma index), as well as the frequency and time of its happening (49, 50).

2.2.3 Emotional processing scale

Baker’s Emotional Processing Scale (EPS) is a 25-item self-reported questionnaire to assess emotional processing styles. Each item of this scale is scored on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (completely agree) to 4 (completely disagree), measuring the five subscales of suppression, unpleasant emotional experience, signs of unprocessed emotions, avoidance, and emotional control. The total score is calculated by dividing the sum score obtained by the participant by the total number of questions, and the same method is used to calculate the score of each subscale (i.e., the total score obtained in each subscale divided by the number of its items). The EPS’s Cronbach’s alpha and retest coefficients have been reported as 0.92 and 0.79, respectively (51, 52). In Iran, EPS’s Cronbach’s alpha coefficient has been reported as 0.95 (53). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha of the EPS score was 0.91.

2.2.4 Dissociative experiences scale

The dissociative experiences scale (DES) is a 28-item self-reported scale used to evaluate depersonalization/derealization, amnesia, and absorption. This scale assesses dissociative (but not disordered) symptoms on a Likert scale from never (0) to always (100). The total score is calculated as the sum of the participants’ scores divided by the number of questions. The score of the subscales is calculated as the sum of the participants’ scores divided by the total number of questions related to each subscale. Cronbach’s alpha of the scale was reported as 0.95, and the test-retest reliability coefficient ranged from 0.84 to 0.96 (54). In Iran, Cronbach’s alpha of this scale has been reported as 0.92 (55). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha of the DES score was 0.97.

2.3 Data analysis

2.3.1 Descriptive statistic

At first, the basic R software package (base R) was used to describe the data. In this section, demographic variables (age, sex, education level, and marital status) and clinical variables (mean and standard deviation of the scales) were described.

2.3.2 Network estimation

We estimated the network via a Graphical Gaussian Model (GGM), in which the nodes are the observed variables interconnected through non-directional edges (i.e., the connections specify no specific direction), representing partial correlation coefficients between two variables after controlling for other variables in the network. GGM estimates many parameters that likely result in some false positive edges. Therefore, it is expected to regularize GGM via the graphical LASSO. The LASSO graphic algorithm was visualized through the estimateNetwork function using the qgraph package, called by the bootnet package in R software. In this algorithm, small connections between pairs of nodes (partial correlation coefficients) reach zero, rendering an uncondensed network to a network with a condensed structure (56, 57). The Glasso algorithm is regulated using the hyperparameter gamma (g) in combination with Extended Bayesian information criteria (EBIC) (58). The hyperparameter controls the trade-off between including possible false-positive edges and removing true edges in the final network.

We chose a conservative value of g=0.5 to drive EBIC, which prefers a dense network structure with few edges. The bootnet package automatically estimates this method in qgraph using the default EBICglasso. In the resultant network, the edges between the nodes represent the conditional independent relationships between the nodes or, more specifically, the partial correlation between pairs of nodes that control the influence of all other nodes (56). We use the LASSO algorithm with the EBICglasso method for network estimation for the following reasons: 1) both the graphical LASSO algorithm and the EBICglasso method are known for their ability to control for false positives in network estimation. This is crucial in psychopathology research to ensure that the inferred connections between symptoms or psychological constructs are meaningful and not spurious (59); 2) these methods are designed to estimate the relationships between variables, assuming that certain variables are conditionally dependent on each other in the network. Understanding these conditional dependencies can gain insight into the complex interplay between symptoms and psychological processes in psychopathology (56); 3) the graphical LASSO algorithm and the EBICglasso method naturally induce sparsity in the estimated networks, which is often desirable in psychopathology research. Sparse networks can help identify the most relevant connections among symptoms or psychological constructs, leading to more interpretable and actionable insights. In simpler terms, sparse networks make understanding the relationships between different aspects of mental health easier and can provide helpful information for developing effective treatments (57). The displayed graph has specific characteristics; nodes are depicted as circles, and edges/partial correlations are shown as lines connecting the circles. Nodes that are associated with thicker edges represent stronger relationships. The direction of node connections was indicated by color; positive relationships between edges were shown in blue, and negative relationships were shown in red. Also, the size of the nodes is determined based on the frequency in the raw data, and the higher the frequency of a node, the larger its corresponding size. This layout was based on the Fruchterman–Reingold algorithm, which places the nodes with more interconnectedness near each other and toward the center of the network (60).

Exploratory graph analysis (EGA), offered by the EGAnet package in R software, was used to investigate if all three variables were members of a single community or if each formed a separate one. EGAnet uses the Louvain community detection algorithm, which can perform better than Spinglass and Walktrap. The structure of identified communities was analyzed using standardized node strength (61), which can be interpreted with the same exploratory factor analysis loading matrix method. However, community loadings are much smaller than the loadings of a classical factor analysis matrix because they show partial correlations. Effect sizes of 0.10, 0.30, and 0.50 were suggested to interpret these loadings, indicating small, medium, and large effects, respectively (62).

We estimated centrality measures in the qgraph package to identify important nodes. Depending on the definition of “importance,” each node may be considered significant from different perspectives. Different centrality measures are sensitive to relationships between a focal unit and other units. In this study, we decided to calculate the strength index and a new index of centrality, known as “expected influence,” which is more relevant and stronger for psychopathology networks than other indicators (56, 63). Expected influence (EI) is characterized by the total number of connections of a node and shows the importance of that node in the network. This is of relative importance because, even in networks with low overall edge weights, there will always be a node with a strong expected influence (64). The expected influence equals the sum of non-absolute values of edge weights/partial correlations directly connected to a node (65). They can be expressed as the active potential of the node across the network of CPTSD symptoms, dissociative symptoms, and emotional processing (26).

2.3.3 Stability and accuracy analysis

Three methods were used to evaluate the accuracy and stability of the network model. First, the accuracy of edge weights was estimated by calculating confidence intervals (CIs) with the Non-parametric bootstrap method. Then, the original data set was randomly resampled to create new data sets, in which 95% CIs were calculated. Sorting the average of the bootstrapped edge weights determines whether the edge weight of the entire sample matches the bootstrap sample (56). Second, the correlation stability coefficient (CS-C) was calculated to evaluate the stability of EI using subset bootstraps (56, 66). CS-C measures the maximum proportion of cases that can be excluded so that the correlation can reach a specific value with 95% confidence. CS-C should generally not be less than 0.25 and preferably above 0.5. Third, bootstrap difference tests were performed to evaluate differences in network characteristics (67). This test was performed based on 95% CI to determine whether the centrality indices of two edge weights or two nodes are significantly different. The bootstrap method and overall stability were tested using the bootnet package.

3 Results

3.1 Sample descriptive

Out of 406 evaluated individuals, 249 (61.3%) fell into the age range of 20 to 25 years. Two hundred forty of the participants were women (59.1%). Half of the participants were single (57.6%), and 156 (38.4%) had a bachelor’s degree. The average number of experienced traumas based on the International Traumatic Event Exposure Measurement Checklist (ITEM) was 4 in childhood, 3 in adolescence, and 4 in adulthood. The mean and standard deviation of the scores of all scales were as follows: PTSD (M=8.78, SD=5.31), DSO (M=11.45, SD=6.69), EPS (M=81.82, SD=16.55), and DES (M=65.50, SD=58.06).

3.2 The network estimation

The graphical LASSO algorithm results of network analysis of CPTSD symptoms, symptoms of dissociative experiences, emotional processing, and traumatic experiences in different developmental stages are displayed in Figure 1. The centrality indices showed that the most central symptoms of the network with the highest EIs included unpleasant emotional experience (EI=1.20) and DSO (EI=1.10), which had the highest EI and were, therefore, stronger than other nodes. In other words, these symptoms had a statistically significant effect on the network and played an important role in the symptoms a person experiences after traumatic experiences. Thus, the centrality estimates show an interconnected network in which UE and DSO were significant in direct and indirect paths. Also, the least important node in the network was adulthood trauma (EI=0.47) (Figure 2). The strongest edges identified between the nodes of dissociative amnesia and depersonalization/derealization (0.644), childhood trauma and adolescent trauma (0.473), depersonalization/derealization and absorption (0.344), and DSO and signs of unprocessed emotions (0.324). There was also a negative node between adolescent trauma and avoidance (-0.228) (Supplementary Figure 3, Supplementary Table 1, Supplementary Material).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Gaussian graphical model. Nodes (circles) are scores of PTSD, DSO, EPS, DES, and traumatic events. Undirected edges, indicated by lines between nodes, represent partial correlations between variables after controlling for relationships with all other nodes. Blue lines between the circles indicate positive conditional associations; the single negative conditional association is represented as a red line (between AT and Look AV). PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; DSO, disturbances in self-organization; EC, Emotional Control; UE, Unpleasant Emotional Experiences; UPE, Signs of Unprocessed Emotional Symptoms; S, Suppression; AV, Avoidance; DE, Depersonalization/Derealization; AM, Amnesia; AB, Absorption; CT, Childhood trauma; AT, Adolescent trauma; ADT, Adulthood trauma.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Showing centrality scores for all variables in the network.

The results indicated excellent accuracy and stability of the network; the correlation stability coefficient was above 0.50 (Strength=0.749, EI=0.749), which means that EI and strength were still related to the original data after removing 75% of the data (r=0.749) (Supplementary Figures 1, 5, Supplementary Material). The results of the non-parametric bootstrap demonstrated the lack of overlap in the 95% CIs of the bootstrap, indicating high accuracy (Supplementary Figure 2, Supplementary Material).

The EGA algorithm identified three distinct symptom communities, each potentially representing a different underlying structure. The first community includes three subscales of traumatic experiences (childhood, adolescence, adulthood), the second community consists of three subscales of dissociative experiences (amnesia, depersonalization or derealization, and absorption), and Finally, five subscales of emotional processing (suppression, unpleasant emotional experience, signs of unprocessed emotion symptoms, avoidance, and emotional control), PTSD, and DSO formed a separate community. The strongest edges in the communities of traumatic experiences, dissociative experiences, and emotional processing were observed between childhood traumas-adolescent traumas (0.473), amnesia-depersonalization/derealization (0.644), and DSO-unprocessed emotions (0.324), respectively, indicating the comorbidity of these symptoms. However, each symptom was directly related to at least one symptom from another community. Therefore, almost all symptoms were associated with DSO; visually, no symptoms appeared separately in the network.

4 Discussion

To our knowledge, our research is the first study that used a network approach to explore the interrelationships between CPTSD, emotional processing, and dissociative experiences after traumatic experiences in different developmental stages. Therefore, our study has an exploratory nature. The overall structure of the network revealed that the EBICglasso method has produced a dense network based on which we can see that childhood trauma has a strong direct relationship with adolescent trauma, adolescent trauma has more strength than childhood and adulthood trauma, and more edges connected to it. One justification for this observation may be explained by the psychoanalytic theory’s “deferred action” concept. The subject revises past traumatic experiences in the future, and it is this revision that gives them the importance and even effectiveness of pathogenic force. The memories that fall under the deferred action concept are suppressed painful traumas that manifest as symptoms (68). Another reason is that people with trauma in the early stages of life are more likely to experience revictimization in the later stages of life (adolescence and adulthood). In explaining the relationship between childhood maltreatment and revictimization in later life, psychoanalytic theory has proposed that people who have survived childhood maltreatment will unconsciously experience posttraumatic events so that they can better manage the traumatic event and its ensued occurrences, a phenomenon known as repetition compulsion (69). Other theories have explained revictimization with different mechanisms. For example, betrayal trauma theory suggests that dissociative amnesia is an underlying mechanism for revictimization (70). When a child suffers maltreatment by a caregiver, the betrayal cannot be effectively processed by avoiding the abuser’s interaction because the child needs the abusive caregiver for physical and psychological survival. Therefore, dissociation in this situation is an adaptive response that contributes to the attachment between the caregiver and the child. However, dissociation, which continues as a habit into adulthood, may interfere with information processing, including recognizing danger cues in similar interpersonal situations, leading to the risk of revictimization. Betrayal trauma theory hypothesizes that the lack of access to past information due to dissociation compromises risk recognition in future life, subsequently facilitating revictimization (71). One of the most significant results of the network was that not all symptoms were equally important in the network structure, highlighting the value of communication between nodes without emphasizing a latent underlying factor. Looking more closely at the relative importance of nodes, considering that many strong edges go to them, UE and DSO can be regarded as the most important nodes in the network. In network analysis, strength refers to the importance or influence of a node in the network, which can also be checked through weighted connections (72). When a node in the network has high strength, it can act as a bridge and communicational facilitator between different parts of the network, allowing for more control over the network flow and for the node to shape information distribution in the network, as well as its behavior and dynamics (73). Network models allow us to see how symptoms directly interact with each other in the overall network structure. So, interventions targeting the DSO and UE nodes can trigger a cascade effecting on different nodes, allowing clinicians to control dissociation and PTSD symptoms by modulating these nodes that are integrated with the emotion-processing community. Consistent with this finding, posttraumatic dissociation can be regarded as a protective mechanism against unprocessed emotions (74) in a manner that the problem in emotional processing can be considered as a node between dissociative experiences and DSO symptoms (75). These nodes can have cross effects on different types of symptoms, and considering that emotional dysregulation is one of the dimensions of DSO, it can be noted that the cluster of DSO symptoms probably plays an important role in keeping dissociative symptoms and processing problems together. This result is consistent with the results obtained from the EGA algorithm, revealing three communities of interconnected symptoms: traumatic experiences, dissociative experiences, and emotional processing. This is one of the exciting findings of the present study because PTSD and DSO fall into the emotional processing community, which could help prioritize therapeutic programs for individuals presenting with PTSD and DSO symptoms. This community displays undefined boundaries with significant connections between symptoms of different communities. Based on our findings in this research, traumatic experiences, dissociative experiences, and emotional processing can all contribute to the formation of the more general class of DSO, which is supported by previous reports (1, 20, 76).

5 Clinical implications

Our results show the central role of DSO and the importance of adolescent trauma in the network of symptoms. Understanding the central role of DSO in the symptom network of individuals with CPTSD highlights the importance of targeting interventions that specifically address disturbances in self-regulation, affect dysregulation, and interpersonal difficulties. Therapeutic modalities can be particularly beneficial in addressing these core features of CPTSD. Also, Psychological interventions for trauma-exposed adolescents that address DSO symptoms in addition to trauma-specific symptoms (e.g., nightmares; avoidance) may be crucial. This includes interventions to reduce risk behaviors, attachment-based interventions aimed at repairing disrupted attachment patterns, fostering secure attachments, and preventing recurrent trauma, given the negative impact of multiple traumatic events on children and adolescents.

6 Limitations

One of the strengths of the present study was the use of network analysis as an innovative method to create new clinical insights into CPTSD symptoms, emotional processing, and dissociative experiences after traumatic experiences in different developmental stages. The network analysis approach in psychopathology has many advantages, including 1) Network analysis provides a robust framework for exploring complex interrelationships among psychological variables, offering insights into how symptoms, behaviors, or traits influence each other within a system (35, 77, 78); 2) It provides a visual representation of relationships, facilitating the identification of central nodes, clusters, and pathways. This information can inform intervention strategies and theoretical models (35, 64, 77); 3) Network analysis can identify critical players or influential nodes in a system, which can help researchers and clinicians focus on specific targets for intervention (35, 77, 78). While network analysis is a valuable approach, past network studies of psychopathology have certain limitations that need to be addressed in future research. Most network studies in psychopathology, including our research, have used cross-sectional designs, limiting the ability to make causal inferences. Although longitudinal network studies may identify predictive relationships, causal inferences cannot be inferred from network analyses without experimental designs (37, 79). Network analyses in our research have found support for DSO symptoms being key nodes within network structures. However, these network studies have been predominantly limited to self-reported symptoms indicated by a single item, raising concerns about the reliability of symptom measurement within these networks. Moreover, network analyses have been criticized for their generalizability (i.e., the convergence of results across similar samples) (57, 80) and stability (i.e., the replicability of results across randomly selected subsamples) (79), which includes issues related to overfitting networks to particular datasets, the accuracy of parameter estimation, and the degree to which network parameters are influenced by sampling variation (57, 67, 80). In summary, while network analysis offers invaluable insights into the interplay of psychological variables, researchers must acknowledge its inherent limitations, particularly regarding data interpretation, cross-sectional designs, and causal inferences. By adopting a cautious and nuanced approach, researchers can take advantage of the strengths of network analysis while addressing potential challenges in psychological research. In addition to the limitations of network analysis, the other limitation was that this study was conducted on a non-clinical population, so it is advisable to conduct similar studies on clinical and vulnerable populations to increase the generalizability of the results.

7 Future directions

Despite some limitations, the findings of this study provide valuable insights into critical areas for future research. Throughout the network literature, there has been substantial discussion about the importance of highly central symptoms as prime targets for interventions with the potential to reduce psychopathology significantly. However, further research is required to assess the effectiveness of focused interventions in disrupting network structures by targeting these domains. It is crucial to acknowledge that central symptoms may be challenging to treat due to their extensive connectivity within the network, as activating any connected symptom could reactivate and maintain the central symptom. Therefore, clinicians will likely need to address central symptoms and identify and target key symptom chains and clusters in which central symptoms are embedded. Furthermore, it remains uncertain whether highly central symptoms are the most impairing aspects of psychopathology networks. In the context of our study, it may be that although DSO symptoms are highly connected within the network structure, trauma-related dissociative experiences may be more disruptive to an individual’s ‘functioning and quality of life. Additionally, due to the cross-sectional nature of the current network structures, it is still being determined to what extent nodes with high centrality temporally influence other symptoms or whether such symptoms result from other causal symptom chains. The study suggests that adapting network approaches at the individual level may provide clinicians with a more precise understanding of personal symptom relations and allow more tailored interventions. In summary, while the study identifies promising directions for future research and intervention strategies, it also underscores the need for further investigation into the complexities of central symptoms within psychopathology networks and the development of more individualized approaches to intervention.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Iran University of Medical Sciences ethics committee, Tehran, Iran. The approval number is (IR.IUMS.REC.1402.454). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

ZM: Conceptualization, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft. MD: Conceptualization, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. FFL: Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. HF: Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing. AA: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors appreciate all the people who participated in the current study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1372620/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Giourou E, Skokou M, Andrew SP, Alexopoulou K, Gourzis P, Jelastopulu E. Complex posttraumatic stress disorder: The need to consolidate a distinct clinical syndrome or to reevaluate features of psychiatric disorders following interpersonal trauma? World J Psychiatry. (2018) 8:12–9. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v8.i1.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Cloitre M, Courtois C, Ford J, Green B, Alexander P, Briere J, et al. The ISTSS expert consensus treatment guidelines for complex PTSD in adults. (2012). Available online at: www.istssorg/ISTSS_Main/media/Documents/ISTSS-Expert-Concesnsus-Guidelines-for-Complex-PTSD-Updated-060315 (Accessed 10 January 2020).

Google Scholar

3. Rogel A. The impact of neurofeedback on children and adults with developmental trauma: two random-control studies. Int J Psychophysiology. (2021) 168:S83–S4. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.07.259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Organization WH. International classification of diseases and related health problems, 11th revision. (2018). Available at: http://wwwwhoint/classifications/apps/icd/icd10online.

Google Scholar

5. Powers A, Petri JM, Sleep C, Mekawi Y, Lathan EC, Shebuski K, et al. complex PTSD, and borderline personality disorder using exploratory structural equation modeling in a trauma-exposed urban sample. J Anxiety Disorder. (2022) 88:102558. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2022.102558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Zepinic V. Complex Trauma Syndrome: Concept and Therapy. London: Austin Macauley Publishing (2021).

Google Scholar

7. Herman JL. Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. J Traumatic Stress. (1992) 5:377–91. doi: 10.1007/BF00977235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Van der Kolk B. The John Bowlby Memorial Lecture 2006. Developmental trauma disorder: a new, rational diagnosis for children with complex trauma histories 1. In: Trauma and attachment. London: Routledge (2018). p. 45–60.

Google Scholar

9. Lewis C, Roberts NP, Andrew M, Starling E, Bisson JI. Psychological therapies for post-traumatic stress disorder in adults: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2020) 11:1729633. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2020.1729633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Parritz RH, Troy MF. Disorders of childhood. In: Development and psychopathology. Boston, Massachusetts: Cengage learning (2013).

Google Scholar

11. Huang YL, Fonagy P, Feigenbaum J, Montague PR, Nolte T, Consortium MDR. Multidirectional pathways between attachment, mentalizing, and posttraumatic stress symptomatology in the context of childhood trauma. Psychopathology. (2020) 53:48–58. doi: 10.1159/000506406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Fizke E, Buchheim A, Juen F. Activation of the attachment system and mentalization in depressive and healthy individuals: An experimental control study. Psihologija. (2013) 46:161–76. doi: 10.2298/PSI1302161F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Frewen PA, Dozois DJ, Neufeld RW, Lanius RA. Disturbances of emotional awareness and expression in posttraumatic stress disorder: Meta-mood, emotion regulation, mindfulness, and interference of emotional expressiveness. psychol Trauma: Theory Research Practice Policy. (2012) 4:152. doi: 10.1037/a0023114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Weiss NH, Tull MT, Davis LT, Dehon EE, Fulton JJ, Gratz KL. Examining the association between emotion regulation difficulties and probable posttraumatic stress disorder within a sample of African Americans. Cogn Behav Ther. (2012) 41:5–14. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2011.621970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Short NA, Norr AM, Mathes BM, Oglesby ME, Schmidt NB. An examination of the specific associations between facets of difficulties in emotion regulation and posttraumatic stress symptom clusters. Cogn Ther Res. (2016) 40:783–91. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9787-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Pugach CP, Campbell AA, Wisco BE. Emotion regulation in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Rumination accounts for the association between emotion regulation difficulties and PTSD severity. J Clin Psychol. (2020) 76:508–25. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ensink K, Bégin M, Normandin L, Godbout N, Fonagy P. Mentalization and dissociation in the context of trauma: Implications for child psychopathology. J Trauma Dissociation. (2017) 18:11–30. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2016.1172536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Chu JA. Rebuilding shattered lives. In: Treating complex PTSD and dissociative disorders. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons (2011).

Google Scholar

19. Hyland P, Shevlin M, Fyvie C, Cloitre M, Karatzias T. The relationship between ICD-11 PTSD, complex PTSD, and dissociative experiences. J Trauma Dissociation. (2020) 21:62–72. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2019.1675113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Jowett S, Karatzias T, Shevlin M, Hyland P. Psychological trauma at different developmental stages and ICD-11 CPTSD: The role of dissociation. J Trauma Dissociation. (2022) 23:52–67. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2021.1934936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. van der Hart O, Nijenhuis E, Steele K, Brown D. Trauma-related dissociation: conceptual clarity lost and found. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2004) 38:906–14. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2004.01480.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Elbert T, Schauer M. Dissociation following traumatic stress. Z für Psychologie/J Psychol. (2010) 218:109–27. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409/a000018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bracha HS. Freeze, flight, fight, fright, faint: Adaptationist perspectives on the acute stress response spectrum. CNS Spectrums. (2004) 9:679–85. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900001954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Noyes R Jr., Kletti R. Depersonalization in response to life-threatening danger. Compr Psychiatry. (1977) 18:375–84. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(77)90010-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Lanius RA, Vermetten E, Loewenstein RJ, Brand B, Schmahl C, Bremner JD, et al. Emotion modulation in PTSD: Clinical and neurobiological evidence for a dissociative subtype. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:640–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09081168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. McNally RJ. Network analysis of psychopathology: Controversies and challenges. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2021) 17:31–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-092850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Borsboom D. Psychometric perspectives on diagnostic systems. J Clin Psychol. (2008) 64:1089–108. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Cramer AO, Waldorp LJ, van der Maas HL, Borsboom D. Comorbidity: A network perspective. Behav Brain Sci. (2010) 33:137–50. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X09991567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kendler KS. The nature of psychiatric disorders. World Psychiatry. (2016) 15:5–12. doi: 10.1002/wps.20292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Kendler KS, Zachar P, Craver C. What kinds of things are psychiatric disorders? Psychol Med. (2011) 41:1143–50. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Cramer AO, Boschloo L, Schoevers RA, Borsboom D. Mental disorders as networks of problems: a review of recent insights. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2017) 52:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1319-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. McNally RJ. Can network analysis transform psychopathology? Behav Res Ther. (2016) 86:95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Edwards JR, Cable DM, Williamson IO, Lambert LS, Shipp AJ. The phenomenology of fit: linking the person and environment to the subjective experience of person-environment fit. J Appl Psychol. (2006) 91:802. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Contreras A, Nieto I, Valiente C, Espinosa R, Vazquez C. The study of psychopathology from the network analysis perspective: A systematic review. Psychother psychosomatics. (2019) 88:71–83. doi: 10.1159/000497425

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Borsboom D, Cramer AO. Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2013) 9:91–121. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Borsboom D. A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry. (2017) 16:5–13. doi: 10.1002/wps.20375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Robinaugh DJ, Hoekstra RH, Toner ER, Borsboom D. The network approach to psychopathology: a review of the literature 2008–2018 and an agenda for future research. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:353–66. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719003404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Rodebaugh TL, Tonge NA, Piccirillo ML, Fried E, Horenstein A, Morrison AS, et al. Does centrality in a cross-sectional network suggest intervention targets for social anxiety disorder? J consulting Clin Psychol. (2018) 86:831. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000336

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Haselgruber A, Knefel M, Sölva K, Lueger-Schuster B. Foster children's complex psychopathology in the context of cumulative childhood trauma: the interplay of ICD-11 complex PTSD, dissociation, depression, and emotion regulation. J Affect Disord. (2021) 282:372–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Gilbar O. Examining the boundaries between ICD-11 PTSD/CPTSD and depression and anxiety symptoms: A network analysis perspective. J Affect Disord. (2020) 262:429–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Owczarek M, Karatzias T, McElroy E, Hyland P, Cloitre M, Kratzer L, et al. Borderline personality disorder (BPD) and complex posttraumatic stress disorder (CPTSD): A network analysis in a highly traumatized clinical sample. J Pers Disord. (2023) 37:112–29. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2023.37.1.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. McBride O, Hyland P, Murphy J, Elklit A. Network analysis of posttraumatic stress experiences of adults seeking psychological treatment for childhood sexual abuse. J Traumatic Stress. (2020) 33:10–8. doi: 10.1002/jts.22381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Karatzias T, Shevlin M, Hyland P, Ben-Ezra M, Cloitre M, Owkzarek M, et al. The network structure of ICD-11 complex post-traumatic stress disorder across different traumatic life events. World Psychiatry. (2020) 19:400. doi: 10.1002/wps.20795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Knefel M, Karatzias T, Ben-Ezra M, Cloitre M, Lueger-Schuster B, Maercker A. The replicability of ICD-11 complex post-traumatic stress disorder symptom networks in adults. Br J Psychiatry. (2019) 214:361–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2018.286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Knefel M, Lueger-Schuster B, Bisson J, Karatzias T, Kazlauskas E, Roberts NP. A cross-cultural comparison of ICD-11 complex posttraumatic stress disorder symptom networks in Austria, the United Kingdom, and Lithuania. J Traumatic Stress. (2020) 33:41–51. doi: 10.1002/jts.22361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. McElroy E, Shevlin M, Murphy S, Roberts B, Makhashvili N, Javakhishvili J, et al. ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD: Structural validation using network analysis. World Psychiatry. (2019) 18:236. doi: 10.1002/wps.20638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hyland P, Shevlin M, Brewin CR, Cloitre M, Downes A, Jumbe S, et al. Validation of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and complex PTSD using the International Trauma Questionnaire. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. (2017) 136:313–22. doi: 10.1111/acps.12771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Cloitre M, Shevlin M, Brewin CR, Bisson JI, Roberts NP, Maercker A, et al. The International Trauma Questionnaire: Development of a self-report measure of ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. (2018) 138:536–46. doi: 10.1111/acps.12956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Rossi R, Socci V, Pacitti F, Carmassi C, Rossi A, Di Lorenzo G, et al. The Italian version of the international trauma questionnaire: Symptom and network structure of post-traumatic stress disorder and complex post-traumatic stress disorder in a sample of late adolescents exposed to a natural disaster. Front Psychiatry. (2023) 13:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.859877

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hyland P, Karatzias T, Shevlin M, McElroy E, Ben-Ezra M, Cloitre M, et al. Does requiring trauma exposure affect rates of ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD? Implications for DSM–5. psychol Trauma: Theory Research Practice Policy. (2021) 13:133. doi: 10.1037/tra0000908

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Baker R, Thomas S, Thomas PW, Owens M. Development of an emotional processing scale. J Psychosomatic Res. (2007) 62:167–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Baker R, Thomas S, Thomas PW, Gower P, Santonastaso M, Whittlesea A. The Emotional Processing Scale: Scale refinement and abridgment (EPS-25). J Psychosom Res. (2010) 68:83–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lotfi S, Abolghasemi A, Narimani M. A comparison of emotional processing and fear of positive/negative evaluations in women with social phobia and normal women. Knowledge Res Appl Psychol. (2017) 14:101–11.

Google Scholar

54. Bernstein E, Putnam FW, Ross CA, Torem M, Coons P, Dill D, et al. Validity of the Dissociative Experiences Scale in screening for multiple personality disorder: A multicenter study. Am J Psychiatry. (1993) 150:1030–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.7.1030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Kargar Barzi H, Karbala Mohammad Meygooni A, Bramas H, Taghi Loo S. Psychometric properties of dissociative experiences questionnaire. Q Educ Measurement. (2019) 9:115–31.

Google Scholar

56. Epskamp S, Borsboom D, Fried EI. Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behav Res Methods. (2018) 50:195–212. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Sparse inverse covariance estimation with the graphical lasso. Biostatistics. (2008) 9:432–41. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxm045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Chen J, Chen Z. Extended Bayesian information criteria for model selection with large model spaces. Biometrika. (2008) 95:759–71. doi: 10.1093/biomet/asn034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Williams DR, Rast P. Back to the basics: Rethinking partial correlation network methodology. Br J Math Stat Psychol. (2020) 73:187–212. doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Fortunato S. Community detection in graphs. Phys Rep. (2010) 486:75–174. doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2009.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Golino H, Shi D, Christensen AP, Garrido LE, Nieto MD, Sadana R, et al. Investigating the performance of exploratory graph analysis and traditional techniques to identify the number of latent factors: A simulation and tutorial. Psychol Methods. (2020) 25(3):292. doi: 10.1037%2Fmet0000255

Google Scholar

62. Christensen AP, Golino H, Silvia PJ. A psychometric network perspective on the validity and validation of personality trait questionnaires. Eur J Personality. (2020) 34:1095–108. doi: 10.1002/per.2265

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Bringmann LF, Elmer T, Epskamp S, Krause RW, Schoch D, Wichers M, et al. What do centrality measures measure in psychological networks? J Abnormal Psychol. (2019) 128:892. doi: 10.1037/abn0000446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Epskamp S, Cramer AO, Waldorp LJ, Schmittmann VD, Borsboom D. qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J Stat Software. (2012) 48:1–18. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Robinaugh DJ, Millner AJ, McNally RJ. Identifying highly influential nodes in the complicated grief network. J Abnormal Psychol. (2016) 125:747. doi: 10.1037/abn0000181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Costenbader E, Valente TW. The stability of centrality measures when networks are sampled. Soc networks. (2003) 25:283–307. doi: 10.1016/S0378-8733(03)00012-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Epskamp S, Fried EI. A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol Methods. (2018) 23:617. doi: 10.1037/met0000167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Laplanche J, Pontalis J-B. The language of psychoanalysis. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis (1973).

Google Scholar

69. Van der Kolk BA. The compulsion to repeat the trauma: Re-enactment, revictimization, and masochism. Psychiatr Clinics North America. (1989) 12:389–411. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(18)30439-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Freyd JJ, DePrince AP, Gleaves DH. The state of betrayal trauma theory: Reply to McNally—Conceptual issues, and future directions. Memory. (2007) 15:295–311. doi: 10.1080/09658210701256514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Messman-Moore TL, Long PJ. The role of childhood sexual abuse sequelae in the sexual revictimization of women: An empirical review and theoretical reformulation. Clin Psychol Rev. (2003) 23:537–71. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00203-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Lawyer G. Understanding the influence of all nodes in a network. Sci Rep. (2015) 5:8665. doi: 10.1038/srep08665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Gupta U, Trivedi G, Singh D. Chapter Eleven - Human AI: Social network analysis. In: Garg M, Koundal D, editors. Emotional AI and Human-AI Interactions in Social Networking. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Academic Press (2024). p. 213–35.

Google Scholar

74. Grajewski P, Dragan M. Adverse childhood experiences, dissociation, and anxious attachment style as risk factors of gaming disorder. Addictive Behav Rep. (2020) 11:100269. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Ford JD, Courtois CA. Complex PTSD, affect dysregulation, and borderline personality disorder. Borderline Pers Disord Emotion Dysregulation. (2014) 1:1–17. doi: 10.1186/2051-6673-1-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Hyland P, Shevlin M, Brewin CR. The memory and identity theory of ICD-11 complex posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Rev. (2023) 130:1044–65. doi: 10.1037/rev0000418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Bringmann LF, Vissers N, Wichers M, Geschwind N, Kuppens P, Peeters F, et al. A network approach to psychopathology: new insights into clinical longitudinal data. PloS One. (2013) 8:e60188. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Farahani H, Blagojević M, Azadfallah P, Watson P, Esrafilian F, Saljoughi S. An Introduction to Artificial Psychology. In: Application Fuzzy Set Theory and Deep Machine Learning in Psychological Research using R. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Nature (2023). doi: 10.1007/978-3-031-31172-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Fried EI, Cramer AO. Moving forward: Challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2017) 12:999–1020. doi: 10.1177/1745691617705892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Yarkoni T, Westfall J. Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: Lessons from machine learning. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2017) 12:1100–22. doi: 10.1177/1745691617693393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: complex PTSD, emotional processing, dissociative experiences, psychological trauma, network analysis, ICD-11

Citation: Mohammadi Z, Dehghani M, Fathali Lavasani F, Farahani H and Ashouri A (2024) A network analysis of ICD-11 Complex PTSD, emotional processing, and dissociative experiences in the context of psychological trauma at different developmental stages. Front. Psychiatry 15:1372620. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1372620

Received: 18 January 2024; Accepted: 27 February 2024;
Published: 12 March 2024.

Edited by:

Alessandra Maria Passarotti, University of Illinois Chicago, United States

Reviewed by:

Naseem Akhtar Qureshi, Al-Falah University, India
Timea Bezdan, Singidunum University, Serbia

Copyright © 2024 Mohammadi, Dehghani, Fathali Lavasani, Farahani and Ashouri. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mahmood Dehghani, m-dehghani@iums.ac.ir; , mdehghani2004@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.