Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 03 April 2023
Sec. Sleep Disorders

The contribution of transdiagnostic vulnerability factors in patients with chronic insomnia

Mojtaba Habibi Asgarabad,,,,
&#x;Mojtaba Habibi Asgarabad1,2,3,4,5*Hoda Doos Ali Vand
&#x;Hoda Doos Ali Vand6*Pardis Salehi Yegaei&#x;Pardis Salehi Yegaei1Farzaneh Hooman&#x;Farzaneh Hooman7Reza Ahmadi,&#x;Reza Ahmadi1,8Chiara Baglioni,&#x;Chiara Baglioni9,10Shahram Moradi&#x;Shahram Moradi11
  • 1Health Promotion Research Center, Iran University of Medical Science, Tehran, Iran
  • 2Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
  • 3Department of Health Psychology, School of Behavioral Sciences and Mental Health (Tehran Institute of Psychiatry), Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
  • 4Positive Youth Development Lab, Human Development & Family Sciences, Texas Tech University, Texas, TX, United States
  • 5Center of Excellence in Cognitive Neuropsychology, Institute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran
  • 6Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Medicine, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
  • 7Department of Psychology, Ahvaz Branch, Islamic Azad University, Ahvaz, Iran
  • 8Department of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, Zanjan University of Medical Sciences, Zanjan, Iran
  • 9Human Sciences Department, University of Rome Guglielmo Marconi Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 10Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Faculty, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
  • 11Department of Health, Social and Welfare Studies, Faculty of Health and Social Sciences, University of South-Eastern Norway, Porsgrunn, Norway

Introduction: Various transdiagnostic factors have been associated with insomnia severity. The current study aimed to predict insomnia severity based on a group of transdiagnostic factors including neuroticism, emotion regulation, perfectionism, psychological inflexibility, anxiety sensitivity, and repetitive negative thinking after controlling for depression/anxiety symptoms and demographic characteristics.

Methods: Two hundred patients with chronic insomnia disorder were recruited from a sleep disorder clinic. Participants completed the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI), Clinical Perfectionism Questionnaire (CPQ), Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II), Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3), Repetitive Thinking Questionnaire (RTQ-10), Big Five Inventory (BFI-10), Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), and Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21).

Results: After controlling for the confounding variables (depression/anxiety symptoms and demographic characteristics), hierarchical multiple linear regression suggested the significant association of neuroticism (BFI), cognitive reappraisal (ERQ), personal standards (CPQ), evaluative concerns (CPQ), physical concerns (ASI), cognitive concerns (ASI), and repetitive negative thinking (RTQ) with insomnia severity.

Discussion: The findings support the role of transdiagnostic factors, especially physical concerns, repetitive negative thinking, and neuroticism in chronic insomnia. Future research using longitudinal designs is required to verify the causal status of transdiagnostic variables.

1. Introduction

Insomnia refers to the dissatisfaction with sleep quality or quantity, leading to clinically significant distress or impairments in significant areas of functioning (1). Specifically, this disorder pertains to the difficulty maintaining sleep, characterized by frequent awakenings or problems returning to sleep after awakening (1). High rates of comorbidity between chronic insomnia and other medical and psychiatric disorders have been found (2, 3). Two disorders that commonly occur along with insomnia disorder are anxiety and depression. A prospective study on the general population found that insomnia was closely linked with depression and anxiety (4), the result that was replicated in a large body of evidence [(e.g., 5–9)]. As such, the effect of sociodemographic characteristics on insomnia severity has been documented in various studies. For instance, lower education level was shown to be a risk factor for psychological and social stressors and as a result, leads to insomnia (10–12). Insomnia was also found to be more common in women and older adults [(13); (e.g., 10)]. From these evidence, one may conclude that the role of psychological transdiagnostic factors on insomnia severity might be affected or confounded by the individual’s level of depression/anxiety symptoms and demographic characteristics.

In recent years, research and treatment has focused more on the transdiagnostic approach (14). Despite the “disorder-focused” approach that focuses exclusively on a single disorder to understand its etiology and develop its interventional protocol (15), transdiagnostic approach concentrates on factors that are common among comorbid disorders (16). The common transdiagnostic factors that their association with insomnia were well documented include emotion dysregulation (17), neuroticism (18), psychological inflexibility (19), perfectionism (20), anxiety sensitivity (21), and repetitive negative thinking (22).

Maladaptive emotion regulation strategies have been identified as underlying mechanisms in insomnia (23). In their overview of literature, Cerolini et al. (24) found that empirical research highlighted the vital role of emotion dysregulation in the insomnia onset and maintenance, the result that was replicated in another review study (25). Davoodi et al. (26) also investigated the predictor role of various transdiagnostic factors on insomnia, finding that greater difficulties in emotion regulation was a significant contributor to this sleep–wake disorder.

Another transdiagnostic factor implicated in insomnia is neuroticism, also known as negative affectivity (27),which refers to the tendency to experience negative affects, so that people with high neuroticism can hardly maintain their calmness when they are emotionally aroused (28). A study on general population indicated that a greater level of neuroticism in young adults is longitudinally related to the onset of insomnia (29). Gurtman et al. (30) showed that the intensity of insomnia increases with the increase in neuroticism.

Psychological inflexibility can also play a role in the onset and maintenance of insomnia (19). Psychological inflexibility is characterized as “the rigid dominance of psychological reactions over chosen values and contingencies in guiding action” (31). Kato (32) found that psychological inflexibility contributes to insomnia severity, beyond the effect of neuroticism, and could mediate the relation between neuroticism and insomnia severity.

Perfectionism is another transdiagnostic factor associated with insomnia disorder. Maladaptive or clinical perfectionism is characterized by the fear of making mistakes (personal standards), perceiving unrealistic expectations by others and the fear of negative evaluation and rejection by society associated with not being perfect (evaluative concerns) (33). Individuals with chronic insomnia are likely to show more maladaptive perfectionism than healthy controls (34). In addition, greater levels of perfectionism was found to have a positive link with insomnia severity in university students (35).

Another important transdiagnostic factor associated with insomnia is anxiety sensitivity, which refers to the trait-like fear of feelings and sensations related to anxiety (36). A higher level of anxiety sensitivity has been associated with longer sleep onset latency (i.e., longer time taken to fall asleep) (37). The physical concerns dimension of anxiety sensitivity, which refers to the belief that any bodily sensation due to emotional arousal can lead to physical diseases, was shown to have a moderating role in the relationship of sleep anticipatory anxiety with sleep onset latency (38). The cognitive concerns dimension of anxiety sensitivity, defined as the belief that physical symptoms of anxiety lead to serious psychological disorders, was associated with daytime impairments such as fatigue, in people with chronic insomnia (39).

Repetitive negative thinking is another important transdiagnostic factor, present across various disorders, including insomnia (22, 40, 41). Repetitive negative thinking refers to the process in which cognitive representation of one or multiple psychological stressors becomes chronically or repetitively activated (42). Rumination and worry are the two best-known aspects of repetitive thought (43), that have significant associations with polysomnographic indices of disrupted sleep (44), and sleep disturbances, both in healthy people (45), and in insomnia patients (46).

There is clear evidence to support the relationships between the transdiagnostic factors and chronic insomnia (18, 19, 21); however, the extent to which each of these factors can affect insomnia remains unclear. Assessing such transdiagnostic factors together is helpful to estimate the contribution of each of these factors, among others, on chronic insomnia. Hence, the current study aimed to examine the prediction of insomnia severity based on the six candidate transdiagnostic factors including (1) clinical perfectionism, (2) psychological inflexibility, (3) anxiety sensitivity, (4) repetitive negative thinking, (5) neuroticism, and (6) emotion dysregulation, after controlling for the confounding effects of demographic characteristics and depression/anxiety symptoms. Based on established research findings, we hypothesized that the abovementioned transdiagnostic factors would be positively associated with insomnia severity. However, no directional hypothesis was given on the magnitude of the contribution of each factor, since no previous study had included the present variables at the same time in a predictive model of insomnia severity, making it difficult to say which one has a larger weight in association with insomnia.

2. Materials and method

2.1. Participants

Two hundred and seventeen patients with chronic insomnia disorder were recruited from sleep disorders clinic of Rasoul Akram Hospital in Tehran. Inclusion criteria included: (a) a minimum age of 18 years, (b) fluency in Persian language, and (c) meeting the diagnostic criteria for chronic insomnia disorder (such as difficulty initiating or maintaining sleep, early-morning awakening with inability to fall asleep again, and the presence of the sleep disturbance for at least 3 months) based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fifth Edition [DSM-5; (1)]. The exclusion criteria were having: (a) severe mental disorders (e.g., bipolar disorder and psychosis), (b) brain injury, (c) substance abuse, (d) other sleep disorders (e.g., sleep apnea and restless legs syndrome), and (e) a rotating shift work within the last year Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Axis I Disorders (SCID-5) were used to determine the presence of insomnia. In the case of a suspicion of an occult sleep disorder, individuals underwent polysomnography.

2.2. Measures

Due to the large number of included variables in the present regression model, the following battery of well-stablished and widely used brief measures were selected since they have shown that could maintain the validity and reliability of their long versions.

2.2.1. The structured clinical interview for DSM-5 axis I disorders (SCID-5)

The SCID-5 (47) is a standardized diagnostic interview for classifying mental disorders according to DSM-5 criteria. The Persian version of the SCID-5 had adequate test–retest reliability, ranging from 0.60 to 0.79, and the Kappa reliability was between 0.57 and 0.72 (48).

2.2.2. Insomnia severity index

This is a seven-item self-report index developed by Bastien et al. (49) to assess insomnia severity and its three empirically derived subscales of impact, intensity, and satisfaction during the past two weeks. Items are scored on a 0–4 scale, with the total Insomnia severity index (ISI) ranging from 0 to 28. A score of 0–7 demonstrates “no clinically significant insomnia”; 8–14 shows “subthreshold insomnia”; 15–21 demonstrates “moderate clinical insomnia”; and 22–28 shows “severe clinical insomnia.” The Cronbach’s alpha of the Persian version of the ISI in patients with insomnia has been reported as 0.82 (50), and was 0.71 in this study.

2.2.3. Depression anxiety stress scale (DASS-21)

This 21-item scale (51) is a short version of the DASS-42 (52); it was developed to measure the severity of depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms during the previous week, with 7 items for each subscale. Answers are scored on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me very much or most of the time). In the Iranian population, the test–retest coefficients have been reported as 0.85 for stress, 0.77 for depression, and 0.89 for anxiety, with respective internal consistency values of 0.90, 0.91, and 0.84 (53). In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha for depression, anxiety, and stress subscales were 0.80, 0.75, and 0.77, respectively.

2.2.4. Emotion regulation questionnaire

The emotion regulation questionnaire (ERQ) was designed by Gross and John (54) to evaluate two emotion regulation strategies, namely expressive suppression (four items) and cognitive reappraisal (six items). Answers are based on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). In the Iranian population, the internal consistency for cognitive reappraisal and suppression has been reported as 0.76 and 0.72, respectively (55). In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas were 0.77 for expressive suppression and 0.84 for cognitive reappraisal.

2.2.5. Big five inventory

This 10-item inventory was developed by Rammstedt and John (56) to measure 5 major personality traits: extroversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness, with 2 items for each subscale. It is measured on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The Persian version of the Big five inventory (BFI-10) has appropriate test–retest reliability (r = 0.84) and acceptable internal consistency (57). The Cronbach’s alpha of 0.45 was obtained for BFI-10 in this study.

2.2.6. Acceptance and action questionnaire-II

This seven-item questionnaire (31) was designed to assess psychological inflexibility or rigid reactions, such as avoiding or suppressing undesirable thoughts and feelings, or evaluating feelings negatively. Answers are based on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never true) to 7 (always true), where higher scores indicate less psychological flexibility. Split-half coefficients and Cronbach’s alpha values of the Persian version of the Acceptance and action questionnaire-II (AAQ-II) ranged between 0.71 and 0.89 in different samples (58). Cronbach’s alpha for the AAQ-II was 0.63 in this study.

2.2.7. Clinical perfectionism questionnaire

Clinical perfectionism questionnaire (CPQ) (33) is a 12-item questionnaire developed to measure clinical perfectionism, by determining the extent to which the people’s lives are affected by perfectionism. This questionnaire has two subscales: personal standards and evaluative concerns. Scoring is based on four-point Likert scales, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (all the time). In an Iranian sample, the Omega reliability of CPQ was between 0.73 and 0.86 for men and women (59). We found satisfactory internal consistency for the CPQ total score (α = 0.57), personal standards (α = 0.62), and evaluative concerns (α = 0.60).

2.2.8. Anxiety sensitivity index-3

This 18-item Index (60) measures three dimensions of anxiety, namely physical (α = 0.84), cognitive (α = 0.77), and social concerns (α = 0.84), with 6 items per subscale. Participants are asked to rate their concerns regarding the potential negative consequences of anxiety-related experiences, using five-point Likert scales ranging from 0 (very little) to 4 (very much). The internal consistency of the Anxiety sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3) in the Iranian population was previously reported as 0.90 (61). In this study, these values were 0.84, 0.77, and 0.84 for physical, cognitive, and social concerns, respectively.

2.2.9. Repetitive thinking questionnaire

The repetitive thinking questionnaire (RTQ-10) (62) is a unidimensional scale for measuring repetitive thinking. The RTQ-10 is rated on a 5-point scale, where 1 represents “not at all true,” 3 indicates “somewhat true,” and 5 reflects “very true.” The Persian version of the RTQ-10 was reported as having high internal consistency [α = 0.77; (63)]. The RTQ-10 also had adequate internal consistency (α = 0.89) in the present study.

2.3. Procedure

The sample consisted of individuals who were seeking treatment at the sleep disorders clinic of Rasoul Akram hospitals in Tehran. Individuals who volunteered for the study were first asked to fill written informed consent forms. They were informed that their participation in the study was completely voluntary. In addition, they were assured about the confidentiality and that their data would only be used for research purposes. Then, they underwent a structured clinical interview by a sleep clinician to verify inclusion criteria. Patients with the probability of an occult sleep disorder, underwent PSG to screen the physiological parameters related to a wide range of sleep disturbances (exclusion criterion “d”). Those who were not excluded during this phase, were asked to complete the following questionnaires: ISI, DASS-21, ERQ, BFI-10, CPQ, AAQ-II, ASI, and RTQ-10. Responding to the battery of questionnaires took 20–30 min and the order of questionnaires was varied randomly among respondents (questionnaire rotation) in order to increase the response accuracy. This study was approved by the ethics committee at the Iran University of Medical Sciences (approval code = IR.IUMS.REC.1397.1346).

2.4. Statistical strategies

The data screening included consistency checks and descriptive and graphical analysis checks. We decided whether to remove or keep outliers using the comparison between the mean and the 5% trimmed mean (p > 0.05). The substantive main study findings were not affected by the outliers’ presence or absence. Hence, the data of all participants were preserved and the original data were analyzed (64, 65). For data entry and statistical analyses, SPSS version 28 software was used. The Pearson correlation coefficient was used to indicate the relationship between variables. Hierarchical multiple linear regression was subsequently employed to indicate the predictive power of the included variables. Correlations were interpreted as having small (0.10), medium (0.30), large (0.50), or very large (0.70) effect sizes (66).

3. Results

The majority of participants were 18–45-year-old females (90.8%, Mean age = 29.67, SD = 12.41). Regarding marital status, 62.2% were single, 33.6% were married, and 4.2% were divorced/widowed. Regarding education level, 10.1% had completed elementary school, 12.9 had a diploma, 60.8% had a bachelor’s degree, and 16.1% had a master’s and/or PhD degree.

3.1. Correlation of insomnia severity with demographic and transdiagnostic factors

As shown in Table 1, ISI total score had a significant positive correlation with age (r = 0.14, p < 0.05), depression (DASS-21) (r = 0.58, p < 0.01), anxiety (DASS-21) (r = 0.76, p < 0.01), cognitive reappraisal (ERQ) (r = 0.73, p < 0.01), expressive suppression (ERQ) (r = 0.75, p < 0.01), neuroticism (BFI) (r = 0.70, p < 0.01), AAQ total score (r = 0.80, p < 0.01), CPQ total score (r = 0.13, p < 0.05), personal standards (CPQ) (r = 0.86, p < 0.01), evaluative concerns (CPQ) (r = 0.86, p < 0.01), physical concerns (ASI) (r = 0.47, p < 0.01), cognitive concerns (ASI) (r = 0.61, p < 0.01), social concerns (ASI) (r = 0.63, p < 0.01), and RTQ total score (r = 0.80, p < 0.01). In addition, total ISI score had a significant negative correlation with education level (r = −0.21, p < 0.01).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations of insomnia severity with demographics characteristics, and transdiagnostic vulnerability factors in insomnia.

3.2. Prediction of insomnia severity based on demographic and transdiagnostic factors

A hierarchical linear multiple regression model was performed to test the contribution of demographic and transdiagnostic factors in the prediction of insomnia severity. Demographic characteristics such as gender, age, education level, job status, and marital status were considered as the first block, with depression and anxiety as the second block. The third block comprised of emotion regulation strategies, neuroticism, psychological inflexibility, clinical perfectionism subscales, anxiety sensitivity dimensions, and repetitive negative thinking.

The first hierarchical regression analysis model (R2 = 0.08), performed to assess the predictability of insomnia severity based on demographic characteristics, showed that only education level (β = 0.20, p < 0.01) was significantly able to predict insomnia severity. Gender (β = −0.06, p > 0.05), age (β = 0.04, p > 0.05), job status (β = −0.13, p > 0.05), and marital status (β = 0.01, p > 0.05) did not significantly predict insomnia severity. After controlling for demographic in the second model (R2 = 0.10, R2Δ = 0.007), depression (β = 0.10, p > 0.05) and anxiety (β = 0.03, p > 0.05) were not significant predictors of insomnia severity. In the third model, depression was revealed as a significant predictor of insomnia severity (β = 0.34, p < 0.001). Finally, as seen in Table 2, after controlling for demographic characteristics, depression and anxiety in the third model (R2 = 0.40, R2Δ = 0.30), the following transdiagnostic variables significantly predicted insomnia severity: cognitive reappraisal (ERQ) (β = 0.19, p < 0.01), neuroticism (BFI) (β = 0.24, p < 0.001), evaluative concerns (CPQ) (β = 0.16, p < 0.05), personal standards (CPQ) (β = 0.22, p < 0.01), physical concerns (ASI) (β = 0.34, p < 0.001), cognitive concerns (ASI) (β = 0.18, p < 0.05), and repetitive negative thinking (β = 0.33, p < 0.001). However, expressive suppression (ERQ) (β = 0.13, p > 0.05), psychological inflexibility (β = 0.07, p > 0.05), and social concerns (ASI) (β = −0.02, p > 0.05) did not significantly predict insomnia severity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hierarchical multiple linear regression analyses for insomnia severity in insomniac sample.

4. Discussion

The present study sought to identify the best predictive transdiagnostic factors of chronic insomnia in a sample of patients with chronic insomnia. The results demonstrated that all of the transdiagnostic variables evaluated in the current study had moderate to high correlations with insomnia severity, with the exception of clinical perfectionism (r = 0.13). Demographic characteristics, depression, and anxiety symptoms accounted for 10% of the variance of the ISI. Furthermore, 30% of insomnia severity variance was explained by the transdiagnostic factors (in order of highest to lowest coefficients) of physical concerns (ASI), repetitive negative thinking (RTQ), neuroticism (BFI), personal standards (CPQ), cognitive reappraisal (ERQ), cognitive concerns (ASI), and evaluative concerns (CPQ). In addition, physical concerns (ASI) and repetitive negative thinking (RTQ) served simultaneously as the most significant contributing variables for insomnia severity.

Both cognitive reappraisal (ERQ) and expressive suppression (ERQ) indicated significant correlations with insomnia severity, which is consistent with previous findings (67, 68). However, only cognitive reappraisal (ERQ) played a significant role in insomnia severity, which emphasizes the exclusive effect of emotion regulation strategies on insomnia severity. One possible explanation might be that although cognitive reappraisal during the day can regulate emotions via problem solving and negative affect management, it may play a negative role at bedtime by increasing sleep onset latency and reducing sleep quality (69).

Neuroticism was shown to be another factor that had a key role in the etiology of insomnia severity; which is in line with previous findings (see (70, 71)). High levels of neuroticism reflects negative emotions, guilt, moodiness, and poor active control in a person, leading to pre-sleep cognitive distortions, such as worrying, and active thinking at bedtime, which can be associated with over-excitation and hypersensitivity (30). This lack of capability to stop or manage thoughts at night can lead to insomnia (72).

Psychological inflexibility indicated a high significant correlation with insomnia severity (r = 0.80), which is consistent with previous studies (73, 74). Active “experiential avoidance” from experiencing symptoms or cognitions related to sleep problems may lead to increase in pre-sleep arousal and delay in falling asleep (75). Nevertheless, our study indicates that in the presence of other transdiagnostic factors which may be better risk factors of insomnia severity, psychological inflexibility could not be significantly predict insomnia.

This study, in line with previous studies (76, 77), found a significant but weak association between perfectionism and insomnia severity. Also, its two components of personal standards and evaluative concerns significantly contributed to insomnia severity. Maladaptive perfectionism may lead to heightened insomnia through its association with excessive arousal (78) and chronic fatigue (79), which are thought to be amplifying factors for insomnia (80, 81). Furthermore, perfectionists with chronic insomnia unrealistically expect to have excellent quality sleep and become extremely anxious or frustrated with any sleep deprivation. This may cause them to experience negative emotions, leaving them emotionally aroused, which can interfere with their sleep (82).

The present findings suggest that two anxiety sensitivity dimensions – namely physical and cognitive concerns – could predict insomnia severity, with physical concerns (ASI) carrying the highest weight among all the present transdiagnostic variables. This association is consistent with previous studies (83, 84). According to Harvey’s (72) cognitive model, people with elevated anxiety sensitivity are more alert to the physical and cognitive signs and symptoms associated with poor sleep and are more likely to discover these signs and symptoms. As a result, they catastrophize these cues as predictors of insufficient sleep, an inability to fall asleep, or low levels of daily performance, which in turn, heighten a person’s arousal and increase the likelihood of sleep disturbance. In contrast to physical and cognitive concerns, social concerns failed to contribute to insomnia severity. In general, the previous findings on the sub-dimension of social concerns are scarce, perhaps because the body of research has concentrated only on physical and cognitive concerns [(e.g., 38)].

The study findings indicated that repetitive negative thinking had a strong correlation with insomnia severity. It was also a significant risk factor of insomnia severity, which is consistent with previous studies (44, 85). According to Harvey’s (72) cognitive model of insomnia, excessive worry and rumination (two components of repetitive negative thinking) about sleep, as well as long-term unresolved problems experienced during the day and night, result in attentional bias to sleep-related negative information, which trigger psychological arousal, and subsequently delay falling asleep.

4.1. Study limitations and future directions for research

Although our study provides new insights into the transdiagnostic factors that play crucial roles in insomnia severity, several limitations should be noted when drawing conclusions from our data. First, self-report measures are exposed to bias, owing to common method variance (CMV; (73)). Other methods, including actigraphy or daily sleep diaries, are suggested as alternatives, as well as conducting longitudinal studies to limit the effect of potential CMV biases [(see 86)]. Second, examining a clinical sample limits the generalizability to other samples; thus, the findings must be interpreted cautiously. It is also preferable to compare the results between an insomniac and a non-clinical sample. Third, the majority of study participants were women. Although insomnia is more common in women than in men (13), we suggest that future studies consider equal number of men and women in their sample. This may increase the generalizability of their results to both male and female patients with insomnia and also provide the opportunity to explore the potential gender differences. Forth, low Cronbach’s alpha estimates for AAQ-II, CPQ, and BFI-10 and questionable test–retest reliability for some parts of the Persian version of the SCID-5 may raise doubt about the reliability of these measures. Therefore, the finding related to these constructs must be interpreted cautiously. Fifth, in comparison to previous studies, our yielded correlations among the transdiagnostic factors and insomnia severity were relatively high (Table 1). Given their high standard deviations and the existence of a wide range of insomnia symptoms in the clinical sample, the utility of a correlation matrix to draw conclusions might be criticized. However, since the application of the correlation coefficient test on insomniac patients has not been widely accepted, our conclusions were derived from the results of the regression analysis (Table 2), rather than the correlation matrix. Finally, the cross-sectional design used in this study prevents any causal conclusion. Research with longitudinal design is needed to see what factors predict increases and/or the maintenance of symptom severity or what predicts remission of insomnia symptoms. Longitudinal research adds to our results and help designing an empirical framework to develop more accurate scientific and practical interventions for treating sleep disorders.

5. Conclusion

Our findings underlined the importance of transdiagnostic factors of (in descending order) physical concerns (ASI), repetitive negative thinking (RTQ), neuroticism (BFI), personal standards (CPQ), cognitive reappraisal (ERQ), cognitive concerns (ASI), and evaluative concerns (CPQ) in predicting insomnia severity among patients with chronic insomnia. The transdiagnostic factors of physical concerns (ASI) and repetitive negative thinking (RTQ) accounted for the most significant degree of variance in predicting insomnia severity after controlling for demographic characteristics, depression, and anxiety symptoms. Our findings can be applied for the development of intervention programs targeting transdiagnostic factors that have a greater weight in the augmentation of insomnia severity.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the ethics committee at the Iran University of Medical Sciences (approval code = IR.IUMS.REC.1397.1346). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MA and HD: conceptualization, design, and methodology. PS and FH: data collection. MA: formal analysis, funding acquisition, and supervision. MA, HD, and FH: investigation and project administration. HD, PS, and RA: writing the original draft. HD, PS, CB, SM, and MA: revising the manuscript. All authors have contributed to the conception and design of the study, drafted or have been involved in revising this manuscript, reviewed the final version of this manuscript before submission, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

This research was supported by grant number 97–4–23-13-762 from School of Behavioral Sciences and Mental Health, Iran University of Medical Sciences, received by MA. MA’s time on this research was funded by the Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully thank the patients who kindly participated in this research.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Appleton, SL, Gill, TK, Lang, CJ, Taylor, AW, McEvoy, RD, Stocks, NP, et al. Prevalence and comorbidity of sleep conditions in Australian adults: 2016 sleep Health Foundation national survey. Sleep Health. (2018) 4:13–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2017.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Sarsour, K, Morin, CM, Foley, K, Kalsekar, A, and Walsh, JK. Association of insomnia severity and comorbid medical and psychiatric disorders in a health plan-based sample: insomnia severity and comorbidities. Sleep Med. (2010) 11:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Jansson-Fröjmark, M, and Lindblom, K. A bidirectional relationship between anxiety and depression, and insomnia? A prospective study in the general population. J Psychosom Res. (2008) 64:443–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Baglioni, C, Battagliese, G, Feige, B, Spiegelhalder, K, Nissen, C, Voderholzer, U, et al. Insomnia as a predictor of depression: A meta-analytic evaluation of longitudinal epidemiological studies. J Affect Disord. (2011) 135:10–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Blake, MJ, Trinder, JA, and Allen, NB. Mechanisms underlying the association between insomnia, anxiety, and depression in adolescence: implications for behavioral sleep interventions. Clin Psychol Rev. (2018) 63:25–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hertenstein, E, Feige, B, Gmeiner, T, Kienzler, C, Spiegelhalder, K, Johann, A, et al. Insomnia as a predictor of mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. (2019) 43:96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2018.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Morin, CM, Bjorvatn, B, Chung, F, Holzinger, B, Partinen, M, Penzel, T, et al. Insomnia, anxiety, and depression during the COVID-19 pandemic: an international collaborative study. Sleep Med. (2021) 87:38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2021.07.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Xiao, S, Liu, S, Zhang, P, Yu, J, A, H, Wu, H, et al. The association between depressive symptoms and insomnia in college students in Qinghai Province: the mediating effect of rumination. Front Psych. (2021) 12:751411. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.751411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Al-Smadi, AM, Tawalbeh, LI, Gammoh, OS, Ashour, A, Tayfur, M, and Attarian, H. The prevalence and the predictors of insomnia among refugees. J Health Psychol. (2019) 24:1125–33. doi: 10.1177/1359105316687631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Grandner, MA, Patel, NP, Gehrman, PR, Xie, D, Sha, D, Weaver, T, et al. Who gets the best sleep? Ethnic and socioeconomic factors related to sleep complaints. Sleep Med. (2010) 11:470–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hale, L. Who has time to sleep? J Public Health. (2005) 27:205–11. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdi004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Suh, S, Cho, N, and Zhang, J. Sex differences in insomnia: from epidemiology and etiology to intervention. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2018) 20:69. doi: 10.1007/s11920-018-0940-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Farchione, TJ, and Bullis, JR. Addressing the global burden of mental illness: why transdiagnostic and common elements approaches to evidence-based practice might be our best bet. Cogn Behav Pract. (2014) 21:124–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2013.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Gruber, J, Eidelman, P, and Harvey, AG. Transdiagnostic emotion regulation processes in bipolar disorder and insomnia. Behav Res Ther. (2008) 46:1096–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Harvey, AG. A transdiagnostic approach to treating sleep disturbance in psychiatric disorders. Cogn Behav Ther. (2009) 38:35–42. doi: 10.1080/16506070903033825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zakiei, A, Khazaie, H, Reshadat, S, Rezaei, M, and Komasi, S. The comparison of emotional dysregulation and experiential avoidance in patients with insomnia and non-clinical population. J Caring Sci. (2020) 9:87–92. doi: 10.34172/jcs.2020.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Duggan, KA, Friedman, HS, McDevitt, EA, and Mednick, SC. Personality and healthy sleep: the importance of conscientiousness and neuroticism. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e90628. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Williams, J. Psychological flexibility, insomnia and chronic pain: a study of acceptance, mindfulness and values-based action. Plymouth Student Scientist. (2011) 4:276–92.

Google Scholar

20. Lundh, L-G, Broman, J-E, Hetta, J, and Saboonchi, F. Perfectionism and insomnia. Scand J Behav Ther. (1994) 23:3–18. doi: 10.1080/16506079409455949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Grace, D.M. (2020). Examination of sleep disturbance, anxiety sensitivity, and depression in an undergraduate sample. Honors College (Sally McDonnell Barksdale Honors College), University of Mississippi.

Google Scholar

22. Tousignant, OH, Taylor, ND, Suvak, MK, and Fireman, GD. Effects of rumination and worry on sleep. Behav Ther. (2019) 50:558–70. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2018.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Koch, T, Liedl, A, and Ehring, T. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in Afghan refugees. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. (2020) 12:235–43. doi: 10.1037/tra0000489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Cerolini, S, Ballesio, A, and Lombardo, C. Insomnia and emotion regulation: recent findings and suggestions for treatment. J Sleep Disord Manage. (2015) 1:1–5. doi: 10.23937/2572-4053.1510001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Vanek, J, Prasko, J, Genzor, S, Ociskova, M, Holubova, M, Sova, M, et al. Insomnia and emotion regulation. Neuroendocrinol Lett. (2020) 41:255–69.

Google Scholar

26. Davoodi, I, Neisi, A, Khazei, H, Arshadi, N, and Zakiei, A. The roles of some cognitive and emotional factors in predicting insomnia. J Mazandaran Univ Med Sci. (2017) 27:107–21.

Google Scholar

27. Wilamowska, ZA, Thompson-Hollands, J, Fairholme, CP, Ellard, KK, Farchione, TJ, and Barlow, DH. Conceptual background, development, and preliminary data from the unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders. Depress Anxiety. (2010) 27:882–90. doi: 10.1002/da.20735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hisler, GC, Krizan, Z, DeHart, T, and Wright, AGC. Neuroticism as the intensity, reactivity, and variability in day-to-day affect. J Res Pers. (2020) 87:103964. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103964

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nordfjærn, T, Bjerkeset, O, Moylan, S, Berk, M, and Gråwe, RW. Clusters of personality traits and psychological symptoms associated with later benzodiazepine prescriptions in the general population: the HUNT cohort study. Addict Behav. (2013) 38:2575–80. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Gurtman, CG, McNicol, R, and McGillivray, JA. The role of neuroticism in insomnia. Clin Psychol. (2014) 18:116–24. doi: 10.1111/cp.12029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Bond, FW, Hayes, SC, Baer, RA, Carpenter, KM, Guenole, N, Orcutt, HK, et al. Preliminary psychometric properties of the acceptance and action questionnaire-II: a revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behav Ther. (2011) 42:676–88. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kato, T. Effect of psychological inflexibility on depressive symptoms and sleep disturbance among Japanese young women with chronic pain. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:7624. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17207426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fairburn, C, Cooper, Z, and Shafran, R. The clinical perfectionism questionnaire. Oxford: (2003).

Google Scholar

34. Vincent, N, and Walker, J. Perfectionism and chronic insomnia. J Psychosom Res. (2000) 49:349–54. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00175-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Schmidt, RE, Courvoisier, DS, Cullati, S, Kraehenmann, R, and Linden, MV. Too imperfect to fall asleep: perfectionism, pre-sleep counterfactual processing, and insomnia. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:361–370. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Boswell, JF, Farchione, TJ, Sauer-Zavala, S, Murray, HW, Fortune, MR, and Barlow, DH. Anxiety sensitivity and interoceptive exposure: a transdiagnostic construct and change strategy. Behav Ther. (2013) 44:417–31. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2013.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hoge, EA, Marques, L, Wechsler, RS, Lasky, AK, Delong, HR, Jacoby, RJ, et al. The role of anxiety sensitivity in sleep disturbance in panic disorder. J Anxiety Disord. (2011) 25:536–8. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Babson, KA, Trainor, CD, Bunaciu, L, and Feldner, MT. An examination of anxiety sensitivity as a moderator of the relation between sleep anticipatory anxiety and sleep onset latency. J Cogn Psychother. (2008) 22:258–70. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.22.3.258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Vincent, N, and Walker, J. Anxiety sensitivity: predictor of sleep-related impairment and medication use in chronic insomnia. Depress Anxiety. (2001) 14:238–43. doi: 10.1002/da.1073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Drost, J, van der Does, W, van Hemert, AM, Penninx, BW, and Spinhoven, P. Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety: A conceptual replication. Behav Res Ther. (2014) 63:177–83. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hummel, KV, Trautmann, S, Venz, J, Thomas, S, and Schäfer, J. Repetitive negative thinking: transdiagnostic correlate and risk factor for mental disorders? A proof-of-concept study in German soldiers before and after deployment to Afghanistan. BMC Psychol. (2021) 9:198. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00696-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. McEvoy, PM, Salmon, K, Hyett, MP, Jose, PE, Gutenbrunner, C, Bryson, K, et al. Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic predictor of depression and anxiety symptoms in adolescents. Assessment. (2017) 26:324–35. doi: 10.1177/1073191117693923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Riemann, D, Spiegelhalder, K, Feige, B, Voderholzer, U, Berger, M, Perlis, M, et al. The hyperarousal model of insomnia: a review of the concept and its evidence. Sleep Med Rev. (2010) 14:19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.04.002X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Galbiati, A, Giora, E, Sarasso, S, Zucconi, M, and Ferini-Strambi, L. Repetitive thought is associated with both subjectively and objectively recorded polysomnographic indices of disrupted sleep in insomnia disorder. Sleep Med. (2018) 45:55–61. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Clancy, F, Prestwich, A, Caperon, L, Tsipa, A, and O'Connor, DB. The association between worry and rumination with sleep in non-clinical populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. (2020) 14:427–48. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2019.1700819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Lemyre, A, Belzile, F, Landry, M, Bastien, CH, and Beaudoin, LP. Pre-sleep cognitive activity in adults: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. (2020) 50:101253. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.101253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. First, MB, Williams, JBW, Karg, RS, and Spitzer, RL. Structured clinical interview for DSM-5—Research version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, research version; SCID-5-RV). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2015).

Google Scholar

48. Mohammadkhani, P, Forouzan, AS, Hooshyari, Z, and Abasi, I. Psychometric properties of Persian version of structured clinical interview for DSM-5-research version (SCID-5-RV): a diagnostic accuracy study. Iran J Psychiat Behav Sci. (2020) 14:e100930. doi: 10.5812/ijpbs.100930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bastien, CH, Vallières, A, and Morin, CM. Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. (2001) 2:297–307. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Yazdi, Z, Sadeghniiat-Haghighi, K, Zohal, MA, and Elmizadeh, K. Validity and reliability of the Iranian version of the insomnia severity index. MJMS. (2012) 19:31–6.

Google Scholar

51. Henry, JD, and Crawford, JR. The short-form version of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. (2005) 44:227–39. doi: 10.1348/014466505x29657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lovibond, PF, and Lovibond, SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the Beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. (1995) 33:335–43. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Asghari, A, Saed, F, and Dibajnia, P. Psychometric properties of the depression anxiety stress Scales-21 (DASS-21) in a non-clinical Iranian sample. Int J Psychol. (2008) 2:102–82.

Google Scholar

54. Gross, JJ, and John, OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 85:348–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Foroughi, AA, Parvizifard, A, Sadeghi, K, and Moghadam, AP. Psychometric properties of the Persian version of the emotion regulation questionnaire. Trends Psychiat Psychother. (2021) 43:101–7. doi: 10.47626/2237-6089-2018-0106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Rammstedt, B, and John, OP. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the big five inventory in English and German. J Res Pers. (2007) 41:203–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Mohammad Zadeh, A, and Najafi, M. “Validating of the big five inventory (BFI-10): A very brief measure of the five factor personality model” in Quarterly of educational measurement (2010).

Google Scholar

58. Abasi, E, Fata, L, Molodi, R, and Zarabi, H. Psychometric properties of Persian version of acceptance and action questionnaire-II. J Psychol Methods Models. (2012) 3:65–80.

Google Scholar

59. Moloodi, R, Pourshahbaz, A, Mohammadkhani, P, Fata, L, and Ghaderi, A. Two-factor higher-order model of perfectionism in Iranian general and clinical samples. BMC Psychol. (2021) 9:30. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00529-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Taylor, S, Zvolensky, MJ, Cox, BJ, Deacon, B, Heimberg, RG, Ledley, DR, et al. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the anxiety sensitivity Index-3. Psychol Assess. (2007) 19:176–88. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Foroughi, AA, Mohammadpour, M, Khanjani, S, Pouyanfard, S, Dorouie, N, and Parvizi Fard, AA. Psychometric properties of the Iranian version of the anxiety sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3). Trends Psychiat Psychother. (2019) 41:254–61. doi: 10.1590/2237-6089-2018-0078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. McEvoy, PM, Thibodeau, MA, and Asmundson, GJG. Trait repetitive negative thinking: A brief Transdiagnostic assessment. J Exp Psychopathol. (2014) 5:1–17. doi: 10.5127/jep.037813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Hasani, M, Ahmadi, R, and Saed, O. Psychometric properties and factor structure of repetitive thinking questionnaire: A Persian version of the RTQ-31 and RTQ-10. Trends Psychia Psychother. (2021) 44:e20200058. doi: 10.47626/2237-6089-2020-0058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Pallant, J. SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using IBM SPSS. Orlando, FL: Routledge (2020).

Google Scholar

65. Tabachnick, BG, Fidell, LS, and Ullman, JB. Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Pearson (2007) 14, 249–257.

Google Scholar

66. Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Routledge (2013).

Google Scholar

67. Harvey, AG. Insomnia: symptom or diagnosis? Clin Psychol Rev. (2001) 21:1037–59. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(00)00083-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Palagini, L, Bastien, CH, Marazziti, D, Ellis, JG, and Riemann, D. The key role of insomnia and sleep loss in the dysregulation of multiple systems involved in mood disorders: A proposed model. J Sleep Res. (2019) 28:e12841. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Wicklow, A, and Espie, CA. Intrusive thoughts and their relationship to actigraphic measurement of sleep: towards a cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther. (2000) 38:679–93. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00136-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Chauvin, B, Thibault-Stoll, A, Chassagnon, S, Biry, S, Petiau, C, and Tassi, P. Sleep-related cognitions mediate the impact of neuroticism on insomnia. Am J Health Behav. (2015) 39:623–31. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.39.5.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Stephan, Y, Sutin, AR, Bayard, S, Križan, Z, and Terracciano, A. Personality and sleep quality: evidence from four prospective studies. Health Psychol. (2018) 37:271–81. doi: 10.1037/hea0000577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Harvey, AG. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther. (2002) 40:869–93. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(01)00061-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kato, T. Impact of psychological inflexibility on depressive symptoms and sleep difficulty in a Japanese sample. Springerplus. (2016) 5:712. doi: 10.1186/s40064-016-2393-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Sands, J. An examination of the relationship between psychological flexibility and insomnia. Dearborn: University of Michigan (2020).

Google Scholar

75. Zakiei, A, Khazaie, H, Rostampour, M, Lemola, S, Esmaeili, M, Dürsteler, K, et al. Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) improves sleep quality, experiential avoidance, and emotion regulation in individuals with insomnia-results from a randomized interventional study. Life. (2021) 11:133. doi: 10.3390/life11020133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Akram, U, Gardani, M, Riemann, D, Akram, A, Allen, SF, Lazuras, L, et al. Dysfunctional sleep-related cognition and anxiety mediate the relationship between multidimensional perfectionism and insomnia symptoms. Cogn Process. (2020) 21:141–8. doi: 10.1007/s10339-019-00937-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Johann, AF, Hertenstein, E, Kyle, SD, Baglioni, C, Feige, B, Nissen, C, et al. Perfectionism and polysomnography-determined markers of poor sleep. J Clin Sleep Med. (2017) 13:1319–26. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.6806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Trudel-Fitzgerald, C, Savard, J, Slim, LM, Roy, RC, Flett, GL, Hewitt, PL, et al. The relationship of perfectionism with psychological symptoms in cancer patients and the contributing role of hyperarousability and coping. Psychol Health. (2017) 32:381–401. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2016.1273354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Dittner, AJ, Rimes, K, and Thorpe, S. Negative perfectionism increases the risk of fatigue following a period of stress. Psychol Health. (2011) 26:253–68. doi: 10.1080/08870440903225892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Lichstein, KL, Means, MK, Noe, SL, and Aguillard, RN. Fatigue and sleep disorders. Behav Res Ther. (1997) 35:733–40. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00029-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Schiel, JE, Holub, F, Petri, R, Leerssen, J, Tamm, S, Tahmasian, M, et al. Affect and arousal in insomnia: through a lens of neuroimaging studies. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2020) 22:44. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-01173-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Schmitz, N, Hartkamp, N, Kiuse, J, Franke, GH, Reister, G, and Tress, W. The symptom check-List-90-R (SCL-90-R): a German validation study. Qual Life Res. (2000) 9:185–93. doi: 10.1023/A:1008931926181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Lauriola, M, Carleton, RN, Tempesta, D, Calanna, P, Socci, V, Mosca, O, et al. A correlational analysis of the relationships among intolerance of uncertainty, anxiety sensitivity, subjective sleep quality, and insomnia symptoms. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:3253. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16183253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Leyro, TM, Babson, KA, and Bonn-Miller, MO. Anxiety sensitivity in relation to sleep quality among HIV-infected individuals. J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. (2014) 25:638–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jana.2014.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Sandru, C, and Voinescu, BI. The relationship between emotion regulation, dysfunctional beliefs about sleep and sleep quality-an exploratory study. J Evid Based Psychother. (2014) 14:249–257.

Google Scholar

86. Podsakoff, PM, MacKenzie, SB, and Podsakoff, NP. Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu Rev Psychol. (2012) 63:539–69. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: insomnia, transdiagnostic factors, anxiety, depression, perfectionism, emotion regulation, repetitive thinking

Citation: Habibi Asgarabad M, Doos Ali Vand H, Salehi Yegaei P, Hooman F, Ahmadi R, Baglioni C and Moradi S (2023) The contribution of transdiagnostic vulnerability factors in patients with chronic insomnia. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1162729. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1162729

Received: 09 February 2023; Accepted: 14 March 2023;
Published: 03 April 2023.

Edited by:

Sy Duong-Quy, Lam Dong Medical College, Vietnam

Reviewed by:

Trang Nguyen, Ho Chi Minh City Medicine and Pharmacy University, Vietnam
Mai Nguyen Thi Phuong, Hanoi Medical University, Vietnam

Copyright © 2023 Habibi Asgarabad, Doos Ali Vand, Salehi Yegaei, Hooman, Ahmadi, Baglioni and Moradi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mojtaba Habibi Asgarabad, Mojtaba.h.asgarabad@ntnu.no; Hoda Doos Ali Vand, doosalivand.h@sbmu.ac.ir

†ORCID: Mojtaba Habibi Asgarabad https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5011-2823
Hoda Doos Ali Vand https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1814-979X
Pardis Salehi Yegaei https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0773-650X
Farzaneh Hooman https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9706-7740
Reza Ahmadi https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5179-3280
Chiara Baglioni https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3150-7755
Shahram Moradi https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2103-7721

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.