Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 06 December 2022
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic One Health: The Psychology of Human-Nature Relationships for Planetary and Human Wellbeing, volume II View all 21 articles

Social prescribing of nature therapy for adults with mental illness living in the community: A scoping review of peer-reviewed international evidence

\r\nTamsin Thomas*Tamsin Thomas1*Christina Aggar,Christina Aggar1,2James BakerJames Baker3Debbie MasseyDebbie Massey1Megan ThomasMegan Thomas4Daniel D&#x;AppioDaniel D’Appio3Eric Brymer,Eric Brymer1,5
  • 1Faculty of Health, Southern Cross University, Bilinga, QLD, Australia
  • 2Northern NSW Local Health District, Lismore, NSW, Australia
  • 3Primary and Community Care Services, Thornleigh, NSW, Australia
  • 4Graduate School of Medicine, University of Wollongong, Keiraville, NSW, Australia
  • 5Manna Institute, University of New England, Armidale, NSW, Australia

Social prescribing of nature therapy “green social prescribing” facilitates access to local nature-based activities that improve biopsychosocial wellbeing outcomes, are affordable, accessible, and can be adapted to context. These are becoming increasingly popular and gray literature is emerging, however, peer-reviewed scientific evidence is exiguous. This scoping review aimed to identify and critique peer-reviewed evidence for green social prescribing interventions and develop recommendations for research and clinical practice. Included studies were published in peer-reviewed journals in English on/after 1 January 2000. Participants were community-living adults with mental illness; Intervention was any green social prescribing program; Comparator was not restricted/required; Outcomes were any biopsychosocial measures; and any/all Study Designs were included. Twelve databases were searched on 15 October 2022; these were Academic Search Premier, APA PsycArticles, APA PsycINFO, CINAHL, Cochrane Library, Google Scholar, JSTOR, ProQuest, PubMed, Science Direct, Scopus, and Web of Science. The Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool was used to assess quality. Seven publications describing 6 unique studies (5 UK, 1 Australia) were identified including 3 mixed-methods, 2 qualitative, and 1 RCT. Participants included 334 adults (45% female, aged 35–70 years); sample sizes ranged from 9 to 164. All studies showed improvements in biopsychosocial wellbeing, and participants from most studies (n = 5) reported increased connection to the earth and intention to further access nature. Participant demographics and diagnoses were poorly reported, and intervention activities and assessments varied considerably. However, MMAT scores were good overall suggesting these studies may reliably demonstrate intervention outcomes. We conclude that socially prescribed nature therapy can improve biopsychosocial wellbeing and is a potentially important intervention for mental illness. Recommendations for research and clinical practice are provided.

Introduction

The global prevalence of mental illness (MI) is estimated at 15–20%, accounting for 7–13% of disability-adjusted life-years (Vigo et al., 2016; Rehm and Shield, 2019). In industrialized countries MI costs approximately 4% of GDP (Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development [OECD], 2021) and strains health resources (Allen, 2020). In developing countries MI is often untreated due to limited healthcare resources and infrastructure (World Health Organisation [WHO], 2021). To address this global “public health crisis” (Rosenberg, 2012), WHO prioritizes affordable community-based MI interventions that are evidence-based and address the interconnected biopsychosocial factors implicated in MI (World Health Organisation [WHO], 2004, 2021).

Social prescribing (SP) is a model of healthcare provision aligned with the WHO priorities, whereby community-based health and welfare workers refer clients to local evidence-based sources of non-medical support that address biopsychosocial wellbeing (Knapp et al., 2013; Husk et al., 2019). SP can facilitate access to therapeutic and social interventions without the need for formal healthcare services. Linkages are generally made to local existing public, private, volunteer, and faith-based services, thereby making it affordable and scalable even in countries with overwhelmed or non-existent healthcare services (Knapp et al., 2013). Examples of prescriptible programs include Alcoholics Anonymous and Park Run which span 180 and 22 developed and developing countries respectively (Alcoholics Anonymous, 2021; Parkrun Global, 2021).

Nature therapy (NT) is an umbrella term for nature-based activities designed to improve participant health and wellbeing (Shanahan et al., 2019). NT interventions are varied and include, for example, Horticulture Therapy i.e., gardening (Adevi and Mårtensson, 2013); Conservation activities e.g., tree planting (Husk et al., 2016); remote-area Wilderness Therapy including bush-skills (Rutko and Gillespie, 2013), and Forest Therapy which is a mindfulness and relaxation-based NT program (Kotte et al., 2019). Many NT programs do not require trained facilitators or specialist equipment and can be conducted in any biome. This flexibility means NT is affordable, scalable, and easily adapted to community, cultural, and environmental contexts (Burls, 2007).

In recent years there has been an increase in interest and large-scale investment in SP of NT interventions [hereafter “Green Social Prescribing” (GSP)] (Tierney et al., 2020; Lindsay et al., 2022). However, despite considerable gray literature supporting continued implementation, in particular from the UK National Health Service (National Health Service [NHS], 2019), little reliable, peer-reviewed evidence is available about how effective these interventions are for people with MI.

This paper systematically examines peer-reviewed evidence related to the biopsychosocial benefits of GSP for community-living adults with MI in terms of intervention design and research methods in order to optimize future GSP interventions and direct future research. This resource is intended to (1) inform decision-making by providing governments, non-government organizations, and other interested groups with an outline of possible interventions, the potential health outcomes, and the target beneficiaries, and (2) inform research by explicating the most important features that need to be targeted and evaluated.

There is a growing evidence base for the biopsychosocial impacts of SP (for reviews see Chatterjee et al., 2018; Leavell et al., 2019). SP as a referral pathway for MI services in general, and NT in particular, can improve access to therapeutic interventions for people with MI where formal health service are not available, thereby overcoming personal and structural barriers (Zambas and Wright, 2016; Morris et al., 2022). Referrals can come from trusted community members at a grassroots level thereby facilitating access for groups who generally have poorer mental and physical health, and may not have the skills, capacity, or resources to access formal health services; this includes socioeconomically disadvantaged populations (Allen et al., 2014; Husk et al., 2019), individuals with a disability (Lindsay et al., 2022), minority ethnic groups with cultural, religious, and language barriers (Zeh et al., 2014; Gupta, 2021), and a range of other barriers, for example in the case of Australia First Nations peoples who often do not trust formal healthcare services (Zambas and Wright, 2016).

Research has demonstrated that NT experiences can facilitate a plethora of biopsychosocial improvements. For example, NT has demonstrated improvements in physical health including the biological correlates of stress (salivary cortisol, immunoglobulin), cardiac, respiratory, and immune function (Oh et al., 2017), hypertension, obesity, post-surgical recovery, and pain (Chaudhury and Banerjee, 2020). NT also improves psychological wellbeing including improved affect and vitality, and decreased stress, depression, anxiety, and anger (Hartig et al., 1991; Ulrich et al., 1991; Lee et al., 2009; Ryan et al., 2010; Berman et al., 2012; Van den Berg et al., 2016; Kotera et al., 2022). It also improves cognition, including enhanced concentration and restoration of mental function (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989; Berto, 2005; Berman et al., 2008, 2012).

There also appear to be improvements in psychosocial impacts of NT but this has have received little research attention (Hartig et al., 2014). A meta-analysis of community garden interventions demonstrated moderate improvements in perceived social support, community cohesion, and loneliness (Edwards and Beck, 2002; Spano et al., 2020). Other studies have also demonstrated that green spaces in neighborhoods improve psychosocial wellbeing, for example, perceived neighborhood greenness predicts social cohesion (sense of community, social support) (Sugiyama et al., 2008; De Vries et al., 2013).

Importantly, NT can increase subjective wellbeing over and above the positive effects of physical exercise (Pretty et al., 2007; Nisbet et al., 2009; Gladwell et al., 2013; Araújo et al., 2019; Brymer et al., 2020). This is important as exercise has barriers for individuals with MI that may not be present in NT, including self-consciousness, poor self-efficacy, fatigue, fear of injury, and existing physical injuries/restrictions (Firth et al., 2016). NT interventions can be substantially different to Green Exercise interventions and can overcome many of these issues, for example Forest Therapy can be conducted on wheelchair-accessible paths (Kotte et al., 2019). Some NT interventions do include incidental exercise, for example walking short distances when bird watching but the primary mechanism of therapeutic action is exposure to nature and physical activity is secondary, minimal, and/or optional (Christie and Cole, 2017; Kotte et al., 2019).

Theories explaining the mechanisms by which NT improves wellbeing include Attention Restoration Theory (ART) (Kaplan and Peterson, 1993; Kaplan, 1995), Biophilia (Wilson, 1984), and Stress Reduction Theory (SRT) (Ulrich et al., 1991). ART suggests that some environments are more conducive to restoring mental fatigue resulting from everyday urban lifestyles. The natural world restores cognitive resources and the subsequent ability to focus because attention is held with reduced requirement of effort. A critical review of ART found only partial evidence for the efficacy of ART as an explanatory model as it only demonstrated impacts on executive abilities (Ohly et al., 2016) but not the biopsychosocial benefits of interacting with nature (Hartig and Jahncke, 2017). SRT also considers the impact of metropolitan living and claims that humans have an evolutionary connection with nature and that specific characteristics of nature (complexity, depth, absence of threat) provides restorative benefits (Ulrich et al., 1991). Biophilia notes that humans innately desire connection to life and life-like processes, and exposure to adaptive natural features (e.g., food/water/shelter) is fundamental to biopsychosocial wellbeing (Kellert, 1997). While the SRT and Biophilic frameworks have made a considerable contribution to our understanding of the relationship between human beings and nature, critics suggest they are limited as, for example, they largely overlook the complexity of the relationship between humans and nature, which is inherently multi-dimensional, interactive and multi-sensorial (Joye and Van den Berg, 2011; Brymer et al., 2014; Franco et al., 2017; Araújo et al., 2019).

Despite these potential benefits, peer-reviewed scientific evidence for GSP for MI is sparse. Most extant programs are not formally evaluated or peer-reviewed, with methods and outcomes not published or published in non-scientific documents such as by private consultancy firms or in government reports (i.e., “gray literature”). For example, the UK NHS funds several GSP programs (Rubio et al., 2010; Bragg and Leck, 2017). These include the West Leeds Patient Empowerment Project (The National Health Service [NHS], 2015, 2016) which provided NT interventions such as woodland walks and environmental conservation groups for 5 years (2011–2016) with over 4,000 participants with non-communicable diseases including MI. While the program demonstrated improvements in participant physical and mental health, the evaluation by the private consulting firm Kier Business Services Limited (2016) was not peer-reviewed or published in a scientific journal. This lack of peer review and scientific rigor is unfortunate as this large sample had the potential to provide important evidence for GSP. This lack of good quality peer-reviewed evidence for GSP for adults with MI is unfortunate given gray literature indicating the potential of GSP to be an effective, affordable, and scalable intervention for MI.

Thus, this review is both timely and important; by systematically examining peer-reviewed evidence to identify and critique the characteristics of intervention design and research methods we can improve future GSP research, clinical practice, and participant outcomes in this burgeoning field.

Aim and research question

This review aimed to identify and critique peer-reviewed studies of adults with MI who live in the community (Population) attending GSP programs (Intervention) in order to create recommendations for future research and clinical practice. Included studies assessed any/all aspects of biopsychosocial wellbeing (Outcomes), were not required to include a control group (Comparator) and could comprise any study type (Study Design).

The research questions for this review are: (1) what types of peer-reviewed evidence exists for GSP for adults with MI living in community settings; (2) what is the quality of this evidence; (3) what are the common findings of this evidence; and (4) what are the implications for future research and clinical practice?

Objectives

The objective of this review is to systematically identify, describe, review, and compile peer-reviewed evidence to produce recommendations for research and clinical practice. We use a scoping review methodology which is most appropriate for research, as is the case here, that is limited, complex and heterogenous (Peters et al., 2020).

Methods

Protocol

The protocol for this review was developed according to the Joanna Briggs Institute methodological guidance for the conduct of scoping reviews (Peters et al., 2020) and the PRISMA-ScR extension for scoping reviews checklist (Tricco et al., 2018). It was produced by author TT and reviewed by all authors; the completed checklist is available as Supplementary material.

Eligibility criteria

Inclusion criteria

For inclusion, publications were required to report primary research published in peer-reviewed scientific journals in English from any country published on/after 1 January 2000. Participants were adults (18+ years) of any gender living in the community with MI; the intervention was any GSP; a comparator was not required nor restricted; outcomes included any biopsychosocial measures, and any/all study designs were included. For the purposes of this review MI was defined broadly and included disturbance in cognition, emotional regulation, behavior, psychosocial abilities, or other mental state associated with significant distress or impairment in functioning (World Health Organisation [WHO], 2022). Inclusion criteria are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Scoping review inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Exclusion criteria

Publications were excluded if they did not report peer-reviewed primary research. Review papers were excluded as they are “only as good as the data on which [they are] based” (Charrois, 2015, pp. 145) and as our intention was to identify and evaluate this data ourselves review papers could not provide useful or meaningful sources of information. Non-English publications were excluded due to resource limitations preventing translation. Articles published prior to 2,000 were excluded, thus eliminating outdated/incomparable studies where the classification, diagnosis, and treatment of MI was substantially different. Children (under 18) were excluded as MI in children presents and is treated differently (MFMER, 2002). Inpatient populations were excluded as this review focused on community-based interventions. Animal-assisted therapy was excluded as the main therapeutic action is different to that of NT, being theorized to involve perceived attachment to the animal and oxytocin release in response to caregiving and physical contact (Julius et al., 2012). Furthermore, animal-assisted therapy is resource-intense and not widely available (The Australian National University, 2019), and is thus not consistent with the goals of this review which aims to identify potentially scalable and affordable community-based interventions. Research examining the impact of physical activity as the primary therapeutic mechanism for MI are extensively reported elsewhere and are beyond the scope of this review (Rosenbaum et al., 2014; Stubbs et al., 2018). Publications that were explicitly exercise-based were excluded at title and abstract-review stages, whereas those with ambiguity were reviewed at a full-text level. For studies where the primary therapeutic action was still unclear 3 authors TT, MT, and DD’A initially reviewed the full-text and if questionable, studies were assessed by discussion with the whole research team.

Information sources

Twelve electronic databases were searched on 4 February 2021 (updated on 15 October 2022) for primary research; these were Academic Search Premier, APA PsycArticles, APA PsycINFO, CINAHL, Cochrane Library, Google Scholar, JSTOR, ProQuest, PubMed, Science Direct, Scopus, and Web of Science. Reference lists of included studies and related review papers were scanned, and an expert in the field of SP and NT (author JB) was consulted to identify additional studies. Databases were selected to provide comprehensive coverage of source material from health and human sciences.

Search strategy

The search strategy was drafted by author TT in consultation with an expert university librarian, piloted to ensure known studies were included, and reviewed by all authors. Key concepts covered by search terms included SP, MI, NT, GSP, and Blue SP (NT programs that focus on outdoor water environments) (Britton et al., 2018); see Table 2 for the complete search strategy for PubMed.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Search strategy for PubMed database indicating concept search steps, combinations, and wildcards.

Search terms were developed based on known SP and NT studies and review articles. Search terms regarding blue prescriptions were adapted from a recent blue care review paper (Britton et al., 2020), however, search terms from this review relating to blue exercise activities (e.g., surfing) were excluded. The use of Mesh headings, search terms, wildcards, and limits were adapted to each database. Wildcards ensured all permutations of words were included, and broad search terms captured country-specific vernacular. Concept searches were conducted individually and then combined.

Selection of sources of evidence

All database searches were conducted by author TT with results imported into Endnote X9 (Clarivate, 2013) and duplicates removed. Initial refinement of potential papers according to selection criteria was completed based on title screening, followed by abstract screening of remaining papers (author TT). Full-text review of remaining papers was conducted independently by 3 authors (TT, MT, and DD’A) and disagreement resolved by discussion.

Data charting and items

A data charting tool (data extraction tool specific to scoping reviews) was developed in Microsoft Word (Microsoft, 2022) based on the template by Peters et al. (2017). This tool included the Study Setting, Design, Participant Characteristics, Intervention (and Control), Outcome Measures, and Findings. Piloting of data extraction on 2 papers elucidated the necessity of including an additional data point (i.e., Data Collection). Charting was conducted independently by 2 authors (TT and MT) and inconsistencies resolved with discussion. Study authors were emailed to gain additional required information.

Critical appraisal of evidence

Appraisal was conducted independently by 2 authors (TT and DD’A) using the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool (MMAT; Hong et al., 2018). A consensus approach was also used to confirm the ratings according to the 5 criteria of the MMAT. A scoring system of 1 (no criteria met) to 5 (all criteria met) was used independently by 2 authors (TT and DD’A) and inconsistencies resolved with discussion.

Synthesis of results

Charted study results were imported into a separate Microsoft Word table (Microsoft, 2022) organized by study. Common findings across studies were identified and color-coded by author (TT) and re-tabulated by result. This synthesized results table was checked against full-text papers to ensure accuracy. Frequencies were calculated.

Results

Study selection

Database searches yielded 588 publications and reference list checking another 3; 98 duplicates were removed leaving 493 unique publications (Figure 1). Next, title screening excluded 428 publications, primarily because studies were exercise-based or not SP. Sixty-five abstracts were screened and 44 excluded for the same reasons. Twenty-one-text publications were reviewed and 14 excluded as they were not MI (n = 7), not SP (n = 5), or were exercise-based (n = 2). Harris et al. (2014) were contacted to confirm participants’ mental health status and referral process. Seven publications were included in this review reporting 6 unique studies; these were Wilson et al. (2010, 2011), Harris et al. (2014), Christie and Cole (2017), Maund et al. (2019), McEwan et al. (2019), and Thomson et al. (2020). One study was described in 2 publications separated into qualitative (Wilson et al., 2010) and quantitative (Wilson et al., 2011) components.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scoping review study selection PRISMA diagram. Adapted from Page et al. (2021).

Study selection elucidated the relative lack of peer-reviewed research in this area. Given the number of large GSP programs currently being conducted internationally, the lack of rigorous research was stark. This represents a lost opportunity to demonstrate the efficacy of such programs and thus capacity to gain funding and expand such programs. Additionally, current/future programs are not being adapted to improve their efficacy as additional evidence could allow. Contrastingly, evidence of limited efficacy could prevent diversion of funding to other programs; however, under-publication of negative results “Publication Bias” (Ayorinde et al., 2020) could prevent this regardless of sufficient peer-reviewed evidence.

Quality of included studies

Overall, the quality of reported studies was excellent, as assessed on the MMAT; scores are presented in Table 3. All studies had a clear research question and corresponding findings (MMAT screening item) and are thus excluded from Table 3 for brevity.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Study methods and results summary.

The overall good quality of included studies was surprising as reporting of methods, in particular intervention information and participant characteristics, was poor (discussed further below). However, the focus of the MMAT is the appropriateness and congruence of the research design and analysis in terms of the research question (i.e., methodological rigor). For example, participant characteristics in Harris et al. (2014) were poorly reported but the research question was clear, data collection and analysis were appropriate to answer the question, and the findings/themes were well supported by quotes from participants.

Summary of included studies

The following section outlines (1) learnings from the findings of GSP studies included in the paper, and (2) learnings from research that examines the process and outcomes of GSP. A summary of the methods and findings of all included studies can be seen in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mixed methods appraisal tool quality appraisal scores by study design components.

Findings of included studies

All 6 included studies reported improvements in participant biopsychosocial wellbeing, however, these improvements varied in terms of quality and quantity in each domain. A detailed breakdown of these findings can be seen in Table 5; for brevity, only study-specific results that are not presented in Table 4 are individually referenced in the result synthesis sub-sections below.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Study results for biopsychosocial domains and environment factors.

Global quality of life and wellbeing

All 6 studies reported improvements in at least 1 of physical, psychological, and/or social QoL or a wellbeing domain (elaborated below) but only 3 studies assessed global/overall QoL and wellbeing, defined herein as “an individual’s perception of their position in life in the context of the culture and value systems in which they live and in relation to their goals, expectations, standards and concerns” (WHOQOL Group, 1993). These included 2 quantitative studies that assessed quality of life/wellbeing using a total score on a QoL/wellbeing measure. McEwan et al. (2019) reported significant improvements in overall scores for the Recovering Quality of Life for people with MI questionnaire (ReQoL) (Keetharuth et al., 2018), and Thomson et al. (2020) reported significant improvements in overall wellbeing on the UCL Museum Wellbeing Measure (Thomson and Chatterjee, 2013). Additionally, 1 participant in qualitative study (Harris et al., 2014) noted “it is very important for me to be in the garden for my whole total wellbeing” (p. 9209). McEwan et al. (2019) found greater improvements in QoL were present for those living with MI (versus not living with MI) and for those having spent lower levels of time outside in the year preceding the intervention.

Physical wellbeing

Although this review specifically excludes exercise interventions as they have barriers to participation (Firth et al., 2016), physical wellbeing can and does improve from exposure to nature (Twohig-Bennett and Jones, 2018) and is thus included in this review paper. Three studies reported qualitative improvements in participant perceived physical wellbeing, including sleep quality, vigor, fitness, pain, symptom management, weight loss, and breathing under exertion. Despite participant reports, Wilson et al. (2011) did not find significant quantitative improvements in self-reported physical health as measured by the SF-12v2 Health Survey (Kosinski et al., 2007). Participants did, however, report significantly increased weekly moderate exercise from pre to post-intervention. During qualitative interviews 1 participant stated they now left the house more often and completed more activities suggesting increased incidental exercise (Wilson et al., 2010, 2011). A participant in Maund et al. (2019) stated the incidental exercise during the intervention was enjoyable as it was not formal or mandated.

Psychological wellbeing

Five studies reported improved overall psychological wellbeing, including symptoms of anxiety, depression, and panic. While the quantitative study by Maund et al. (2019) demonstrated a significant improvement in wellbeing, changes reported by Wilson et al. (2011) did not reach significance. All 6 included studies provided evidence of increased positive affect (e.g., happiness), including 2 studies that found significant improvements using quantitative analyses (Maund et al., 2019; McEwan et al., 2019). Maund et al. (2019) also found a significant decrease in negative affect (e.g., anger). In 2 studies (Christie and Cole, 2017; Maund et al., 2019) participants described the interventions as relaxing, a distraction from negativity, and providing a sense of escape, with 1 participant stating it was a “break from reality, a 2-h holiday” (Maund et al., 2019). In these studies participants reported that prior to the intervention they spent most of their time home alone, sometimes in unpleasant conditions (e.g., without electricity), which 1 participant described as a “bleak existence” (Christie and Cole, 2017). In 3 studies participants reported the interventions provided something to look forward to and a reason to get out of bed or leave the house, and that the routine and structure of the program contributed to their psychological wellbeing.

Five studies demonstrated improvements in participant self-esteem and self-confidence, with these largely related to activity-based skill development. Two studies reported improved agency (Christie and Cole, 2017; Thomson et al., 2020), 1 improved self-reliance (Harris et al., 2014), and 3 a better sense of self-identity, with 1 participant describing changes as resulting from overall improvements in perceived self-worth (Thomson et al., 2020). Participants reported satisfaction when they learned, developed, and taught others new skills (e.g., growing crops, constructing bird boxes), with participants using newly developed skills to be productive and complete what they considered meaningful tasks, resulting in a sense of purpose, achievement, and pride (Wilson et al., 2010; Harris et al., 2014; Christie and Cole, 2017; Thomson et al., 2020).

Social wellbeing and capital

The majority of studies (n = 5) reported that prior to the intervention participants were experiencing extreme social isolation. Participation facilitated social connection (n = 5), development of mutually supportive relationships (n = 4), and feelings of community, meaning, and belonging (Harris et al., 2014; Christie and Cole, 2017). Participants in most studies (n = 4) were required to cooperate on tasks and reported these shared experiences and goals “facilitated quick bonding” (Wilson et al., 2010); participants “looked forward to seeing each other” (Christie and Cole, 2017) and they assisted in creating “meaningful and lasting relationships” (Maund et al., 2019).

Participants in 4 studies reported it was beneficial to engage with others with a shared experience of MI (Harris et al., 2014; Christie and Cole, 2017; Maund et al., 2019; Thomson et al., 2020). For example, 1 participant stated “I think a lot of us probably feel like we don’t really fit in, I think here we just understand each other” (Maund et al., 2019) and another said “it was very important to relate to people, that we had a common ground factor, and that was our mental health experiencesI would never be able to have the same chats and the same connection and the same understanding and empathy” (Thomson et al., 2020).

Participants in 3 studies also reported the intervention helped them to develop social skills and confidence (e.g., communication skills). One study (Wilson et al., 2010) conducted follow-up assessment and reported some participants had commenced other support programs, volunteering work, and higher education. Similarly, participants in the study by Harris et al. (2014) reported that experiences in the community garden had helped develop friendships outside the garden due to shared interests in farming.

Connection to nature

Participants in 5 studies reported experiencing a profound connection to nature and the earth, saying things like “it’s something bigger than myself to be connected to” (Maund et al., 2019). In 2 studies participants’ developed new interest and confidence in accessing nature (Wilson et al., 2010; Maund et al., 2019), with 1 participant explaining that the intervention had “opened their eyes to nature and what was available on their doorstep” (Wilson et al., 2010). McEwan et al. (2019) considered 2 separate but related models in explaining these connections; the Nature Relatedness model (NR) (Nisbet et al., 2009) which describes “one’s appreciation for and understanding of our interconnectedness with all other living things on the earth” (p.718). Nature Connectedness (NC) which refers to an emotional, cognitive, and experiential connection with the natural environment (Mayer and Frantz, 2004). Increased noticing of nature in McEwan et al. (2019) significantly increased both NR and NC.

In 2 studies showing improved psychological wellbeing, participants attributed decreased anxiety to the natural environment which they described as peaceful, relaxing, tranquil, and calm (Christie and Cole, 2017; Maund et al., 2019). They also reported the natural beauty and fascination with fauna and flora offered a distraction from their problems. For example, participants in Maund et al. (2019) said “when you come here you are so engrossed in the animals and birds, all your troubles, they just disappear” (p. 9) and “I think walking in the wildflower garden was fabulous for me. All the beautiful variety of plants and flowers, lots of different colors” (p. 9). Clinicians in this study also reported that participants being distracted by natural stimuli that engaged all 5 senses was beneficial (Maund et al., 2019).

These findings in combination may have implications for the long-term impact of NT interventions for MI. By improving participant confidence in accessing natural environments and assisting them to find new connections to nature and earth, and supporting the appreciation of nature, interventions may become self-sustaining with participants continuing to access natural environments after the intervention and therefore having ongoing benefits. This finding was hinted at in 1 study where a participant stated “I wouldn’t have come somewhere like this by myselfbut now I have been because you all [study staff] supported me I think I would feel better about coming here again. Maybe not by myself but with a friend” (Maund et al., 2019). Even in the absence of continued NT, elements of Behavioural Activation (BA) and social connection associated with NT interventions may be sustained after the intervention. In support of this, Wilson et al. (2010) provided evidence of increase BA with participants leaving the house more often, completing more activities, and some commencing volunteering and education at follow-up. Improved social relationships was a consistent theme across studies, with 1 participant noting a clear link between natural environment and social isolation (Christie and Cole, 2017) which is consistent with our evolved innate desire for group inclusion and social interaction (Buss, 2019).

Social BA may be linked to participants experiencing social interaction in natural environments, creating feelings of familiarity and safety due to the calming effect of nature, as proposed by ART (Kaplan, 1995). It may also reduce social isolation through non-confrontational nature-based social activities, or simply because humans evolved to interact and gather resources in groups and as such spending time with other people in nature might be an activity that is hardwired (Buss, 2019).

Research methods and design of included studies

Setting

Of 6 unique studies, 5 were conducted in the UK and 1 in Queensland, Australia. Settings varied widely including a community garden (Australia), 2 woodland settings (homestead and outdoor), an inland wetland, museum, and metropolitan city. The studies included in this review lacked detailed information about the environmental features across the studies such as types of fauna and flora, still or running water, air quality, and weather. However, the setting of Harris et al. (2014) did facilitate growing culturally important vegetables (cassava, maize) which contributed to the apparent psychosocial impacts of the intervention, giving participants links to their country of origin, and allowing them to share their produce with others.

Design

Study designs included 2 qualitative (Harris et al., 2014; Christie and Cole, 2017): 3 mixed methods (Maund et al., 2019; Thomson et al., 2020) plus (Wilson et al., 2010, Wilson et al., 2011); and one RCT (McEwan et al., 2019). In general data were collected either during the intervention or immediately post-intervention, with only (McEwan et al., 2019) collecting any follow-up data. For the qualitative studies (Harris et al., 2014; Christie and Cole, 2017) data collection occurred during the intervention. Wilson et al. (2010, 2011), Maund et al. (2019) and Thomson et al. (2020) all conducted quasi-experiments with quantitative follow-up data collected immediately post-intervention, and qualitative straight after this; Thomson et al. (2020) attempted follow-up but only interviewed 1 participant at 3 and 6 months, and did not report this data. McEwan et al. (2019) conducted an RCT with quantitative data collection immediately pre-intervention, 7 days and 1 month follow-up.

Participants

Across the studies, data on participant characteristics was limited which has direct implications for research. A total of 334 participants were included across the 6 studies (accounting for overlap and dropouts), with samples ranging from 9 to 77 in face-to-face, and 164 in the Smartphone App (McEwan et al., 2019) intervention. Gender balance across studies varied considerably from 26% female (Wilson et al., 2011) to 86% female (Christie and Cole, 2017), although on average about 50% of participants were female across studies. Six papers did not provide full/any age data (participant mean age in Maund et al. (2019) was estimated based on age bracket data provided), and Christie and Cole (2017) had a large age range (35–67 years). Treatment response such as ethnicity or socio-economic status were poorly/not reported.

Recruitment

Reporting of recruitment strategies, referring services, and referral type was poor across studies. General information such as “Local Charities” or “Mental Health Workers” was often provided, except for Wilson et al. (2010) where referring services were specified but not described. Across studies, missing information included the size and scope of services and occupation of referrers (e.g., social workers versus faith-based community leaders), which has implications for participant characteristics such as severity of MI, multi-morbidity, and/or dual diagnosis.

Interventions

There was large variability in the format of interventions, including the time over which the intervention was conducted. The evaluation of GSP interventions that are conducted for as little as 1 week (McEwan et al., 2019) to over 1 year (Christie and Cole, 2017) has implications for practice and research. Face-to-face interventions usually ran from 6 to 12 weeks (n = 3) and were conducted in groups of approximately nine participants (n = 3), but the largest had over 45 participants (Harris et al., 2014). There was some overlap in activities across interventions; 4 interventions included horticulture such as gardening and weeding, 2 interventions included construction activities including small objects (e.g., bird boxes) and larger items (e.g., dry stone walls around garden beds), 3 included art and decorating activities, and 2 interventions included museum visits. Activities unique to specific interventions included canoeing (Maund et al., 2019) and the broad range of activities in Wilson et al. (2010) such as bush craft (e.g., orienteering, shelter building, campfire cooking).

Although there was variability in intervention types many shared hypothesized therapeutic mechanisms similar to those framed by nature connection theories. For example, therapeutic mechanisms in horticulture include completing meaningful and enjoyable tasks, community connection, social connection though shared experiences, and local environment connection (Relf, 1999; Shinew et al., 2004; Husk et al., 2016). These mechanisms of action were evident across all included studies (i.e., 5 included meaningful tasks, 5 included social connection, and 5 increased connection to nature).

In addition to variability in format and tasks, a potential confounder across studies was physical activity which evidence suggests can mediate the impact of NT on MI (Bélanger et al., 2019). However, in all studies the primary activity was exposure to nature and exercise was incidental. For example, in Maund et al. (2019) participants completed activities such as bird watching over an 800 m walk in 2 h; this speed is 8% of average adult walking pace and well below the required “brisk” pace (for any age group) where physical and mental health status are likely to improve (Ainsworth et al., 2011). Similarly, in Christie and Cole (2017), despite being referred to as an “exercise intervention” by the authors, participants could choose to complete non-exercise activities, and all of their included activities (e.g., gardening) were classified as nature-based rather than exercise-based in all other included studies. Additionally, in McEwan et al. (2019) the smartphone intervention did not affect participant physical activity and significantly improved wellbeing. Furthermore, participants frequently reported improved wellbeing from relaxing during non-exercise activities (i.e., viewing fauna and flora improved their mood).

Evaluations

These findings are associated with research and describes the quality of evaluations being undertaken in the included studies.

Qualitative evaluations

Trustworthiness of qualitative research indicates the meaningfulness and usefulness of results, also conceived of as the validity and relevance of results (Mays and Pope, 2000). The quality of evaluations was assessed according to the procedure of Nowell et al. (2017) as it is widely used and accepted as an appropriate assessment of quality (Stenfors et al., 2020). Quality was assessed based on the credibility, dependability, transferability, and confirmability of the research (Nowell et al., 2017). Trustworthiness of analysis was only explicitly considered by Christie and Cole (2017) who addressed the credibility of their findings by specifying that all authors independently generated codes/themes and analyzed data. Harris et al. (2014) did not explicitly mention trustworthiness but credibility was partially addressed as initial codes/themes were generated by one researcher which data was re-analyzed by a second. No other studies reported procedures that compared “triangulated” theme generation or analysis (Nowell et al., 2017).

Dependability i.e., replicability (Nowell et al., 2017), was limited across studies which all used inductive analyses but poorly reported any further details. Good quality qualitative research should provide detailed information about the research team, study design, data analysis, and findings (Tong et al., 2007) and this reporting was varied. For example, while the theoretical framework was well reported, interview settings and data collection methods (including questions and prompts) varied across studies, thereby limiting replicability and critique of study methods (Tong et al., 2007).

Transferability of findings to other populations/settings (Nowell et al., 2017) was limited by poor reporting of participant characteristics, sample sizes and consideration of data saturation (no new emerging themes) (Marshall et al., 2013).

Confirmability (neutrality of findings) across studies was difficult to assess as interviewer characteristics (gender, occupation, training, etc.) were not reported; this limits assessment of bias from researcher’s perspective such a background and so on (Tong et al., 2007; Nowell et al., 2017). Nonetheless, studies provided clear findings and sufficient participant quotes to provide context, which are central in determining study quality (Hong et al., 2018). This suggests that that despite limited reporting it is reasonable to assume the findings are at least somewhat trustworthy, and that future research should investigate these themes further.

Quantitative evaluations

Two studies (Maund et al., 2019; Thomson et al., 2020) used the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988) and 2 used the Warwick Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale (WEMWBS; Stewart-Brown and Janmohamed, 2008); no other questionnaires overlapped across studies (McEwan et al. (2019) used the ReQoL and Thomson et al. (2020) the UCL Museum Wellbeing Measure). The lack of a standard set of instruments makes it difficult to compare findings across studies and assess the statistical or clinical significance of findings. Small sample sizes and lack of statistical power was a limitation for quantitative studies (Maund et al., 2019; Thomson et al., 2020). Where the sample size of future studies allows, validated and widely used quantitative measures should be utilized.

Discussion

This review aimed to identify and critique peer-reviewed evidence for GSP for MI and produce recommendations for research and clinical practice. Results indicated a lack of peer-reviewed evidence in the area and generally poor reporting of research methods and outcomes in existing literature. However, included studies suggested that GSP may improve biopsychosocial wellbeing and connection to nature.

Study designs included 1 RCT with random assignment and independent variable manipulation with all others quasi-experiences and/or qualitative studies. Quantitative components of these studies were all immediately post-intervention and provided evidence for significant improvements in wellbeing. Whilst traditional thinking would suggest that the limited number of “Gold Standard” RCTs may make reliable evaluation of study outcomes difficult, it is not necessarily a problem. Evidence suggests that RCTs can be subject to design flaws, for example incorrect follow-up periods (Paraskevas et al., 2019) which is relevant here given the lack of follow-up in all but 1 study (McEwan et al., 2019) and lack of an established standard follow-up period in this area of research (Oh et al., 2017; Spano et al., 2020). Similarly, RCTs are not always representative of reality (Paraskevas et al., 2019) which may be true here as limited extant evidence in the area could impact content and construct validity, in which case the contribution of the qualitative studies may be more valuable in guiding future programs and research (Jiménez-Buedo and Russo, 2021). Additionally, given the nature of the interventions utilized, traditional RCT research design might prove difficult. For example, Christie and Cole (2017) examined participants that had attended a country homestead for over a year. A 1-year control group is not feasible due to ethical issues in delayed intervention for a waitlist control, limited resources, and lack of established “treatment as usual” for an active control in this emerging area of research. Additionally, the frequent use of retrospective evaluation design negates the opportunity for RCTs and limits opportunity for quantitative analysis. However, the qualitative studies provided rich data into the mechanisms of action and potential areas of improvement.

Qualitative evaluation methods were assessed using the widely accepted credibility, dependability, transferability, and confirmability framework (Sparkes and Smith, 2009; Nowell et al., 2017); in general, these aspects of the qualitative studies were poorly reported. Quantitative evaluations were limited by small sample sizes and a lack of standard set of instruments. However, assessment using the MMAT indicated that the studies were of overall good quality. For each study the research question was clear, data collection and analysis were appropriate to answer the question, and the findings/themes were well supported by quotes from participants. This suggests that despite limited reporting it is reasonable to assume the findings are at least somewhat trustworthy, and that future research should investigate these themes further.

Recruitment and referral strategies were also poorly reported, which has implications for practice and research. SP varies from signposting to link workers providing wraparound care (Kimberlee, 2015) but without this information reported it is impossible to know which referral pathways are effective for which groups of participants, and thus specific recommendations for future research and clinical practice are limited.

All studies reported improvements in psychological wellbeing including, mood, self-confidence, and self-worth, with participants indicating that a sense of escape and skill development were important in these improvements. Participants in most studies also reported reduced loneliness and social isolation, improved sense of community, meaning, and belonging. A recent study by Dobson et al. (2020) of an allotment gardening intervention in the UK duplicated these findings whereby participants shared food, knowledge and skills, and reported high social cohesion and a sense of community. This finding is also supported by a recent meta-analysis where horticulture interventions significantly improved psychosocial wellbeing (Spano et al., 2020), and more broadly, a review that found that even in urban settings NT interventions can improve social connectedness (Leavell et al., 2019).

Participants also reported improved social skills and confidence which appears to be in part due to a shared experience of MI that made participants feel more comfortable and confident interacting in groups, as though they were part of a safe “in-group,” for example saying they “just understand each other” (Maund et al., 2019) and “had a common ground factor” (Thomson et al., 2020). These findings are important for MI recovery as opportunities to socialize comfortably as part of the in-group and develop social skills may influence short and long-term outcomes (Hendryx et al., 2009). However, MI diagnoses were very poorly reported across studies allowing limited capacity to assess the relative impact of MI diagnosis in group cohesion. Similarly, poor reporting of demographics was a problem as it limited the ability to assess what in-groups may have formed within interventions, and identify how these could have affected outcomes, for example social isolation tends to increase with age (Fakoya et al., 2020), so the benefits of social interaction may have been more pronounced in older adults.

Despite apparent improvements in biopsychosocial wellbeing across studies, the generalizability of findings is limited due to extensive under-reporting. In particular limited reporting of MI diagnosis was common and is problematic as this can vastly affect the efficacy of interventions (Barlow, 2021) and it is impossible to ascertain the range and severity of MI within and between study samples. For example, McEwan et al. (2019) found greater improvements in QoL for participants with MI versus those without but could not offer any further insights into what works for whom. However, the nature of the populations and referring services of included studies somewhat explains this; for example, community-based welfare workers may identify participants who require intervention but do not have the skills to conduct a formal assessment and diagnosis. Or in the case of the Harris et al. (2014), migrant status was used to infer experiences of trauma and post-traumatic stress disorder but formal assessment of MI was not conducted due to potential to be distressing or (re)traumatizing (Blackmore et al., 2020). Finally, other participant characteristics that may impact treatment response such as ethnicity or socio-economic status were poorly/not reported. This is important as marginalized or disadvantaged groups have lower access to MI treatment, and these groups may have benefited more than others due to variable and somewhat limited existing treatments (Villatoro et al., 2018).

This review found that GSP increased perceived connection to nature which extant evidence indicates correlates strongly with mental wellbeing (Capaldi et al., 2014). Participants across studies delighted in seeing and interacting with fauna and flora which is consistent with the Affordances Framework of Brymer et al. (2020) where the experience of nature is related to existing and potential interactions rather than passive observation (Brymer et al., 2014, 2020). However, participants also reported that mindful emersion was also healing and restorative (Christie and Cole, 2017; Maund et al., 2019) which is consistent with ART (Kaplan, 1995) in which relaxed attention to natural objects (“soft fascination”) results in cognitive restoration as it allows unconscious processing that facilitates psychological healing and reduces cognitive overload (Daniel, 2014). This finding is unsurprising given the enormous body of evidence for the benefits of mindfulness, and its central role in other types of NT, for example Forest Therapy (Kotte et al., 2019). However, these theories are not mutually exclusive as mindful awareness can include all 5 senses i.e., touching, and therefore interacting with, natural objects. Future research could examine the benefits of both passive and active immersion in nature, and the efficacy for different groups of people, e.g., physical capacity for interaction. Similarly, clinical practice would benefit from considering the capacity and needs of clients when choosing more passive or active interventions for referral.

Study settings varying widely and were poorly reported, nonetheless results indicated that GSPs can be successful in a variety of environments. Variability between biomes does impact the feasibility of outdoor interventions, however, and future research could benefit from detailed reporting of intervention environment including inclement weather conditions (Goldman et al., 2020). ART and SRT suggest that beneficial environments should be restorative or unthreatening (Ulrich et al., 1991; Kaplan, 1995), however, other evidence indicates NT can have benefits regardless of biome (Burls, 2007). Therefore, future research needs to examine how the environment mediates the benefits of GSP interventions (for example is a thunderstorm intimidating or exhilarating). The wide variety of settings is consistent with the findings of Hartig et al. (2014) who state that there is no consistent definition of “nature” or “natural environments” in the research and thus NT interventions include anything from remote jungle to pot plants in urban apartments. There are also issues regarding classification of locations, for example even “allotment gardens and urban parks comprise natural features, appear natural, and provide opportunities to engage with and follow natural processes, but they are typically designed, constructed, regulated, and maintained” (Hartig et al., 2014, p.208).

Likewise, variability in type of NT intervention and therapeutic mechanisms (e.g., gardening vs. bird watching) may impact generalizability (Sempik et al., 2003), but given the reported benefits of NT across interventions it is likely exposure to, and engagement with, nature in general rather than specific activities improves wellbeing. The variability and flexibility of GSP activities, including adaptability to local population and biome is a major strength of this area as it can provide appropriate and effective interventions for a wider range of people. For example, the intervention setting of Harris et al. (2014) allowed for growing culturally important vegetables and illustrates the value of GSP in facilitating access and optimizing interventions for local community members, in this case culturally and linguistically diverse minorities (Harris et al., 2014). Similarly, even within local areas biomes can vary considerably as, for example, even small changes in elevation can impact the fauna and flora. This provides further opportunities for GSP to be matched to participant needs, e.g., not referring a client with hydrophobia to a coastal intervention when an inland one is also available.

Results demonstrated improvements in quantitative and qualitative measures of physical activity and increased incidental exercise. Whilst this review aimed to assess studies where exposure to nature and not physical activity were the primary therapeutic mechanisms, studies did include some exercise to differing extents which evidence suggests can mediate the impact of NT on MI (Bélanger et al., 2019). For example, activities in Wilson et al. (2010) ranged in physicality from minimal (e.g., wreath making) to more physically demanding (e.g., building shelters out of branches). However, exposure to nature has impacts in its own right which are above and beyond the benefits of exercise (Pretty et al., 2007; Nisbet et al., 2009; Gladwell et al., 2013; Araújo et al., 2019; Brymer et al., 2020), and as such assessment of the relative impacts of exercise versus nature-exposure in these studies is important. While even small amounts of incidental exercise can improve wellbeing (Rosenbaum et al., 2014; Stubbs et al., 2018) individuals with MI are more likely to have physical illness that may prevent any exercise (Firth et al., 2019) knowing what nature-exposure interventions work without an exercise component is important. Within this review it is impossible to assess the extent of exercise in most included studies as the proportion of participants and or/time spent across these activities is unreported. Furthermore, even if this activity was reported it would be impossible to make a meaningful comparison across studies e.g., building bird feeders (Christie and Cole, 2017) versus bird watching (Maund et al., 2019). The heterogenous nature of NT and difficulty separating the impact of physical activity mean this generalizability and comparability will always be a limitation, however, detailed reporting in future research could somewhat address this problem. Additionally, careful consideration of the quantity and quality of incidental exercise is important for clinical practice, given the barriers to participation that this could present (Firth et al., 2016).

These findings suggest GSP can act as an effective form of diversional therapy for people with MI, providing opportunities for escape, leisure and socialization for participants, and reducing the impacts of loneliness and other social determinants of health. As there is some evidence that GSP may provide additional benefit where activities incorporate skill development and mastery, provide a routine, and facilitate social engagement, it would be of value for future research to evaluate the benefits of longer-term NT interventions with regular, ongoing sessions that incorporate these elements.

Limitations

A limitation of this review is indicative of this area of the research in general in that many NT interventions have a physical activity component; this limits ability to identify the therapeutic impact of nature exposure alone and excludes potentially informative studies from this review. For example, a recent quantitative prospective study by Smyth et al. (2022) involved a SP “Green Gym” intervention with 892 participants with MI and/or long-term conditions, and showed significant improvements in participant wellbeing, with greater improvements for participants with poorer baseline wellbeing. This study used the WEMWBS (Stewart-Brown and Janmohamed, 2008) which is used in 2 included studies (Wilson et al., 2011; Maund et al., 2019), and included an appropriately long 13-month follow-up. Despite being a rigorous study in a peer-reviewed journal it does not meet the inclusion criteria for this review due to its primary focus on physical activity. However, this intervention included many activities that overlap with those in this review, e.g., gardening, weeding, and learning about local species (Smyth et al., 2022). It is also similar to that of Christie and Cole (2017) in which all activities are opt-in and range from learning about local species (very low physical activity requirements) to tree planting (high physical activity requirements). Smyth et al. (2022) and many papers in this review included incidental exercise, however, a greater focus on physical activity meant it did not strictly fit the inclusion criteria. Similarly Lindsay et al. (2022) conducted a novel SP fishing intervention in order to identify barriers to participation in GSP for people with long-term conditions. However, participant diagnoses were not collected or reported which precluded its inclusion in this review and also the conclusions that can be drawn about the efficacy of this novel GSP interventions for MI. This is both a limitation of this review and area of research.

An additional limitation of this review is that it only considered English-language literature; this may be a problem due to the emergence of types of NT particularly Forest Therapy in non-English-speaking countries such as Japan, China, and Korea. However, evidence suggests that excluding non-English studies generally does not impact the outcomes of reviews (Jüni et al., 2002).

Recommendations

This review aimed to identify and critique extant GSP interventions in order to make recommendations for future research and clinical practice. These are as follows:

Design and assessment

Stronger research designs including RCTs, improve reporting of qualitative assessments, and implement follow-up assessments. Clinicians should assess participant wellbeing pre- and post-intervention and consider publication to improve the evidence base.

Recruitment

Reporting of referral pathways in detail and examine possible mediation effects. Clinicians should adapt the type and intensity of referral to individual client needs.

Participant characteristics

Reporting of detailed demographic information and consider stratifying groups to explore demographics as mediators of effects. Clinicians should consider cohort demographics when referring to GSP programs to increase group cohesion.

Setting

Reporting of environmental conditions in detail so a meaningful evidence base can be developed. Clinicians should consider local biome variability and match this to client needs.

Intervention characteristics

Incorporation of skill development/sharing into GSP interventions. Clinicians should match this aspect of interventions to participant capacity and interests. Additionally, researchers should examine the benefits of both passive and active immersion in nature in particular mindfulness versus active engagement. Clinicians should consider client capacity when choosing GSP programs.

Physical activity

Careful planning and reporting of the quality and quantity of physical activity within GSP interventions. Clinicians should consider physical activity capacity when referring clients.

Conclusion

The primary finding of this review was that chronic under-reporting limits capacity to inform research and practice. In particular, intervention setting, participant characteristics, and recruitment strategies were poorly described, and without clarity on the “where, who, and how” it is difficult for these findings to be used to develop meaningful research and clinical practice recommendations. However, this review demonstrates that GSP can improve biopsychosocial wellbeing across a large variety of locations and intervention types. We conclude that future research in this growing area is worthwhile, but that detailed reporting of research methods is essential in order to develop a solid evidence base that can move the area forward. In terms of clinical practice, we conclude that GSP can improve biopsychosocial wellbeing in adults with MI, but unfortunately evidence is unclear on what works best for different groups. However, given the large variety in intervention types and activity levels we suggest that clinicians consider the needs and ability of the client before deciding which intervention to prescribe.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

TT, CA, DM, and JB: conceptualization and methodology. TT, CA, DM, JB, MT, and DD’A: validation. TT, MT, and DD’A: formal analysis, investigation, and data curation. TT, CA, DM, JB, MT, DD’A, and EB: writing – original draft. TT, CA, JB, MT, DD’A, and EB: writing – review and editing. TT: visualization and project administration. CA, DM, JB, and EB: supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Funding for open access publication will come from Primary and Community Care Services, for which author JB is CEO.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the contribution of Carlie Nekrasov (Librarian, Southern Cross University) for her help in developing the search strategy for this review.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1041675/full#supplementary-material

References

Adevi, A. A., and Mårtensson, F. (2013). Stress rehabilitation through garden therapy: The garden as a place in the recovery from stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 12, 230–237. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2013.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainsworth, B. E., Haskell, W. L., Herrmann, S. D., Meckes, N., Bassett, D. R., Tudor-Locke, C., et al. (2011). 2011 Compendium of physical activities: A second update of codes and MET values. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 43, 1575–1581. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31821ece12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcoholics Anonymous (2021). Welcome to alcoholics anonymous. Ireland: Alcoholics Anonymous GSO.

Google Scholar

Allen, J., Balfour, R., Bell, R., and Marmot, M. (2014). Social determinants of mental health. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 26, 392–407.

Google Scholar

Allen, S. (2020). 2020 global health care outlook: Laying a foundation for the future. Australia: Deloitte Australia.

Google Scholar

Araújo, D., Brymer, E., Brito, H., Withagen, R., and Davids, K. (2019). The empowering variability of affordances of nature: Why do exercisers feel better after performing the same exercise in natural environments than in indoor environments? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 42, 138–145.

Google Scholar

Ayorinde, A. A., Williams, I., Mannion, R., Song, F., Skrybant, M., Lilford, R. J., et al. (2020). Assessment of publication bias and outcome reporting bias in systematic reviews of health services and delivery research: A meta-epidemiological study. PLoS One 15:e0227580. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, D. H. (2021). Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual. New York, NY: Guilford publications.

Google Scholar

Bélanger, M., Gallant, F., Doré, I., O’Loughlin, J. L., Sylvestre, M.-P., Abi Nader, P., et al. (2019). Physical activity mediates the relationship between outdoor time and mental health. Prev. Med. Rep. 16:101006. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2019.101006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., and Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1207–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, M. G., Kross, E., Krpan, K. M., Askren, M. K., Burson, A., Deldin, P. J., et al. (2012). Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J. Affect. Disorder. 140, 300–305.

Google Scholar

Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 249–259.

Google Scholar

Blackmore, R., Boyle, J. A., Fazel, M., Ranasinha, S., Gray, K. M., Fitzgerald, G., et al. (2020). The prevalence of mental illness in refugees and asylum seekers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 17:e1003337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bragg, R., and Leck, C. (2017). Good practice in social prescribing for mental health: The role of nature-based interventions. York: Natural England Commissioned Reports.

Google Scholar

Britton, E., Kindermann, G., Domegan, C., and Carlin, C. (2018). Blue care: a systematic review of blue space interventions for health and wellbeing. Health Promot. Int. 35, 50–69. doi: 10.1093/heapro/day103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, E., Kindermann, G., Domegan, C., and Carlin, C. (2020). Blue care: a systematic review of blue space interventions for health and wellbeing. Health Promot. Int. 35, 50–69.

Google Scholar

Brymer, E., Araújo, D., Davids, K., and Pepping, G.-J. (2020). Conceptualizing the human health outcomes of acting in natural environments: an ecological perspective. Front. Psychol. 11:1362. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brymer, E., Davids, K., and Mallabon, L. (2014). Understanding the psychological health and well-being benefits of physical activity in nature: An ecological dynamics analysis. Ecopsychology 6, 189–197.

Google Scholar

Burls, A. (2007). People and green spaces: promoting public health and mental well-being through ecotherapy. J. Public Men. Health 6, 24–39. doi: 10.1108/17465729200700018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. (2019). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Capaldi, C. A., Dopko, R. L., and Zelenski, J. M. (2014). The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 5:976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charrois, T. L. (2015). Systematic reviews: what do you need to know to get started? Can. J. Hosp. Pharm. 68, 144–148. doi: 10.4212/cjhp.v68i2.1440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatterjee, H. J., Camic, P. M., Lockyer, B., and Thomson, L. J. (2018). Non-clinical community interventions: a systematised review of social prescribing schemes. Arts Health 10, 97–123. doi: 10.1080/17533015.2017.1334002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaudhury, P., and Banerjee, D. (2020). “Recovering with nature”: A review of ecotherapy and implications for the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Public Health 8:604440. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.604440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christie, M. A., and Cole, F. (2017). The impact of green exercise on volunteers’ mental health and wellbeing – Findings from a community project in a woodland setting. J. Ther. Hortic. 27, 16–33. doi: 10.2307/26111989

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarivate (2013). EndNote X9. Philadelphia, PA: Clarivate.

Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., and Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385–396.

Google Scholar

Daniel, R. M. (2014). The Effects of the natural environment on attention restoration, ed. L. Emery (Boone, NC: Appalachian State University).

Google Scholar

De Vries, S., Van Dillen, S. M., Groenewegen, P. P., and Spreeuwenberg, P. (2013). Streetscape greenery and health: Stress, social cohesion and physical activity as mediators. Soc. Sci. Med. 94, 26–33.

Google Scholar

Dobson, M. C., Reynolds, C., Warren, P. H., and Edmondson, J. L. (2020). My little piece of the planet”: the multiplicity of well-being benefits from allotment gardening. Br. Food J. 123, 1012–1023. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-07-2020-0593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, N. E., and Beck, A. M. (2002). Animal-assisted therapy and nutrition in Alzheimer’s disease. Western J. Nurs. Res. 24, 697–712.

Google Scholar

Fakoya, O. A., McCorry, N. K., and Donnelly, M. (2020). Loneliness and social isolation interventions for older adults: A scoping review of reviews. BMC Public Health 20:129. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-8251-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, J., Rosenbaum, S., Stubbs, B., Gorczynski, P., Yung, A. R., and Vancampfort, D. (2016). Motivating factors and barriers towards exercise in severe mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 46, 2869–2881. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, J., Siddiqi, N., Koyanagi, A., Siskind, D., Rosenbaum, S., Galletly, C., et al. (2019). The Lancet Psychiatry Commission: a blueprint for protecting physical health in people with mental illness. Lancet Psychiatry 6, 675–712. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30132-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franco, L. S., Shanahan, D. F., and Fuller, R. A. (2017). A review of the benefits of nature experiences: More than meets the eye. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 14, 864. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14080864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Mitra, R., Franks, L., Richter, A., and Rockliff, H. (2008). Feeling safe and content: A specific affect regulation system? Relationship to depression, anxiety, stress, and self-criticism. J. Posit. Psychol. 3, 182–191. doi: 10.1080/17439760801999461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gladwell, V. F., Brown, D. K., Wood, C., Sandercock, G. R., and Barton, J. L. (2013). The great outdoors: how a green exercise environment can benefit all. Extreme Physiol. Med. 2, 1–7. doi: 10.1186/2046-7648-2-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, M., Flynn, A., Cartwright, A., Bell, R., Davis, S., Bui, D., et al. (2020). Bushfire smoke impact survey 2019-2020. Chatswood: Asthma Australia.

Google Scholar

Gupta, A. K. (2021). Social prescribing in ethnic minority communities. Br. J. Gen. Pract. 71:109. doi: 10.3399/bjgp21X715037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, N., Minniss, F. R., and Somerset, S. (2014). Refugees connecting with a new country through community food gardening. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 11, 9202–9216. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110909202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T., and Jahncke, H. (2017). Attention restoration in natural environments: Mixed mythical metaphors for meta-analysis. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B 20, 305–315. doi: 10.1080/10937404.2017.1363101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T., Mang, M., and Evans, G. W. (1991). Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ. Behav. 23, 3–26. doi: 10.1177/0013916591231001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T., Mitchell, R., De Vries, S., and Frumkin, H. (2014). Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 35, 207–228.

Google Scholar

Hendryx, M., Green, C. A., and Perrin, N. A. (2009). Social support, activities, and recovery from serious mental illness: STARS study findings. J. Behav. Health Serv. Res. 36, 320–329. doi: 10.1007/s11414-008-9151-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, Q. N., Pluye, P., Fàbregues, S., Bartlett, G., Boardman, F., Cargo, M., et al. (2018). Mixed methods appraisal tool (MMAT), version 2018. Montreal: Department of Family Medicine McGill University.

Google Scholar

Husk, K., Elston, J., Gradinger, F., Callaghan, L., and Asthana, S. (2019). Social prescribing: Where is the evidence? Br. J. Gen. Pract. 69, 6–7. doi: 10.3399/bjgp19X700325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Husk, K., Lovell, R., Cooper, C., Stahl-Timmins, W., and Garside, R. (2016). Participation in environmental enhancement and conservation activities for health and well-being in adults: A review of quantitative and qualitative evidence. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. CD010351. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD010351.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiménez-Buedo, M., and Russo, F. (2021). Experimental practices and objectivity in the social sciences: re-embedding construct validity in the internal–external validity distinction. Synthese 199, 9549–9579.

Google Scholar

Joye, Y., and Van den Berg, A. (2011). Is love for green in our genes? A critical analysis of evolutionary assumptions in restorative environments research. Urban Forest. Urban Green. 10, 261–268.

Google Scholar

Julius, H., Beetz, A., Kotrschal, K., Turner, D., and Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (2012). Attachment to pets: An integrative view of human-animal relationships with implications for therapeutic practice. Newburyport, MA: Hogrefe Publishing.

Google Scholar

Jüni, P., Holenstein, F., Sterne, J., Bartlett, C., and Egger, M. (2002). Direction and impact of language bias in meta-analyses of controlled trials: Empirical study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 31, 115–123. doi: 10.1093/ije/31.1.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, R., and Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge: CUP Archive.

Google Scholar

Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 15, 169–182. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, S., and Peterson, C. (1993). Health and environment: A psychological analysis. Landsc. Urban Plan. 26, 17–23.

Google Scholar

Keetharuth, A. D., Brazier, J., Connell, J., Bjorner, J. B., Carlton, J., Buck, E. T., et al. (2018). Recovering Quality of Life (ReQoL): A new generic self-reported outcome measure for use with people experiencing mental health difficulties. Br. J. Psychiatry 212, 42–49. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2017.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellert, S. R. (1997). The value of life: Biological diversity and human society. Washington, DC: Island press.

Google Scholar

Kier Business Services Limited (2016). NHS Leeds west clinical commissioning group: Patient empowerment project final evaluation report. Salford: Kier Business Services Limited.

Google Scholar

Kimberlee, R. (2015). What is social prescribing? Adv. Soc. Sci. Res. J. 2.

Google Scholar

Knapp, M., Bauer, A., Perkins, M., and Snell, T. (2013). Building community capital in social care: is there an economic case? Commun. Dev. J. 48, 313–331. doi: 10.1093/cdj/bss021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosinski, M., Ware, J. E., Turner-Bowker, D. M., and Gandek, B. (2007). User’s manual for the SF-12v2 health survey: with a supplement documenting the SF-12§health survey. Johnston, RI: QualityMetric Incorporated.

Google Scholar

Kotera, Y., Richardson, M., and Sheffield, D. (2022). Effects of shinrin-yoku (forest bathing) and nature therapy on mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Men. Health Addict. 20, 337–361. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00363-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotte, D., Michalsen, A., Shin, W. S., and Qing, L. (2019). International handbook of forest therapy. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publisher.

Google Scholar

Leavell, M. A., Leiferman, J. A., Gascon, M., Braddick, F., Gonzalez, J. C., and Litt, J. S. (2019). Nature-based social prescribing in urban settings to improve social connectedness and mental well-being: A Review. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 6, 297–308. doi: 10.1007/s40572-019-00251-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Park, B.-J., Tsunetsugu, Y., Kagawa, T., and Miyazaki, Y. (2009). Restorative effects of viewing real forest landscapes, based on a comparison with urban landscapes. Scandin. J. Forest Res. 24, 227–234.

Google Scholar

Lindsay, R. K., Carmichael, C., Allen, P. M., Fossey, M., Godier-McBard, L., Butler, L., et al. (2022). Fishing participation, motivators and barriers among UK anglers with disabilities: Opportunities and implications for green social prescribing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:4730. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19084730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowther, M., Mutrie, N., Loughlan, C., and McFarlane, C. (1999). Development of a scottish physical activity questionnaire: A tool for use in physical activity interventions. Br. J. Sports Med. 33, 244–249.

Google Scholar

Marshall, B., Cardon, P., Poddar, A., and Fontenot, R. (2013). Does sample size matter in qualitative research?: A review of qualitative interviews in IS research. J. Comput. Inform. Syst. 54, 11–22.

Google Scholar

Maund, P. R., Irvine, K. N., Reeves, J., Strong, E., Cromie, R., Dallimer, M., et al. (2019). Wetlands for wellbeing: Piloting a nature-based health intervention for the management of anxiety and depression. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:4413. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16224413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, F. S., and Frantz, C. M. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 24, 503–515. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mays, N., and Pope, C. (2000). Assessing quality in qualitative research. BMJ 320, 50–52.

Google Scholar

McEwan, K., Richardson, M., Sheffield, D., Ferguson, F. J., and Brindley, P. (2019). A smartphone app for improving mental health through connecting with Urban Nature. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:3373. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16183373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MFMER. (2002). “Mental illness in children: Know the signs,” in Healthy lifestyle: Children’s health, (Rochester, MN: Mayo Foundation for Medical Education and Research).

Google Scholar

Microsoft (2022). Microsoft Word. Redmond, WA: Microsoft.

Google Scholar

Morris, D., Thomas, P., Ridley, J., and Webber, M. (2022). Community-enhanced social prescribing: Integrating community in policy and practice. Int. J. Commun. Well Being 5, 179–195. doi: 10.1007/s42413-020-00080-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Health Service [NHS] (2019). The NHS long term plan. Rome: National Health Service.

Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., and Murphy, S. A. (2008). The nature relatedness scale: Linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 41, 715–740. doi: 10.1177/0013916508318748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., and Murphy, S. A. (2009). The nature relatedness scale: Linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 41, 715–740.

Google Scholar

Nowell, L. S., Norris, J. M., White, D. E., and Moules, N. J. (2017). Thematic analysis: Striving to meet the trustworthiness criteria. Int. J. Qualitat. Methods 16, 1–13. doi: 10.1177/1609406917733847

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, B., Lee, K. J., Zaslawski, C., Yeung, A., Rosenthal, D., Larkey, L., et al. (2017). Health and well-being benefits of spending time in forests: Systematic review. Environ. Health Prevent. Med. 22:71. doi: 10.1186/s12199-017-0677-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohly, H., White, M. P., Wheeler, B. W., Bethel, A., Ukoumunne, O. C., Nikolaou, V., et al. (2016). Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health, Part B 19, 305–343.

Google Scholar

Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development [OECD] (2021). A new benchmark for mental health systems: Tackling the social and economic costs of mental Ill-health. Paris: Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Developmen.

Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paraskevas, K. I., de Borst, G. J., and Veith, F. J. (2019). Why randomized controlled trials do not always reflect reality. J. Vascular Surg. 70, 607.–614.

Google Scholar

Parkinson, J. (2006). Establishing national mental health and well-being indicators for Scotland. J. Public Ment. Health 5, 42–48.

Google Scholar

Parkrun Global (2021). Welcome to Parkrun. Available online at: https://www.parkrun.com/ (Accessed 26 May, 2021).

Google Scholar

Peters, M., Godfrey, C., McInerney, P., Baldini Soares, C., Khalil, H., and Parker, D. (2017). “Chapter 11: Scoping reviews,” in Joanna briggs institute reviewer’s manual, eds E. Aromataris and Z. Munn (Adelaide: JBI).

Google Scholar

Peters, M., Marnie, C., Tricco, A. C., Pollock, D., Munn, Z., Alexander, L., et al. (2020). Updated methodological guidance for the conduct of scoping reviews. JBI Evid. Synth. 18, 2119–2126. doi: 10.11124/jbies-20-00167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pretty, J., Peacock, J., Hine, R., Sellens, M., South, N., and Griffin, M. (2007). Green exercise in the UK countryside: Effects on health and psychological well-being, and implications for policy and planning. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 50, 211–231.

Google Scholar

Rehm, J., and Shield, K. D. (2019). Global burden of disease and the impact of mental and addictive disorders. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 21, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s11920-019-0997-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Relf, P. D. (1999). The role of horticulture in human well-being and quality of life. J. Therap. Horticult. 10, 10–14. doi: 10.5860/choice.30-3273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, S., Tiedemann, A., Sherrington, C., Curtis, J., and Ward, P. B. (2014). Physical activity interventions for people with mental illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychiatry 75, 964–974. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13r08765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, L. (2012). Behavioral disorders: The new public health crisis. J. Behav. Health Serv. Res. 39, 1–2. doi: 10.1007/s11414-011-9265-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubio, D. M., Schoenbaum, E. E., Lee, L. S., Schteingart, D. E., Marantz, P. R., Anderson, K. E., et al. (2010). Defining translational research: Implications for training. Acad. Med. J. Assoc. Am. Med. Coll. 85, 470–475. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e3181ccd618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutko, E. A., and Gillespie, J. (2013). Where’s the wilderness in wilderness therapy? J. Exp. Educ. 36, 218–232. doi: 10.1177/1053825913489107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Weinstein, N., Bernstein, J., Brown, K. W., Mistretta, L., and Gagné, M. (2010). Vitalizing effects of being outdoors and in nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W., Shriver, C., Tabanico, J. J., and Khazian, A. M. (2004). Implicit connections with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 24, 31–42. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00022-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sempik, J., Aldridge, J., and Becker, S. (2003). Social and therapeutic horticulture: Evidence and messages from research. Loughborough: Loughborough University.

Google Scholar

Shanahan, D. F., Astell–Burt, T., Barber, E. A., Brymer, E., Cox, D. T., Dean, J., et al. (2019). Nature–based interventions for improving health and wellbeing: The purpose, the people and the outcomes. Sports 7:141. doi: 10.3390/sports7060141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shinew, K. J., Glover, T. D., and Parry, D. C. (2004). Leisure spaces as potential sites for interracial interaction: Community gardens in urban areas. J. Leis. Res. 36, 336–355. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2004.11950027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smyth, N., Thorn, L., Wood, C., Hall, D., and Lister, C. (2022). Increased wellbeing following engagement in a group nature-based programme: The green gym programme delivered by the conservation volunteers. Healthcare (Basel) 10:978. doi: 10.3390/healthcare10060978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spano, G., D’Este, M., Giannico, V., Carrus, G., Elia, M., Lafortezza, R., et al. (2020). Are community gardening and horticultural interventions beneficial for psychosocial well-being? A Meta analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:3584. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17103584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparkes, A. C., and Smith, B. (2009). Judging the quality of qualitative inquiry: Criteriology and relativism in action. Psychol. Sport Exercise 10, 491–497. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B., and Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Arch. Int. Med. 166, 1092–1097.

Google Scholar

Stenfors, T., Kajamaa, A., and Bennett, D. (2020). How to…assess the quality of qualitative research. Clin. Teach. 17, 596–599.

Google Scholar

Stewart-Brown, S., and Janmohamed, K. (2008). Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale. Coventry: Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick.

Google Scholar

Stubbs, B., Vancampfort, D., Hallgren, M., Firth, J., Veronese, N., Solmi, M., et al. (2018). EPA guidance on physical activity as a treatment for severe mental illness: A meta-review of the evidence and position statement from the european psychiatric association (EPA), supported by the International organization of physical therapists in mental health (IOPTMH). Eur. Psychiatry 54, 124–144. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiyama, T., Leslie, E., Giles-Corti, B., and Owen, N. (2008). Associations of neighbourhood greenness with physical and mental health: Do walking, social coherence and local social interaction explain the relationships? J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 62, e9–e9. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.064287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The Australian National University. (2019). Pets & animal-assisted therapy. Canberra: ANU.

Google Scholar

The National Health Service [NHS] (2015). Patient empowerment project (PEP). Rome: The National Health Service.

Google Scholar

The National Health Service [NHS] (2016). Patient empowerment project (PEP). Leeds: Leeds West Clinical Commissioning Group.

Google Scholar

Thomson, L. J., and Chatterjee, H. J. (2013). UCL museum wellbeing measures toolkit. London: AHRC, 28.

Google Scholar

Thomson, L. J., and Chatterjee, H. J. (2015). Measuring the impact of museum activities on well-being: Developing the museum well-being measures toolkit. Museum Manag. Curatorship 30, 44–62.

Google Scholar

Thomson, L. J., Morse, N., Elsden, E., and Chatterjee, H. J. (2020). Art, nature and mental health: Assessing the biopsychosocial effects of a ‘creative green prescription’ museum programme involving horticulture, artmaking and collections. Perspect Public Health 140, 277–285. doi: 10.1177/1757913920910443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tierney, S., Wong, G., Roberts, N., Boylan, A.-M., Park, S., Abrams, R., et al. (2020). Supporting social prescribing in primary care by linking people to local assets: A realist review. BMC Med. 18:49. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-1510-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, A., Sainsbury, P., and Craig, J. (2007). Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): A 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. Int. J. Qual. Health Care 19, 349–357. doi: 10.1093/intqhc/mzm042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tricco, A. C., Lillie, E., Zarin, W., O’Brien, K. K., Colquhoun, H., Levac, D., et al. (2018). PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and explanation. Ann. Int. Med. 169, 467–473. doi: 10.7326/M18-0850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twohig-Bennett, C., and Jones, A. (2018). The health benefits of the great outdoors: A systematic review and meta-analysis of greenspace exposure and health outcomes. Environ. Res. 166, 628–637. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.06.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230.

Google Scholar

Van den Berg, A. E., Joye, Y., and Koole, S. L. (2016). Why viewing nature is more fascinating and restorative than viewing buildings: A closer look at perceived complexity. Urban For. Urban Green. 20, 397–401.

Google Scholar

Vigo, D., Thornicroft, G., and Atun, R. (2016). Estimating the true global burden of mental illness. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 171–178. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00505-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villatoro, A. P., Mays, V. M., Ponce, N. A., and Aneshensel, C. S. (2018). Perceived need for mental health care: The intersection of race, ethnicity, gender, and socioeconomic status. Soc. Ment. Health 8, 1–24. doi: 10.1177/2156869317718889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ware, J., Kosinski, M., Turner-Bowker, D., and Gandeck, B. (2007). QualiMetric user’s manual for the SF-12v2TM Health Survey. Lincoln, NE: QualiMetric Incorporated.

Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54:1063. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

WHOQOL Group (1993). Study protocol for the world health organization project to develop a quality of life assessment instrument (WHOQOL). Qual. Life Res. 2, 153–159.

Google Scholar

Wilson (1984). Biophilia: The human bond with other species. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Wilson, Fleming, S., Jones, R., Lafferty, K., Cathrine, K., Seaman, P., et al. (2010). Green shoots of recovery: The impact of a mental health ecotherapy programme. Ment. Health Rev. J. 15, 4–14. doi: 10.5042/mhrj.2010.0366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, N. W., Jones, R., Fleming, S., Lafferty, K., Knifton, L., Cathrine, K., et al. (2011). Branching out: The impact of a mental health ecotherapy program. 3, 51–57. doi: 10.1089/eco.2010.0049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organisation [WHO] (2004). Promoting mental health: Concepts, emerging evidence, practice. Geneva: WHO.

Google Scholar

World Health Organisation [WHO] (2021). Mental health action plan 2013–2030 (extended to 2030). Geneva: WHO.

Google Scholar

World Health Organisation [WHO] (2022). Mental disorders. Geneva: WHO.

Google Scholar

Zambas, S. I., and Wright, J. (2016). Impact of colonialism on Mâori and Aboriginal healthcare access: A discussion paper. Contempo. Nurse 52, 398–409. doi: 10.1080/10376178.2016.1195238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeh, P., Sandhu, H. K., Cannaby, A. M., and Sturt, J. A. (2014). Cultural barriers impeding ethnic minority groups from accessing effective diabetes care services: A systematic review of observational studies. Divers Equal Health Care 11, 9–33.

Google Scholar

Keywords: social prescribing, community referral, community health services, mental illness, nature therapy, ecotherapy

Citation: Thomas T, Aggar C, Baker J, Massey D, Thomas M, D’Appio D and Brymer E (2022) Social prescribing of nature therapy for adults with mental illness living in the community: A scoping review of peer-reviewed international evidence. Front. Psychol. 13:1041675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1041675

Received: 11 September 2022; Accepted: 16 November 2022;
Published: 06 December 2022.

Edited by:

Annalisa Setti, University College Cork, Ireland

Reviewed by:

Marica Cassarino, University College Cork, Ireland
Simon Bell, University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Thomas, Aggar, Baker, Massey, Thomas, D’Appio and Brymer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tamsin Thomas, t.thomas.26@student.scu.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.