Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 February 2022
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Parenting in the Context of Opioid Use: Mechanisms, Prevention Solutions, and Policy Implications View all 14 articles

Reduced Child-Oriented Face Mirroring Brain Responses in Mothers With Opioid Use Disorder: An Exploratory Study

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health, Renaissance School of Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Obstetrics, Gynecology, and Reproductive Medicine, Renaissance School of Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, United States
  • 4Program in Public Health, Renaissance School of Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States

While the prevalence of opioid use disorder (OUD) among pregnant women has multiplied in the United States in the last decade, buprenorphine treatment (BT) for peripartum women with OUD has been administered to reduce risks of repeated cycles of craving and withdrawal. However, the maternal behavior and bonding in mothers with OUD may be altered as the underlying maternal behavior neurocircuit (MBN) is opioid sensitive. In the regulation of rodent maternal behaviors such as licking and grooming, a series of opioid-sensitive brain regions are functionally connected, including the ventral pallidum (VP). In humans, these brain regions, interact with the supplementary motor area (SMA) to regulate maternal behaviors and are functionally dysregulated by opioids. It is unclear how these brain regions respond to the emotions of their child for mothers receiving BT. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) pilot study in 22 mothers within the first postpartum year, including six mothers receiving BT and 16 non-OUD mothers as a comparison group (CG), we devised a child face mirroring task in fMRI settings to assess maternal responses to pictures of facial expressions of own child and an unknown child in an empathic mirroring condition (Join) and a non-mirroring observation condition (Observe). In each condition, faces of neutral, ambiguous, distressed, and joyful expressions of each child were repeatedly displayed in a random order. The response of SMA during empathic mirroring (Join) vs. non-mirroring (Observe) of own child was reduced among BT/OUD vs. CG. Within MBN, the left VP, critical for parental sensitivity, had a similar deficit. This study outlines potential mechanisms for investigating the risks of deficits in the neural responses to actual maternal sensitivity and parenting behavior in mothers with OUD, and potential targets for interventions that reduce stress and augment maternal behavior and child outcome.

Introduction

Every day in the United States, approximately 200 people die after overdosing on opioids (CDC/NCHS, 2021). The incidence of pregnant women with opioid use disorder (OUD) quadruped from 1999 to 2014 (from 1.5/1,000 delivery hospitalizations to 6.5) (Haight et al., 2018). In this epidemic, 2.5% of pregnant women use opioids chronically (Krans and Patrick, 2016) such that about 100,000 postpartum women and their families are afflicted with OUD every year. However, pregnant women with OUD may receive “gold standard” buprenorphine treatment (BT) for withdrawal (Jones et al., 2012; Nanda et al., 2015; Krans et al., 2016; Rosenthal et al., 2016; Zedler et al., 2016). Buprenorphine is a semisynthetic morphine-derived opioid used to treat OUD and chronic pain with very high affinity for the μ-receptor as a partial agonist and high affinity for the κ-receptor as an antagonist. Despite withdrawal reduction with BT, pregnant and postpartum women remain at high risk for problems, for which treatment is lacking. Indeed, relapse is common, with comorbid high stress, depression, polysubstance use, and maladaptive parenting behaviors (Rutherford et al., 2011; Rutherford and Mayes, 2017; Swain and Ho, 2019; Swain et al., 2019) risking child maltreatment and costly foster care utilization (Conway et al., 2006). Thus far in humans, however, there is still little research on mother and child bonding and health with buprenorphine treatment for OUD (Salihu et al., 2019).

Of additional concern to mothers with OUD, exogenous opioid-induced deficits have been shown for maternal behaviors in animal models (Bridges and Grimm, 1982; Grimm and Bridges, 1983; Slamberova et al., 2001). At least in part, these effects appear to be mediated by the activation of μ-opioid receptors in the hypothalamic (HYP) medial preoptic area (mPOA) (Rubin and Bridges, 1984; Mann et al., 1991; Stafisso-Sandoz et al., 1998). As part of the opioid-sensitive brain, the mPOA regulates a series of neurocircuits in the regulation of many salient behavioral outputs (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2015). For rodent maternal behaviors, the HYP normally activates the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and ventral pallidum (VP) (Numan and Young, 2016). Human mothers have a homologous and adaptable maternal behavior neurocircuit (MBN) as outlined by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and responses to infant stimuli (Swain et al., 2007, 2019; Swain and Lorberbaum, 2008; Kim et al., 2016; Swain and Ho, 2017).

The MBN regulates mother–infant bonding, balances sensitive caring vs. aggressive defensive maternal behaviors in humans and other mammals, and adapts to a variety of circumstances (Swain and Ho, 2019). In addition to the mPOA and VP, many other MBN areas are sensitive to exogenous opioids (Wallin et al., 2021), including the NAc, VTA for parental care, and PAG for parental defensive behavior. In brain models extended to include substance use, the VP has previously been proposed as a common pathway for drug seeking initiated by stress, drug-associated cues, or the drug itself (Kalivas and Volkow, 2005). In fact, normal function in the VP is extremely important for discriminating between natural and exogenous drug-related rewards (Root et al., 2015). In preclinical animal models, natural offspring stimuli cause maternal brain activation of the NAc-VP circuit (via dopamine and oxytocin) to facilitate selective offspring recognition, behavioral reactivity, and lasting social attraction. This can occur when NAc-GABAergic efferents to the VP are suppressed (via cortical dopamine-induced disinhibition), releasing the VP from NAc inhibitory control and enhancing VP response to pup stimuli (Hansen et al., 1993; Numan and Insel, 2003; Champagne et al., 2004; Numan, 2007; Ikemoto, 2010). The MBN is modulated by the opioid-sensitive extended amygdala, including the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Klampfl and Bosch, 2019), insula, and orbitofrontal cortex (Gholampour et al., 2020) with connections to motor cortical regions for maternal behavioral output (Figure 1), such as the supplementary motor area (SMA) (Zhang et al., 2012). The SMA is activated by infant crying sounds, for which picking up, holding, and talking to their infants are behaviors common to mothers across multiple cultures (Bornstein et al., 2017). Thus far, however, there is little research on mothers with buprenorphine treatment for OUD (Salihu et al., 2019), and just a few studies recently reviewed on the underlying MBN among mothers with OUD (Swain and Ho, 2021).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The maternal behavior neurocircuit (MBN) is comprised of two reciprocally inhibiting subsystems for: (1) maternal care, mediated by the medial preoptic area (mPOA) of hypothalamus (HYP), ventral tegmental area (VTA), nucleus accumbens (NAc), and ventral pallidum (VP), which is functionally connected to the supplementary motor area (SMA), and (2) maternal defense, mediated by periaqueductal gray (PAG). These opposing subsystems are regulated by the amygdala (AMY), insula, and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC).

In our first report, BT mothers with OUD compared with a control group (CG) showed greater HYP and PAG responses to own vs. other baby-cry and differential functional connectivity between the HYP and PAG associated with parenting stress, suggesting that BT may dysregulate the normal balance between maternal caregiving and defensive/aggressive circuits (Swain et al., 2019). In another study of the same cohort, BT vs. CG differences in resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) between the PAG and HYP were studied at 1 month (T1) and 4 months postpartum (T2) (Swain and Ho, 2019). The authors found that BT mothers differed from CG mothers in PAG-dependent rs-FC with the HYP, amygdala, insula, and other brain regions that regulate caring at T1, with many of these differences not evident at T2. Furthermore, the authors also found that the PAG-dependent rs-FCs were related to maternal bonding problems as evidenced by the fact that “rejection and pathological anger” subscale of the Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire (PBQ) at T2 was associated with the increases from T1 to T2 in PAG-dependent rs-FC with the HYP and amygdala. This suggests that possible maternal bonding problems for mothers with BT OUD in the early postpartum may be linked to connectivity differences between specific care and defense maternal brain circuits, which may also be modulated by buprenorphine treatment. More work is required to elucidate how the MBN regulates specific parenting behaviors in OUD mothers such as maternal sensitivity that relate to infant outcome.

Parent–child interactions involving sensitively sharing joy and coping with distress are crucial for child development (Swain et al., 2017). Parental intersubjectivity has been identified as a key resilience factor against the adverse effects of parental stress and depressive moods on parent–child relationships (Camoirano, 2017; Bernard et al., 2018). Intersubjectivity is defined here as the understanding of the internal models of self and others, intentions, and feelings underlying overt behaviors. Parental intersubjectivity enables a parent to feel what the inner experience of a child is like, without diminishing the distinction between the inner experiences of the parent and the child. Parental intersubjectivity is embedded in several parenting-related constructs, such as parental sensitivity (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bernard et al., 2013), parental reflective functioning (Fonagy et al., 1991; Slade, 2005), parental empathic attunement (Rowe and MacIsaac, 2004), and parental embodied mentalizing (Shai and Belsky, 2011). These constructs commonly point to the capacity of a parent to rely on dyadic interactions to understand the child and provide sensitive care to foster healthy development.

A key attribute underlying intersubjectivity is face mirroring, i.e., spontaneous mimicry or voluntary imitation of the facial expressions or manual gestures of others. The rudimentary capacity of intersubjectivity is innate (Trevarthen and Aitken, 2001). Indeed, infants can spontaneously mimic facial expressions soon after birth (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977). While mothers with secure parent–child bonding show greater child-oriented face mirroring (Kim et al., 2014), unfortunately, maternal intersubjectivity may be impaired in mothers exposed to excessive parenting stress (Shai et al., 2017), interpersonal violence (Dayton et al., 2016), or depressive mood disorders (Bernard et al., 2018). We have previously demonstrated that a parenting intervention delivered a few years postpartum reduced parenting stress with associated increases in parent–child intersubjective function in the MBN (Ho et al., 2020). Specifically, we found that SMA and other MBN regions were differentially activated during the condition in which the mothers empathically mirrored the facial expressions and emotions of the child. In this study, we contributed data from a pilot project on mothers with BT mothers with OUD using an empathic mirroring fMRI task described below. We hypothesized that the MBN required for mothers to empathically mirror the emotions of their child in infant-oriented sensitive behaviors may be altered for mothers under the stressful conditions of OUD receiving BT.

Materials and Methods

The research reported in this study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States. All research was performed in accordance with relevant IRB guidelines and regulations.

Participants

All participants (N = 22) were recruited from University of Michigan Health System. There were six participants in the buprenorphine replacement treatment group (BT) and 16 participants in the comparison group (CG) who underwent the fMRI task within 1 year postpartum. The participants in BT and CG groups were not different in age [BT: M = 30.67, s.e. = 2.68; CG: M = 29.63, s.e. = 1.64, F(1,20) = 0.11, MSerror = 43.154, p = 0.74], the age of their youngest child [BT: M = 0.25, s.e. = 0.06; CG: M = 0.20, s.e. = 0.04, F(1,20) = 2.47, MSerror = 0.024, p = 0.13], and the number of offspring [BT: M = 1.83, s.e. = 0.24; CG: M = 1.50, s.e. = 0.15, F(1,20) = 1.42, MSerror = 0.34, p = 0.25] (refer to Table 1 for other demographics). The BT OUD mothers were monitored with urine screens and interview as part of clinical care during pregnancy, such that the only exogenous opioid was prescribed buprenorphine. As recorded every 2 weeks postpartum and during our study, it was 12.67 ± 1.63 mg (mean ± SD) daily with all mothers stabilized between 12 and 16 mg daily. We have reported fMRI studies using different tasks completed in the same cohort including a baby-cry task (Swain et al., 2019) and resting-state task (Swain and Ho, 2019).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics.

Child Face Mirroring Task

In Child Face Mirroring Task (CFMT), as described previously (Ho et al., 2020) and illustrated in Figure 2, the participants were presented repeatedly with the same pictures of their own child and of an unknown child in three task conditions, namely, Observe, React, and Join. By design, the Observe task should elicit the unresponsive observation of face-like visual objects of participants; the React task should elicit the usual, voluntary responses of participants to the presented child, and the Join task should elicit the empathic mirroring of participants of the presented child. The React condition was designed for a pre- and post-treatment study (Ho et al., 2020) and thus was not included in the analysis of this study. The task instructions, design, and stimuli have been described elsewhere (Ho et al., 2020).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The design of child face mirroring task [adapted from Ho et al. (2020)]. Note that the task order in this figure did not represent the actual order.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging Procedures

The magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) procedures, image acquisition, and data preprocessing have been described elsewhere (Ho et al., 2020). No head movements.

First-Level Analysis

Following image preprocessing described elsewhere (Ho et al., 2020), we constructed a first-level fixed effect general linear model (GLM) to examine condition-dependent responses. The first-level model consisted of a matrix of regressors modeling six trial types (3 Tasks × 2 Child Identities: Observe Own, React Own, and Join Own and Observe Other’s, React Other’s, and Join Other’s Child), in addition to a regressor for Cue periods (seven regressors total). In this study, we focused on the contrast of the Join vs. Observe contrast of Own Child. Handedness and possible functional lateralization of brain function in the participants were not considered in this study.

Second-Level Analysis

Due to the small sample size, we focused on one contrast of interest, i.e., Join vs. Observe of the Face of Own Child, pooling across facial expressions in this study. This contrast of interest from the first level GLMs was submitted to a second-level random effect GLM, testing BT vs. CG effects on several regions of interest (ROIs), with Bonferroni family-wise small volume corrections (s.v.c.) in each ROI. The ROIs were identified as the subcortical regions known to modulate maternal behaviors (Numan and Woodside, 2010; Swain and Ho, 2017, 2019), with their masks derived from the wfu_pickatlas toolbox (Maldjian et al., 2003), including amygdala [as defined in wfu_pickatlas’ AAL domain (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002)], periaqueductal gray (PAG) (an 8 mm × 6 mm × 8 mm box centered at [0, −28, −12] in MNI coordinates), hypothalamus (as defined in wfu_pickatlas’ TD Brodmann areas+ domain; Maldjian et al., 2003), midbrain (as defined in wfu_pickatlas’ TD Lobes domain; Maldjian et al., 2003), nucleus accumbens (NAc) (a 18 mm × 8 mm × 10 mm box centered at [0, 10, −14] in MNI coordinates), and pallidum (as defined in AAL; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). In addition, as from an independent sample of late postpartum mothers (N = 45), reported previously (Ho et al., 2020), we found that SMA (MNI coordinates: [0, 6, 58], 76 voxels, Z = 5.28, p < 0.001 whole-brain corrected) was the only cluster surviving the whole brain family-wise Bonferroni correction in the main effect of the contrast of interest (Join vs. Observe Own Child). We, therefore, selected SMA as another ROI in this study and created a mask defined by this cluster (682 voxels) as found in that independent sample at a threshold of p = 0.005, uncorrected.

Results

Main Effect of Join Versus Observe of Own Child

We focused on the contrast of Join vs. Observe of Own Child in this study. As hypothesized, we found that, pooling across BT and CG groups, the SMA showed significant Join > Observe differential neural responses in this contrast (MNI coordinates: [0, 2, 58], 65 voxels, Z = 3.53, p = 0.014 Bonferroni family-wise s.v.c., Figure 3). There were no other ROIs that showed significant differential neural responses in this contrast.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The SMA showed Join > Observe of Own Child differential neural response, pooling across groups. The statistical map is presented with an activation threshold of p = 0.005, uncorrected.

Buprenorphine Treatment Versus Comparison Group Contrast

We examined the group differences in the contrast of Join vs. Observe of Own Child. We found that BT showed lesser differential neural responses than CG in the SMA ([2, 8, 60], 222 voxels, Z = 3.13, p = 0.045 Bonferroni family-wise s.v.c., Figure 4A) and the left pallidum ([−16, −4, −6], 23 voxels, Z = 4.26, p = 0.001 Bonferroni family-wise s.v.c., Figure 4B). As depicted in the bar chart (Figure 5), in the SMA and left pallidum both, the CG group showed significant Join > Observe differential responses, but the BT group showed Join < Observe differential response. These results suggested that BT mothers may have altered emotional mirroring responses in brain regions important for parenting behaviors.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) The SMA (the red area indicates the overlap with the Figure 2) and (B) the left pallidum showed CG > BT group difference in Join > Observe of Own Child differential neural response. The statistical map is presented with an activation threshold of p = 0.005, uncorrected.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The bar charts for the Join vs. Observe of Own Child differential responses of BT and CG groups (mean ± s.e.) in the SMA and left pallidum. *indicates that significant group difference in the fMRI analysis.

Discussion

In the midst of an unprecedented opioid overdose crisis (CDC/NCHS, 2021), many peripartum women with OUD are successfully treated with opioid replacement treatment that reduces withdrawal yet poses potential concerns for the psychology of parenting (Salihu et al., 2019). Animal model research has raised substantial concerns that opioids may disrupt maternal behavior by acting on opioid-sensitive maternal brain circuits including the hypothalamus and VP (Bridges and Grimm, 1982; Grimm and Bridges, 1983; Slamberova et al., 2001). Although allied research in humans has suggested that opioids like buprenorphine might reduce separation distress and offer treatment for some forms of depression (Panksepp and Yovell, 2014; Yovell et al., 2016), there have also been concerns that opioids may usurp healthy parent–infant separation distress and reward circuits that may be critical to mother–infant bonding (Swain et al., 2005). Indeed, “high opioid tone” has been recently discussed as a concern in the development of autism spectrum disorder (Anugu et al., 2021), which is arguably one of several developmental disorders showing impaired intersubjective function (Trevarthen and Aitken, 2001). Thus, although BT is highly effective for reducing withdrawal, intersubjectivity-dependent face mirroring may be adversely influenced. By examining the multifaceted psychosocial effects of BT in the early postpartum on the maternal brain, this article begins to address the potential risks and benefits of buprenorphine beyond the basic relief of withdrawal in OUD.

An emerging human neuroimaging literature supports specific mechanisms at work in mothers with BT/OUD for maternal response to the baby cry and functional connectivity in care and defense brain systems (Swain and Ho, 2021). In this pilot study, we tested the MBN for BT vs. CG group differences in child-oriented face mirroring, a foundational aspect of parental intersubjectivity, that may have long-term consequences for infant development (Feldman, 2012), using the contrast of Join vs. Observe of Own Child in CFMT. Pooling across both groups, the SMA showed significant differential activation in this contrast, replicating the results from an independent sample of healthy mothers scanned at a later postpartum timepoint with the same task (Ho et al., 2020). We also found preliminary effects of BT/OUD on the differential neural responses during child-oriented mirroring, i.e., BT/OUD mothers, as compared to CG, showed altered differential neural responses in the SMA and left VP, an opioid sensitive part of the MBN. While the CG showed Join > Observe differential responses, the BT/OUD group showed little differential response in the SMA and in the opposite direction for Observe > Join differential responses in the VP.

The results suggested that, as related to the comparison group, BT/OUD mothers showed impairments in the own-child-oriented face mirroring responses in brain areas that are critical to maternal intersubjectivity including the VP and SMA. The VP is an important part of the MBN in the regulation of maternal caregiving (Swain et al., 2019) and reward processing in addictions (Kalivas and Volkow, 2005; Root et al., 2015). In animal models, the VP has been demonstrated to be a target of maternal brain motivational output of the NAc (Numan and Woodside, 2010; Numan, 2014) and involved in primate models of cued reaching (Jaeger et al., 1995) and other motivated movements (Hegeman et al., 2016). Among human mothers, the VP has been activated in fMRI studies of mothers observing salient own vs. other baby stimuli (Swain, 2011) and increased for mothers viewing the feeding behavior of their own vs. other children at 2–3 years of age (Noriuchi et al., 2019). Furthermore, VP and SMA responses correlated with maternal caregiving behaviors (Hipwell et al., 2015) and responded in a face mirroring task similar to that used in this article (Ho et al., 2020) for non-OUD mothers. Across continents and cultures, the SMA was highlighted in response to own vs. other baby-cry for infant-oriented preparation for movement and vocalization (Bornstein et al., 2017) and also demonstrated to be important for child-oriented empathy in a parent decision-making fMRI task (Ho et al., 2014). Finally, SMA connectivity to amygdala was reported to be heightened during maternal responses to infant distress according to maternal childhood maltreatment and decreased maternal intrusive behaviors, suggesting the potential for transgenerational adaptations to early life adversity that could include brain responses and infant-oriented behaviors to increase maternal sensitivity (Olsavsky et al., 2021). Perhaps plasticity in the SMA could be a future target for interventions to address maternal health from early childhood maltreatment to OUD such as transcranial magnetic stimulation.

This study on responses to a face mirroring task for mothers with BT OUD is preliminary and with notable limitations. First, an optimal comparison group of OUD mothers without BT is neither feasible nor ethical because of the practical impossibility of recruiting subjects with untreated OUD and the medical imperative to treat the suffering of any subject with OUD, respectively. Thus, our CG mothers were not affected by OUD, related stress, or the possible influence of previous opioid use. Future research may need to adopt approximate controls according to the measures of stress or longitudinal designs in which subjects may be their own controls at different doses and time points. Second, replication with larger sample sizes is needed to confirm these findings and include full characterization of participants with OUD, including the quantity and frequency of all prescription, licit and illicit drug using, cravings, withdrawal, and the gold standard “time-line follow back” interview with calendar prompts and other memory aids to facilitate comprehensive and accurate recall of drug use (Sobell et al., 1988, 1998; Carey, 1997). These data in future studies will allow us to test the assumption of it for a range of critical factors. Indeed, the effects of childhood adverse experiences, sociodemographic factors, and other medical conditions constitute important areas of future research on intersubjective parental function, since we already know that parental stress, poverty, anxiety, and postpartum depression affect the parental brain (Moses-Kolko et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2015; Ho and Swain, 2017; Guo et al., 2018). Currently lacking studies of deficits and resiliencies in addition to possible lateralization in maternal brain function connected with mother–child bonding, parenting behavior and child outcome may contribute insights into the long-term consequences of OUD toward improved prophylaxis and treatment (Moningka et al., 2019).

Conclusion

Although preliminary, this study probes potential buprenorphine effects on intersubjective child face mirroring responses in mothers affected by OUD. These preliminary results strengthen the hypotheses that specific MBN regions that are required for mothers to empathically mirror the emotions of their child in infant-oriented sensitive behaviors may be altered for mothers with the stressful condition of OUD receiving BT. With replication and converging research on parental interventions that affect the same regions and correlate with inexpensive and convenient questionnaires, it may be possible to maximize intervention effects on specific neural targets for mothers to augment maternal intersubjectivity and reduce transgenerational mental health risks. Perhaps future interventions will be tailored according to neural targets as needed just as other treatments in medicine target-specific physiological systems that may be malfunctioning. This report calls for more attention to parental intersubjectivity and the roles of SMA and VP in the MBN as possible underlying brain mechanisms to better assess opioid-sensitive parental brain functions in the context of parent–child bonding and parenting.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Michigan. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JS and SH wrote the manuscript—shared in conceptualization, writing, reviewing, editing grant support, and project administration and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work has been supported by the Research Foundation for the State University of New York (SUNY) and the National Institutes for Health (NIH): National Center for Advanced Translational Sciences (NCATS) via the Michigan Institute for Clinical Health Research UL1TR000433, and National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) R01 DA047336-01.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank our many colleagues participating in this original collection and database management of these study data, including Eric Finegood, Susan Hamilton, Zainab Mahmood, Madalyn Meldrim, and Robert Varney.

References

Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns Of Attachment: A Psychological Study Of The Strange Situation. Oxford: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Anugu, V., Ringhisen, J., and Johnson, B. (2021). Autism case report: cause and treatment of “high opioid tone” autism. Front. Psychol. 12:657952. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.657952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, K., Meade, E. B., and Dozier, M. (2013). Parental synchrony and nurturance as targets in an attachment based intervention: building upon Mary Ainsworth’s insights about mother–infant interaction. Attach. Hum. Dev. 15, 507–523. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.820920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, K., Nissim, G., Vaccaro, S., Harris, J. L., and Lindhiem, O. (2018). Association between maternal depression and maternal sensitivity from birth to 12 months: a meta-analysis. Attach. Hum. Dev. 20, 578–599. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1430839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86, 646–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Rigo, P., Esposito, G., Swain, J. E., Suwalsky, J. T. D., et al. (2017). Neurobiology of culturally common maternal responses to infant cry. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, E9465–E9473. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712022114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridges, R. S., and Grimm, C. T. (1982). Reversal of morphine disruption of maternal behavior by concurrent treatment with the opiate antagonist naloxone. Science 218, 166–168. doi: 10.1126/science.7123227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camoirano, A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: a review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Front. Psychol. 8:14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, K. B. (1997). Reliability and validity of the time-line follow-back interview among psychiatric outpatients: a preliminary report. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 11, 26–33.

Google Scholar

CDC/NCHS (2021). Opioid Overdose Crisis [Online]. Available online at: https://www.drugabuse.gov/drugs-abuse/opioids/opioid-overdose-crisis#one (accessed March 9, 2021).

Google Scholar

Champagne, F. A., Chretien, P., Stevenson, C. W., Zhang, T. Y., Gratton, A., and Meaney, M. J. (2004). Variations in nucleus accumbens dopamine associated with individual differences in maternal behavior in the rat. J. Neurosci. 24, 4113–4123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5322-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, K. P., Compton, W., Stinson, F. S., and Grant, B. F. (2006). Lifetime comorbidity of DSM-IV mood and anxiety disorders and specific drug use disorders: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. J. Clin. Psychiatry 67, 247–257. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayton, C. J., Huth-Bocks, A. C., and Busuito, A. (2016). The influence of interpersonal aggression on maternal perceptions of infant emotions: associations with early parenting quality. Emotion 16, 436–448. doi: 10.1037/emo0000114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm. Behav. 61, 380–391. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Steele, H., and Steele, M. (1991). Maternal representations of attachment during pregnancy predict the organization of infant-mother attachment at one year of age. Child Dev. 62, 891–905. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01578.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gholampour, F., Riem, M. M. E., and van den Heuvel, M. I. (2020). Maternal brain in the process of maternal-infant bonding: review of the literature. Soc.. Neurosci. 15, 380–384. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2020.1764093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimm, C. T., and Bridges, R. S. (1983). Opiate regulation of maternal behavior in the rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 19, 609–616. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(83)90336-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, C., Moses-Kolko, E., Phillips, M., Swain, J. E., and Hipwell, A. E. (2018). Severity of anxiety moderates the association between neural circuits and maternal behaviors in the postpartum period. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 426–436. doi: 10.3758/s13415-017-0516-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haight, S., Ko, J., Tong, V., Bohm, M., and Callaghan, W. (2018). Opioid use disorder documented at delivery hospitalization — united states, 1999–2014. MMWR Morb. Mortal Wkly Rep. 67, 845–849. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6731a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, S., Bergvall, A. H., and Nyiredi, S. (1993). Interaction with pups enhances dopamine release in the ventral striatum of maternal rats: a microdialysis study. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 45, 673–676. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(93)90523-v

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hegeman, D. J., Hong, E. S., Hernandez, V. M., and Chan, C. S. (2016). The external globus pallidus: progress and perspectives. Eur. J. Neurosci. 43, 1239–1265. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hipwell, A. E., Guo, C., Phillips, M. L., Swain, J. E., and Moses-Kolko, E. L. (2015). Right frontoinsular cortex and subcortical activity to infant cry is associated with maternal mental state talk. J. Neurosci. 35, 12725–12732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1286-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, S. S., and Swain, J. E. (2017). Depression alters maternal extended amygdala response and functional connectivity during distress signals in attachment relationship. Behav. Brain Res. 325(Pt B), 290–296. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.02.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, S. S., Konrath, S., Brown, S. L., and Swain, J. E. (2014). Empathy and stress related neural responses in maternal decision making. Front. Neurosci. 8:152. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, S. S., Muzik, M., Rosenblum, K. L., Morelen, D., Nakamura, Y., and Swain, J. E. (2020). Potential neural mediators of mom power parenting intervention effects on maternal intersubjectivity and stress resilience. Front. Psychiatry 11:568824. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.568824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikemoto, S. (2010). Brain reward circuitry beyond the mesolimbic dopamine system: a neurobiological theory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 129–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, D., Gilman, S., and Aldridge, J. W. (1995). Neuronal activity in the striatum and pallidum of primates related to the execution of externally cued reaching movements. Brain Res. 694, 111–127. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00780-t

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, H. E., Heil, S. H., Baewert, A., Arria, A. M., Kaltenbach, K., Martin, P. R., et al. (2012). Buprenorphine treatment of opioid-dependent pregnant women: a comprehensive review. Addiction 107 Suppl 1, 5–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.04035.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalivas, P. W., and Volkow, N. D. (2005). The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 1403–1413. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, P., Ho, S. S., Evans, G. W., Liberzon, I., and Swain, J. E. (2015). Childhood social inequalities influences neural processes in young adult caregiving. Dev. Psychobiol. 57, 948–960. doi: 10.1002/dev.21325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, P., Strathearn, L., and Swain, J. E. (2016). The maternal brain and its plasticity in humans. Horm. Behav. 77, 113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., Fonagy, P., Allen, J., Martinez, S., Iyengar, U., and Strathearn, L. (2014). Mothers who are securely attached in pregnancy show more attuned infant mirroring 7 months postpartum. Infant Beha. Dev. 37, 491–504. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klampfl, S. M., and Bosch, O. J. (2019). When mothers neglect their offspring: an activated CRF system in the BNST is detrimental for maternal behavior. Arch. Womens Ment. Health 22, 409–415. doi: 10.1007/s00737-018-0897-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krans, E. E., and Patrick, S. W. (2016). Opioid use disorder in pregnancy: health policy and practice in the midst of an epidemic. Obstet. Gynecol. 128, 4–10. doi: 10.1097/AOG.0000000000001446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krans, E. E., Bogen, D., Richardson, G., Park, S. Y., Dunn, S. L., and Day, N. (2016). Factors associated with buprenorphine versus methadone use in pregnancy. Subst. Abus 37, 550–557. doi: 10.1080/08897077.2016.1146649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., and Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, P. E., Kinsley, C. H., and Bridges, R. S. (1991). Opioid receptor subtype involvement in maternal behavior in lactating rats. Neuroendocrinology 53, 487–492. doi: 10.1159/000125762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198, 75–78. doi: 10.1126/science.198.4312.75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moningka, H., Lichenstein, S., Worhunsky, P. D., DeVito, E. E., Scheinost, D., and Yip, S. W. (2019). Can neuroimaging help combat the opioid epidemic? A systematic review of clinical and pharmacological challenge fMRI studies with recommendations for future research. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 259–273. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0232-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moses-Kolko, E. L., Horner, M. S., Phillips, M. L., Hipwell, A. E., and Swain, J. E. (2014). In search of neural endophenotypes of postpartum psychopathology and disrupted maternal caregiving. J. Neuroendocrinol. 26, 665–684. doi: 10.1111/jne.12183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nanda, S., Brant, R., Regier, M., and Yossuck, P. (2015). Buprenorphine: a new player in neonatal withdrawal syndrome. W. V. Med. J. 111, 16–21.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Noriuchi, M., Kikuchi, Y., Mori, K., and Kamio, Y. (2019). The orbitofrontal cortex modulates parenting stress in the maternal brain. Sci. Rep. 9:1658. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-38402-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Numan, M. (2007). Motivational systems and the neural circuitry of maternal behavior in the rat. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 12–21. doi: 10.1002/dev.20198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Numan, M. (2014). Neurobiology of Social Behavior: Toward an Understanding of the Prosocial and Antisocial Brain. London: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Numan, M., and Insel, T. R. (2003). “Neurochemistry and molecular biology of maternal behavior,” in The neurobiology of parental behavior, eds M. Numan and T. R. Insel (New York, NY: Springer-Verlag), 190–245. doi: 10.1007/0-387-21799-1_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Numan, M., and Woodside, B. (2010). Maternity: neural mechanisms, motivational processes, and physiological adaptations. Behav. Neurosci. 124, 715–741. doi: 10.1037/a0021548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Numan, M., and Young, L. J. (2016). Neural mechanisms of mother-infant bonding and pair bonding: similarities, differences, and broader implications. Horm. Behav. 77, 98–112. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsavsky, A. K., Stoddard, J., Erhart, A., Tribble, R., and Kim, P. (2021). Reported maternal childhood maltreatment experiences, amygdala activation and functional connectivity to infant cry. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 418–427. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Yovell, Y. (2014). Preclinical modeling of primal emotional affects (Seeking, Panic and Play): gateways to the development of new treatments for depression. Psychopathology 47, 383–393. doi: 10.1159/000366208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Root, D. H., Melendez, R. I., Zaborszky, L., and Napier, T. C. (2015). The ventral pallidum: subregion-specific functional anatomy and roles in motivated behaviors. Prog. Neurobiol. 130, 29–70. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R. N., Lofwall, M. R., Kim, S., Chen, M., Beebe, K. L., and Vocci, F. J. (2016). Effect of buprenorphine implants on illicit opioid use among abstinent adults with opioid dependence treated with sublingual buprenorphine: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA 316, 282–290. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.9382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, C. E., and MacIsaac, D. S. (2004). Empathic Attunement: The “Technique” Of Psychoanalytic Self Psychology. Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson.

Google Scholar

Rubin, B. S., and Bridges, R. S. (1984). Disruption of ongoing maternal responsiveness in rats by central administration of morphine sulfate. Brain Res. 307, 91–97. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90464-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, H. J., and Mayes, L. C. (2017). Parenting and addiction: neurobiological insights. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 15, 55–60. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, H. J., Williams, S. K., Moy, S., Mayes, L. C., and Johns, J. M. (2011). Disruption of maternal parenting circuitry by addictive process: rewiring of reward and stress systems. Front. Psychiatry 2:37. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salihu, H. M., Salinas, A., Medina, I., Krishnaswami, J., and Aliyu, M. H. (2019). Biopsychosocial determinants of opioid use disorder (OUD) and implications for maternal and child health research: a scoping review. J. Opioid. Manag. 15, 77–91. doi: 10.5055/jom.2019.0488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shai, D., and Belsky, J. (2011). When words just won’t do: introducing parental embodied mentalizing. Child Dev. Perspect. 5, 173–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00181.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shai, D., Dollberg, D., and Szepsenwol, O. (2017). The importance of parental verbal and embodied mentalizing in shaping parental experiences of stress and coparenting. Infant Behav. Dev. 49, 87–96. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: an introduction. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 269–281. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slamberova, R., Szilagyi, B., and Vathy, I. (2001). Repeated morphine administration during pregnancy attenuates maternal behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 565–576. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00012-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., Leo, G. I., and Cancilla, A. (1988). Reliability of a timeline method: assessing normal drinkers’ reports of recent drinking and a comparative evaluation across several populations. Br. J. Addict. 83, 393–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1988.tb00485.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobell, M. B., Breslin, F. C., and Sobell, L. C. (1998). Project MATCH: the time has come to talk of many things. J. Stud. Alcohol 59, 124–125. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1998.59.124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stafisso-Sandoz, G., Polley, D., Holt, E., Lambert, K. G., and Kinsley, C. H. (1998). Opiate disruption of maternal behavior: morphine reduces, and naloxone restores, c-fos activity in the medial preoptic area of lactating rats. Brain Res. Bull. 45, 307–313. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00375-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E. (2011). The human parental brain: in vivo neuroimaging. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1242–1254. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., and Ho, S. S. (2017). Neuroendocrine mechanisms for parental sensitivity: overview, recent advances and future directions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 15, 105–110. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., and Ho, S. S. (2019). Early postpartum resting-state functional connectivity for mothers receiving buprenorphine treatment for opioid use disorder: a pilot study. J. Neuroendocrinol. 31:e12770. doi: 10.1111/jne.12770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., and Ho, S. S. (2021). Opioids and maternal brain-behavior adaptation. Neuropsychopharmacology 46, 265–266. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00858-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., and Lorberbaum, J. P. (2008). “Imaging the human parental brain,” in Neurobiology of the Parental Brain, ed. R. Bridges (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 83–100. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-374285-8.00006-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., Ho, S. S., Fox, H., Garry, D., and Brummelte, S. (2019). Effects of opioids on the parental brain in health and disease. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 54:100766. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2019.100766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., Ho, S. S., Rosenblum, K. L., Morelen, D., Dayton, C. J., and Muzik, M. (2017). Parent–child intervention decreases stress and increases maternal brain activity and connectivity during own baby-cry: an exploratory study. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 535–553. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., Lorberbaum, J. P., Kose, S., and Strathearn, L. (2007). Brain basis of early parent-infant interactions: psychology, physiology, and in vivo functional neuroimaging studies. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 262–287. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01731.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., Mayes, L. C., and Leckman, J. F. (2005). Endogenous and exogenous opiates modulate the development of parent–infant attachment. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 364–365.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C., and Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 3–48. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallin, C. M., Bowen, S. E., and Brummelte, S. (2021). Opioid use during pregnancy can impair maternal behavior and the maternal brain network: a literature review. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 86:106976. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2021.106976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yovell, Y., Bar, G., Mashiah, M., Baruch, Y., Briskman, I., Asherov, J., et al. (2016). Ultra-low-dose buprenorphine as a time-limited treatment for severe suicidal ideation: a randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 491–498. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15040535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zedler, B. K., Mann, A. L., Kim, M. M., Amick, H. R., Joyce, A. R., Murrelle, E. L., et al. (2016). Buprenorphine compared with methadone to treat pregnant women with opioid use disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of safety in the mother, fetus and child. Addiction 111, 2115–2128. doi: 10.1111/add.13462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S., Ide, J. S., and Li, C. S. (2012). Resting-state functional connectivity of the medial superior frontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 22, 99–111. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: opioid, maternal behavior neurocircuit, face mirroring, intersubjectivity, empathy, supplementary motor area (SMA), magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), ventral pallidum (VP)

Citation: Swain JE and Ho SS (2022) Reduced Child-Oriented Face Mirroring Brain Responses in Mothers With Opioid Use Disorder: An Exploratory Study. Front. Psychol. 12:770093. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.770093

Received: 03 September 2021; Accepted: 29 December 2021;
Published: 04 February 2022.

Edited by:

Leslie Leve, University of Oregon, United States

Reviewed by:

Daniela Flores Mosri, Universidad Intercontinental, Mexico
Daiki Hiraoka, University of Fukui, Japan

Copyright © 2022 Swain and Ho. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: James E. Swain, james.swain@stonybrookmedicine.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.