Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 18 September 2020
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic In Search of Individually Optimal Movement Solutions in Sport: Learning between Stability and Flexibility View all 12 articles

Can Individual Movement Characteristics Across Different Throwing Disciplines Be Identified in High-Performance Decathletes?

\r\nFabian HorstFabian Horst1Daniel JanssenDaniel Janssen2Hendrik BeckmannHendrik Beckmann1Wolfgang I. Schllhorn*Wolfgang I. Schöllhorn1*
  • 1Department of Training and Movement Science, Institute of Sport Science, Johannes Gutenberg-University Mainz, Mainz, Germany
  • 2Gymnasium Dionysianum, Rheine, Germany

Although the individuality of whole-body movements has been suspected for years, the scientific proof and systematic investigation that individuals possess unique movement patterns did not manifest until the introduction of the criteria of uniqueness and persistence from the field of forensic science. Applying the criteria of uniqueness and persistence to the individuality of motor learning processes requires complex strategies due to the problem of persistence in the learning processes. One approach is to examine the learning process of different movements. For this purpose, it is necessary to differentiate between two components of movement patterns: the individual-specific component and the discipline-specific component. To this end, a kinematic analysis of the shot put, discus, and javelin throwing movements of seven high-performance decathletes during a qualification competition was conducted. In total, joint angle waveforms of 57 throws formed the basis for the recognition task of individual- and discipline-specific throwing patterns using a support vector machine. The results reveal that the kinematic throwing patterns of the three disciplines could be distinguished across athletes with a prediction accuracy of up to 100% (57 of 57 throws). In addition, athlete-specific throwing characteristics could also be identified across the three disciplines. Prediction accuracies of up to 52.6% indicated that up to 10 out of 19 throws of a discipline could be assigned to the correct athletes, based on only knowing these athletes from the kinematic throwing patterns in the other two disciplines. The results further suggest that individual throwing characteristics across disciplines are more pronounced in shot put and discus throwing than in javelin throwing. Applications for training and learning practice in sports and therapy are discussed. In summary, the chosen approach offers a broad field of application related to the search of individualized optimal movement solutions in sports.

Introduction

Most of us are familiar with the experience of identifying friends or colleagues by their walk (Cutting and Kozlowski, 1977), even from a distance and with limited visibility (Stevenage et al., 1999). Practitioners in the field of sports science and physical therapy often report the same experience of identifying individual athletes or patients based on their movement characteristics (e.g., a characteristic forehand stroke in tennis or a unique hand movement). Additionally, most of us have observed people mastering certain tasks easily, while struggling to become proficient in others. Both experiences serve as evidence of the individuality of human movements, though they may hold various meanings and act epistemologically on different time scales (Newell et al., 2001). The tacit, universal acceptance of movement as a method of identifying individuals suggests that most of us understand individualized movement, yet the perfunctory nature of this acceptance has inhibited a deeper investigation of the concept’s essential aspects and consequences.

In human movement science, anecdotal evidence has made claims of “individuality” for years (Bernstein, 1967; Marteniuk, 1974). Although the importance of individuality in sports training has been recognized since the origins of sports science (Matveev, 1970), the phenomenon has been mostly regarded as a negligible side effect or as an exception in the search for universal scientific laws (Harre, 1969, 2013; Matveev, 1970; Huber, 1977; Schmidt and Young, 1991; Nitsch et al., 1997; Schnabel et al., 1997). In most cases, individuality appeared in the context of reliability studies that compared intra- and inter-individual variance (Bates et al., 1983). These reliability studies led to the standard requirement that an average of 10 to 25 movement trials be conducted for each individual participant to achieve an appropriate level of reliability or reproducibility (Bates et al., 1983; DeVita and Bates, 1988; Gollhofer et al., 1990). The extent to which the inter-individual variance distributions overlap to discriminate individuals from one another was not investigated.

In the past, the term individuality most often has been normatively applied in three ways: (1) when no classification criteria could be found (Brüggemann et al., 1991; Schöner et al., 1992; Button et al., 2000; Hecksteden et al., 2015; Barth et al., 2019), (2) to explicitly circumvent “the difficulty of achieving statistical significance” by creating smaller standard deviations and by describing several single cases (Davids et al., 1999; Button et al., 2000; Nuzzo, 2014), and (3) when individuality was predetermined in the form of case studies (Mendoza and Schöllhorn, 1990; Schöllhorn, 1993, Schöllhorn, 1998; Wallace et al., 1994; Bauer and Schöllhorn, 1997; Button et al., 2006; Chow et al., 2006). Frequently, all three interpretations were used in combination and reflected a rougher approximation of the phenomenon than scientific evidence would suggest.

While movement and sports science still struggle to balance the demands of group-oriented science and individual athlete- and patient-oriented practice, forensic science remains primarily concerned with individual cases that must lend legal validity. Therefore, the field of forensic science has developed specific methods and criteria for the identification of individuals (disjunct separation) (Kaye, 2010). In this context, Jain et al. (2006) suggested that before individuality can be assumed, one must test the probability of uniqueness (indicating that no two persons have identical characteristics) and the persistence/permanence of a physiological or behavioral characteristic (meaning that the characteristic should be invariant with time).

The first steps toward such criteria took place in the analysis of everyday and sports movements and revealed the identification of individual people based on gait (Schöllhorn et al., 1999, 2002; Nixon et al., 2006), running (Simon and Schöllhorn, 1995), pole vaulting (Jaitner and Schöllhorn, 1995), discus (Bauer and Schöllhorn, 1997), and javelin throwing (Schöllhorn and Bauer, 1998). The proposed approaches used self-organizing Kohonen maps, in combination with cluster analysis, as early representatives for machine learning classification in human movement science. The results indicated the structural application of a statistical approach that is, similar to forensic proceedings, oriented toward a generic understanding of probability. As follows, the probability of an event occurring is equal to the number of ways of achieving success relative to the possible number of outcomes. For example, Schöllhorn and Bauer (1998) recorded 10 javelin throws by a single athlete at different competitions over 5 years. Subsequently, the kinematic patterns of these 10 throws were clustered together out of 51 kinematic patterns of throws from several other athletes. The probability of achieving this classification by chance is extremely low (<1 × 10–17). This outcome is far below the magnitude of common probabilities used, first, in the statistical model based on the work of Fisher and Mackenzie (1923) or Neyman and Pearson (1928) and, second, in the magnitude of becoming legally relevant.

Meanwhile, the uniqueness and persistence of individual movement patterns could be validated for versatile whole-body movements such as walking (Horst et al., 2017b, 2019), pedaling (Hug et al., 2019), basketball throwing (Schmidt, 2012), horse riding (Schöllhorn et al., 2006), or playing a musical instrument (Albrecht et al., 2014). Up to this point, one might still be tempted to argue that a single movement pattern is optimal for an individual athlete. Initial doubts were raised, however, when it was remembered that none of the aforementioned studies could demonstrate identical repetitions of movement patterns, and thus strong indications were provided for the intuitively assumed (Bernstein, 1967) and previously biomechanically derived (Hatze, 1986) extremely low probability of identical repetitions. Theoretically, however, the continuous fluctuations that were observed during the proof of persistence could have been due to limitations in the biomechanical measurement resolutions or could have simply been random, unstructured noise. More detailed investigations of fluctuations between repeated movement executions within individual persons surprisingly revealed strong evidence of fine structures within a class of movement patterns. These finer structures showed a dependence on fluctuations in emotion (Janssen et al., 2008; Janssen, 2017), on fatigue (Jäger et al., 2003; Janssen et al., 2011), or on time (Bauer and Schöllhorn, 1997; Schöllhorn et al., 2002; Rein et al., 2010) with different time scales (Horst et al., 2016, 2017a). The individuality of movement patterns in connection with their fine structures thus indicate that individual movement patterns continuously change and adapt over time.

In practice, the identification of athletes based on their individual movement patterns and their corresponding fine structure does not require individually tailored learning or training methods. If we assume that individual differences exist from birth, then theoretically, the same learning and training content could have led to individually distinguishable movement patterns at a later age. However, this awareness is subject to the assumption that everyone responds in the same way to intervention measures. To shed more light on these questions, further studies on individual learning behavior should be conducted.

Indications for individual responses on a similar intervention came from physiological (Bouchard and Rankinen, 2001) and biomechanical studies (Cole et al., 1995; Schöllhorn et al., 2001, 2002). In the meantime, an increasing number of studies have observed phenomena that indicate the individuality of adaptations and learning (Schöllhorn et al., 2006; Kostrubiec et al., 2012; Caballero et al., 2017). References to the advantages of learning with individual role models also began to question learning approaches based on average-oriented group role models (Brisson and Alain, 1996). Despite these initial signs, scientific evidence of individual learning processes according to the criteria of uniqueness and persistence is still missing (Fisher et al., 2018). Because of the normative nature of these studies, a criteria-driven analysis, as proposed by Jain et al. (2006), is suggested. Applying the same criteria of uniqueness and persistence to motor learning and adaptation processes requires, first, that each athlete/patient (e.g., in terms of changes in movement patterns or performance outcomes) respond differently to a particular intervention (uniqueness) and, second, that individual responses to multiple interventions can be repeatedly demonstrated (persistence).

While the first criterion could be tested indirectly via the degree of learning progress each participant achieves, testing the criterion of persistence is more complicated. The simplest way to prove the persistence of individual learning characteristics would be to allow athletes to learn the same skill several times after wash-out phases. However, a limitation with this approach has been raised by re-learning studies (Newell et al., 2001; Liu et al., 2003). Once a movement is acquired and forgotten, it is re-learned more quickly (Malone et al., 2011). In consequence, initial learning conditions cannot be reproduced exactly when a skill is re-learned several times, even with adequate wash-out phases. This fact makes it almost impossible to compare the persistence of learning processes adequately. Alternatively, individual characteristics of learning processes should be observable in the acquisition of different skills. Therefore, finding approaches that can detach the individual-specific characteristics from the task-specific characteristics in various movements would be helpful. Interestingly, previous studies on the uniqueness and persistence of movement patterns have only been carried out within a single movement task.

This pilot study aims to analyze the three throwing disciplines in the decathlon (shot put, discus throw, and javelin throw) and to search for athlete- and discipline-specific similarities in the kinematic throwing patterns of these disciplines using automatic classification by means of machine learning. Instead of merely testing the individuality of athletes in a single throwing discipline, the present classifications are used to test whether the kinematic throwing patterns will be assigned to the correct throwing discipline and whether the knowledge of, for example, an individual athlete’s shot put movement patterns predicts the individual’s discus or javelin movement patterns. For this objective, high-performance athletes competing in a national decathlon qualification competition were selected. This high performance level served to guarantee sufficient stability for all three throwing movements. A competition was selected for this study because it is a setting in which athletes often demonstrate their best performances, which we assume can increase the expression of the individuality in their movement patterns (Schöllhorn et al., 2002).

Materials and Methods

Seven right-handed, male decathletes (18.9 ± 0.4 years), who were members of the German junior national team with at least 5 years of experience in the decathlon, were recorded during a national decathlon qualifying competition. The final throwing phases of 19 shot puts, 19 discus, and 19 javelin throws were analyzed (Table 1). For right-handed athletes, the final throwing phases all begin when the left foot touches down and end when the throwing object is released from the hand. Most of the increase in velocity of throwing object is produced during this phase (Hay, 1993; Bauersfeld and Schröter, 1998). The throwing performances ranged from 11.70 to 15.06 m (shot put), 33.66 to 43.74 m (discus), and 40.08 to 58.03 m (javelin).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Number of throwing trials per athlete and discipline.

The recordings were taken using two high-frequency video cameras (Weinberger MiniVis Eco-2; frequency: 200 fps; resolution: 1280 × 1024 pixel), which were positioned orthogonal to each other, one facing toward the flight direction of the throwing object. A space of 3 × 3 × 3 m3 was covered for the analysis using a calibration cube with 25 marker points. Due to the official competition rules, no marker points were allowed on the athletes. Data of both cameras were synchronized using an electric impulse transmitted from the master camera to the slave camera during each throw. Neither the athletes nor the experienced digitizers were informed about the aim of the study. The following anatomical body landmarks were digitized: the manubrium sterni, the left and right acromion, the epicondylus lateralis, the processus steyloidus ulnae, the spina illiaca anterior superior, the trochanter major, the lateral end of the femur (knee), the patella, the articulatio tibo fibulare talare, the calcaneus, the phalanx distalis, and the hallux.

The digitization of these points allowed the estimation of three-dimensional joint angles of the right and left shoulder, elbow, hand, hip, knee, and ankle. All videos were manually digitized with SIMI Motion Software 5.0 (SIMI Reality Motion Systems, Germany). Data were filtered using a recursive, second-order Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 14 Hz. All trials were digitized for an additional 10 video frames at both phase boundary ends because of filter effects at the beginning and end of each signal.

For javelin, the duration of the final throwing phase lasts about 130 ms; for discus throwing, about 400 ms; and for shot put, about 200 ms (Ballreich and Kuhlow, 1986; Ballreich et al., 1989). Despite the variable duration of these final throwing phases, commonalities between all three throwing disciplines are assumed and used to economize training in combined events (Hay, 1993). The trials were time normalized to 26 intervals to compare the kinematic patterns of three disciplines. After time normalization, the amplitudes were normalized over all trials and all athletes into the interval [0;1].

Time- and amplitude-normalized data formed the input vectors for the classifications using a support vector machine (SVM) (Cortes and Vapnik, 1995). The classification of the kinematic throwing patterns based on SVM represents a supervised learning approach for pattern recognition in data sets. The ability to distinguish kinematic throwing patterns was investigated in multi-class classifications using a “one-vs.-all” algorithm. The L2-regularized, L2-loss, support-vector classification of the Liblinear Toolbox 1.4.1 (Fan et al., 2008) was applied with a linear kernel function within the software environment Scilab 6.0.2 (Scilab Enterprises, France). A grid search within the range of C = 2–5, 2–4.75, …, 215 was conducted to determine C experimentally before the training and testing of the SVM models. An athlete-classification using a leave-discipline-out cross-validation and a discipline-classification using a leave-athlete-out cross-validation were performed. This processing means that data from one discipline (in the case of the athlete-classification) or from one athlete (in the case of the discipline-classification) were used either as training or as test data during the cross-validation of the SVM models. A schematic overview of the entire approach with data acquisition, processing, and classification is depicted in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic overview of our approach to data acquisition, processing, and classification. (A) Seven right-handed athletes (A1–A7), who were members of the German junior national team with at least 5 years of experience in the decathlon, were recorded during a national decathlon qualifying competition. The kinematic analysis included all valid trials of the competition in the three throwing disciplines: S, shot put; D, discus throw; J, javelin throw. The final throwing phases of 19 shot puts, 19 discus, and 19 javelin throws were analyzed using two orthogonally positioned high-frequency video cameras. After the digitization of 23 anatomical body landmarks, the three-dimensional joint angles of the right and left shoulder, elbow, hand, hip, knee, and ankle were estimated. (B) Time- and amplitude-normalized joint angle waveforms formed the input vectors for the classifications using a support vector machine (SVM). An athlete-classification using a leave-discipline-out cross-validation and a discipline-classification using a leave-athlete-out cross-validation were performed. (C) The performance of the classification models was assessed based on the prediction accuracy.

In the case of athlete-classification, the kinematic data of one discipline were used in the cross-validation, either as training or as test data (cf., athlete-classification in Figure 1). This use of data means that the classification model did not “see” the throwing patterns of the athletes in the tested discipline during the training process. In the first cross-validation split, the classification model was first trained to distinguish the athletes based on the normalized kinematic patterns of all shot puts and javelin throws. Then, the performance of the classification model was tested using the normalized kinematic patterns of all discus throws.

In the case of the discipline-classification, the SVMs were trained with the corresponding partitions of variable waveforms of all athletes (except one) in all three throwing disciplines. The remaining waveforms of the one athlete were used to test the performance of the SVM models for classification into one of the three possible throwing disciplines.

Results

The results of the athlete-classification are shown in Table 2. When the classification models were tested with data from the discus throws, the results showed the highest prediction accuracy of 52.6% for athletes when the SVM considered all kinematic variables except the variables of the throwing arm. The lowest predictive accuracy of 21.1% was obtained when only the kinematic data of the lower-body joint angles were considered.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Prediction accuracy of the athlete-classification with leave-discipline-out cross-validation for different data partitions.

Similar results were found when the performance of SVM models was determined using the kinematic patterns of shot puts as test data. While the prediction accuracy is slightly lower when all variables are considered, the lowest prediction accuracy of 31.6% is larger than for the discus split and is achieved when all variables and only the lower-body variables are used as test data.

The lowest prediction accuracies are generally found when the SVM models are tested based on javelin throwing patterns (15.8–31.6%). When the SVM models for athlete-classification are trained on discus and shot put data, it seems to be more difficult to assign the movement patterns of javelin throwing to the individuals. Similar results can be observed in the pairwise cross-validations, which are also shown in Table 2.

In Table 3, the results of the discipline-classification are listed with the same variable partitions as in Table 2. When all variables were included in the discipline-classification, the respective disciplines could be predicted with an accuracy of 100%, based on the kinematic throwing patterns.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Prediction accuracy of the discipline-classification with leave-athlete-out cross-validation for different data partitions.

Discussion

The results of this study reveal that the kinematic patterns of the three throwing disciplines in the decathlon (shot put, discus throw, and javelin throw) could be distinguished independently of the athlete with a prediction accuracy of up to 100% (57 of 57 throws) using an automatic classification using machine learning (i.e., SVMs). In addition, prediction accuracies of up to 52.6% (10 of 19 throws) also indicate the persistence of individual throwing characteristics of athletes across different throwing disciplines. The results further suggest that individual throwing characteristics across disciplines are more pronounced in shot put and discus throwing than in javelin throwing. This finding demonstrates that the approach of classifying movement patterns using machine learning methods allows for the identification of athlete- and discipline-specific similarities in throwing patterns across different disciplines in high-performance athletes and suggests new ways to explore sports training in different disciplines.

In the following sections, we discuss the results in more detail. In the athlete-classification, the highest prediction accuracies by SVM models based on all variables except the throwing arm variables mean that every second shot put kinematic pattern was correctly assigned to the corresponding individual athlete when the SVM model was trained on the kinematic throwing patterns of all athletes in javelin and discus throw. Prediction accuracies over ∼50%, which are well above the random baseline of 14.3%, provide a strong indication that individual movement signatures can be detected in different movements (e.g., different throwing disciplines). The present findings reinforce previous studies that showed the uniqueness and persistence of individual movement patterns within various movements and support the call for a stronger focus on individual athletes or patients in sports and movement science (e.g., Horst et al., 2017b).

Note that the highest prediction accuracy is achieved using SVM models that consider all joint angle waveforms except the angles of the throwing arm. Comparatively lower prediction accuracies using SVM models that take into account all joint angle waveforms (including the ones of the throwing arm) might be traced to a slightly reduced individuality and a predominant expression of the discipline specificity in the throwing-arm, joint-angle waveforms. However, further research is needed to determine whether this lower prediction accuracy is due to the specificity of the disciplines or due to the variability in throwing arm movements. In this regard, a joint angle-wise classification and determination of movement variability could provide further clarification.

Higher prediction accuracies for SVM models based on the joint angles waveforms of the upper body without the throwing arm in shot put and discus throwing provide evidence for increased individuality of the movement of the left arm, trunk, and head in comparison to the waveforms of the lower-body joint angles, which are more restricted by their contact to the ground. Whether lower prediction accuracies of the SVM models based on lower-body joint angles are only due to the comparably coarse biomechanical data acquisition without anatomical markers or due to the small geometric differences in the leg movements cannot be resolved satisfactorily here.

Considerably lower prediction accuracies of SVM models for athlete-classification that were trained with the kinematic patterns of shot put and discus throw and tested with javelin throws are in line with findings of national (Kunz, 1980) and international (Pavlović and Idrizović, 2017) decathletes, who showed a high correlation between performances in shot put and discus throwing, but no linear correlation with performance in javelin throwing. The finding that the individual throwing characteristics across the disciplines are more pronounced in shot put and discus throwing than in javelin throwing gives rise to the speculation about a more individual coupling of the joint angles of the trunk and lower body with the left arm and head in shot put and discus throwing. Future research is necessary to investigate whether cross-disciplinary individual characteristics in shot put and discus throwing also foster a positive transfer from training in one discipline to the other. An analysis of individual muscle activation signatures (Hug et al., 2019) during shot putting, discus, and javelin throwing could provide interesting insights in this context.

In discipline classification, a prediction accuracy of 100% for most cross-validation splits and combinations of considered variables implies an automatic and differentiated recognition of shot put, discus, and javelin throwing movement patterns. The results provide promising evidence for the ability of pattern recognition approaches using machine learning methods to distinguish between different qualities of whole-body movements (Schöllhorn and Bauer, 1997; Schorer et al., 2007).

Finally, some specificities of this pilot study should be kept in mind. The chosen pattern recognition approach based on probabilities relative to the number of choices is distinguished from null-hypothesis-testing approaches. No claims for generalization are made. In addition, the demand for a relatively high level of performance in different sports disciplines limited the possibilities for empirical data collection enormously. Some limitations arise from selecting decathletes on their way from juvenile to adult competition classes as the object for this pilot study. The athletes’ age suggests that some may not have completed puberty, and ongoing physical growth could have an additional influence on the consistency of their movement patterns. To what extent incomplete physical growth influences throwing patterns and throwing consistency requires further research.

Conclusion

The results offer evidence for the possibility of automatic recognition of kinematic movement patterns originating from different sports disciplines and confirm the assumption of a strong and cross-disciplinary importance of individuality in at least two of the throwing disciplines investigated. That certain individual movement characteristics can be identified in the kinematic patterns of both shot put and discus throwing is intriguing. This finding must be distinguished from the recognition of an individual athlete within a single discipline, as shown for discus (Bauer and Schöllhorn, 1997) and javelin throwing (Schöllhorn and Bauer, 1998). An extension of this approach to the kinematic movement patterns in other sports disciplines such as the tennis serve, handball throw, or volleyball smash is reasonable. Exploring the respective proportion of individual characteristics in movement patterns in more detail, even for dissimilar movement classes, will be a challenge for future research. This exploration can be compared with the search for analogies between different biometric characteristics.

A further criterion for individuality, which could be summarized by homomorphism, could be added to the necessary criteria of uniqueness and persistence. Different from static biometric measures such as fingerprints, facial characteristics, or ear shapes, which are frequently directly related to static genetics, movement-based biometry is subject to dynamic changes and uncertain associations to the genome. While it is difficult to find a common underlying basis for the biometrics of finger, face, or ear apart from genetics, the comprehension of individual commonalities in different movements (e.g., throwing disciplines) could provide access to the underlying individuality of central nervous physiology and structure. Future applications of this approach could investigate the extent to which the central nervous system or the muscle physiology are modifiable beyond an individual’s range.

Against this backdrop, the probability of finding a single (time-independent) optimal movement pattern for an individual athlete is more than challenging. Instead, rethinking the understanding of an optimal movement pattern is promising. An extension of the term “optimal” by situation-optimal, as the currently optimal solution for an individual athlete, may be initially tempting. However, an optimal solution would only serve as a theoretical model and could never be realistically achieved. Because the motor system of an individual is constantly changing and adapting, the model of a situation-optimal movement pattern would also have to constantly change and adapt. Alternatively, the assumption of a situation-optimal model that is constantly changing could be more advantageous for motor learning than for the pursuit of an insurmountable goal.

The study showed that an applied pattern recognition approach based on a machine learning classification provides an alternative and holistic approach for the analysis of biomechanical movement data. This approach is closely connected to a statistical method based on the original concept of probabilities and may help to circumvent some of the limitations connected with the Fisher and Mackenzie (1923) and Neyman and Pearson (1928) statistics.

Taken together, the findings of human movement science regarding the uniqueness and persistence of individual movement patterns based on machine learning methods and the insights into the influencing factors indicated in this study suggested that we are still at the beginning of understanding the individuality of moving and learning human beings.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the article, critically revised the manuscript, and approved the final version. WIS designed the experiment. DJ, HB, and WIS conducted the acquisition and processing of the data. FH, DJ, and HB analyzed the data. FH, DJ, and WIS designed the figure. FH and WIS interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

The study was funded by the German Federal Institute of Sport Science (BISp; Grant No. 070613/08).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Albrecht, S., Janssen, D., Quarz, E., Newell, K. M., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (2014). Individuality of movements in music - finger and body movements during playing of the flute. Hum. Mov. Sci. 35, 131–144. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballreich, R., and Kuhlow, A. (1986). “Biomechanik des Kugelstoßes,” in Biomechanik der Leichtathletik, eds R. Ballreich and A. Kuhlow (Stuttgart: Enke), 89–109.

Google Scholar

Ballreich, R., Schöllhorn, W., and Menzel, H.-J. (1989). “Stoß- und wurfdisziplinen,” in Biomechanik der Sportarten, ed. K. Willimczik (Reinbek: Rowohlt), 197–230.

Google Scholar

Barth, V., Käsbauer, H., Ferrauti, A., Kellmann, M., Pfeiffer, M., Hecksteden, A., et al. (2019). Individualized monitoring of muscle recovery in elite badminton. Front. Physiol. 10:778. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, B. T., Osternig, L. R., Sawhill, J. A., and James, S. L. (1983). An assessment of subject variability, subject-shoe interaction, and the evaluation of running shoes using ground reaction force data. J. Biomech. 16, 181–191. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(83)90125-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, H. U., and Schöllhorn, W. (1997). Self-organizing maps for the analysis of complex movement patterns. Neural Process. Lett. 5, 193–199. doi: 10.1023/A:1009646811510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauersfeld, K. H., and Schröter, G. (1998). Grundlagen der Leichtathletik. Berlin: Sportverlag.

Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The Co-ordination and Regulation of Movements. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.

Google Scholar

Bouchard, C., and Rankinen, T. (2001). Individual differences in response to regular physical activity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 33, S446–S451.

Google Scholar

Brisson, T. A., and Alain, C. (1996). Should common optimal movement patterns be identified as the criterion to be achieved? J. Mot. Behav. 28, 211–223. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1996.9941746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brüggemann, G. P., Gattermann, E., Göhner, U., Jander, H., Lampe, L., and Mester, J. (1991). Individualisierung der Skitechnik – Grundlage für gesundes, sicheres und freudvolles Skilaufen. Schriftreihe Dtsch. Skiverbandes 21, 25–48.

Google Scholar

Button, C., Davids, K., Bennett, S. J., and Taylor, M. A. (2000). Mechanical perturbation of the wrist during one-handed catching. Acta Psychol. 105, 9–30. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(00)00044-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Button, C., Davids, K., and Schöllhorn, W. (2006). “Coordination profiling of movement systems,” in Movement System Variability, eds K. Davids, S. Bennett, and K. Newell, (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics), 133–152.

Google Scholar

Caballero, C., Moreno, F. J., Reina, R., Roldán, A., Coves, Á., and Barbado, D. (2017). The role of motor variability in motor control and learning depends on the nature of the task and the individual’s capabilities. Eur. J. Hum. Mov. 38, 12–26.

Google Scholar

Chow, J. Y., Davids, K., Button, C., and Koh, M. (2006). Organization of motor system degrees of freedom during the soccer chip: an analysis of skilled performance. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 37, 207–229.

Google Scholar

Cole, G. K., Nigg, B. M., Fick, G. H., and Morlock, M. M. (1995). Internal loading of the foot and ankle during impact in running. J. Appl. Biomech. 11, 25–46. doi: 10.1123/jab.11.1.25

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortes, C., and Vapnik, V. (1995). Support-vector networks. Mach. Learn. 20, 273–297. doi: 10.1007/BF00994018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutting, J. E., and Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: gait perception without familiarity cues. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 9, 353–356. doi: 10.3758/BF03337021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davids, K., Button, C., and Bennett, S. (1999). Modeling human motor systems in nonlinear dynamics: intentionality and discrete movement behaviors. Nonlinear Dynamics Psychol. Life Sci. 3, 3–30. doi: 10.1023/A:1022429522099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVita, P., and Bates, B. T. (1988). Intraday reliability of ground reaction force data. Hum. Mov. Sci. 7, 73–85. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(88)90005-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, R. E., Chang, K.-W., Hsieh, C.-J., Wang, X.-R., and Lin, C.-J. (2008). Liblinear: a library for large linear classification. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 9, 1871–1874.

Google Scholar

Fisher, A. J., Medaglia, J. D., and Jeronimus, B. F. (2018). Lack of group-to-individual generalizability is a threat to human subjects research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, E6106–E6115. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1711978115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, R. A., and Mackenzie, W. A. (1923). Studies in crop variation: the manurial response of different potato varieties. J. Agric. Sci. 13, 311–320. doi: 10.1017/s0021859600003592

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollhofer, A., Horstmann, G. A., Schmidtbleicher, D., and Schönthal, D. (1990). Reproducibility of electromyographic patterns in stretch-shortening type contractions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 60, 7–14. doi: 10.1007/BF00572179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harre, D. (1969). Trainingslehre. Berlin: Sportverlag.

Google Scholar

Harre, D. (2013). Principles of Sports Training. Muskegon, MI: Ultimate Athlete Concepts.

Google Scholar

Hatze, H. (1986). Motion variability–its definition, quantification, and origin. J. Mot. Behav. 18, 5–16. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1986.10735368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hay, J. G. (1993). The Biomechanics of Sports Techniques. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Hecksteden, A., Kraushaar, J., Scharhag-Rosenberger, F., Theisen, D., Senn, S., and Meyer, T. (2015). Individual response to exercise training - a statistical perspective. J. Appl. Physiol. 118, 1450–1459. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00714.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horst, F., Eekhoff, A., Newell, K. M., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (2017a). Intra-individual gait patterns across different time-scales as revealed by means of a supervised learning model using kernel-based discriminant regression. PLoS One 12:e0179738. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0179738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horst, F., Kramer, F., Schäfer, B., Eekhoff, A., Hegen, P., Nigg, B. M., et al. (2016). Daily changes of individual gait patterns identified by means of support vector machines. Gait Posture 49, 309–314. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.07.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horst, F., Lapuschkin, S., Samek, W., Müller, K.-R., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (2019). Explaining the unique nature of individual gait patterns with deep learning. Sci. Rep. 9:2391. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-38748-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horst, F., Mildner, M., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (2017b). One-year persistence of individual gait patterns identified in a follow-up study - A call for individualised diagnose and therapy. Gait Posture 58, 476–480. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, H. P. (1977). Single case analysis. Behav. Anal. Modifications 2, 1–15.

Google Scholar

Hug, F., Vogel, C., Tucker, K., Dorel, S., Deschamps, T., Le Carpentier, É., et al. (2019). Individuals have unique muscle activation signatures as revealed during gait and pedaling. J. Appl. Physiol. 127, 1165–1174. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01101.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jäger, J. M., Alichmann, M., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (2003). “Erkennung von Ermüdungszuständen anhand von Bodenreaktionskräften mittels Neuronaler Netze,” in Biologische Systeme – Mechanische Eigenschaften und Ihre Adaptation bei Körperlicher Belastung, eds G. P. Brüggemann and G. Morey-Klapsing (Hamburg: Czwalina), 179–183.

Google Scholar

Jain, A. K., Bolle, R., and Pankanti, S. (2006). “Introduction to biometrics,” in Biometrics: Personal Identification in Networked Society, eds A. K. Jain, R. Bolle, and S. Pankanti, (New York, NY: Springer), 1–41. doi: 10.1002/9780470997949.ch1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaitner, T., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (1995). “Prozessorientierte Bewegungsanalyse am Beispiel des Stabhochsprungs,” in Sport im Lebenslauf, eds D. Schmidtbleicher, K. Bös, and A. Müller (Hamburg: Czwalina), 293–298.

Google Scholar

Janssen, D. (2017). Analyse und Klassifikation der Effekte Unterschiedlicher Emotionsinduktionsmethoden auf das Menschliche Gangmuster Sowie den Übergang Zwischen Gehen und Laufen. Hamburg: Kovac.

Google Scholar

Janssen, D., Schöllhorn, W. I., Lubienetzki, J., Fölling, K., Kokenge, H., and Davids, K. (2008). Recognition of emotions in gait patterns by means of artificial neural nets. J. Nonverbal Behav. 32, 79–92. doi: 10.1007/s10919-007-0045-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, D., Schöllhorn, W. I., Newell, K. M., Jäger, J. M., Rost, F., and Vehof, K. (2011). Diagnosing fatigue in gait patterns by support vector machines and self-organizing maps. Hum. Mov. Sci. 30, 966–975. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2010.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaye, D. H. (2010). Probability, individualization, and uniqueness in forensic science evidence. Brooklyn Law. Rev. 75, 1163–1185.

Google Scholar

Kostrubiec, V., Zanone, P.-G., Fuchs, A., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2012). Beyond the blank slate: routes to learning new coordination patterns depend on the intrinsic dynamics of the learner-experimental evidence and theoretical model. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:222. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunz, H. (1980). Leistungsbesimmende Faktoren im Zehnkampf. Eine Laengsschnittstudie an Schweizer Spitzenathleten. Doctoral dissertation, Universität Heidelberg, Heidelberg.

Google Scholar

Liu, Y.-T., Mayer-Kress, G., and Newell, K. M. (2003). Beyond curve fitting: a dynamical systems account of exponential learning in a discrete timing task. J. Mot. Behav. 35, 197–207. doi: 10.1080/00222890309602133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, L. A., Vasudevan, E. V. L., and Bastian, A. J. (2011). Motor adaptation training for faster relearning. J. Neurosci. 31, 15136–15143. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1367-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marteniuk, R. G. (1974). Individual differences in motor performances and learning. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2, 103–130.

Google Scholar

Matveev, L. P. (1970). Problemy izuèenija struktury trenirovki. Teor. Prak. Fiz. Kult. 33, 5–10.

Google Scholar

Mendoza, L., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (1990). “Technical training in the field of high performance athletes with a biomechanical feedback system,” in Proceedings of the Techniques in Athletics - The First International Conference, eds G. P. Brüggemann, and J. K. Rühl, (Köln: Strauß), 412–419.

Google Scholar

Newell, K. M., Liu, Y. T., and Mayer-Kress, G. (2001). Time scales in motor learning and development. Psychol. Rev. 108, 57–82. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neyman, J., and Pearson, E. S. (1928). On the use and interpretation of certain test criteria for purposes of statistical inference: part I. Biometrika 20A, 175–240. doi: 10.1093/biomet/20A.1-2.175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsch, J. R., Neumaier, A., Mester, J., and Marées, H. D. (1997). Techniktraining: Beiträge zu Einem Interdisziplinären Ansatz. Schorndorf: Hofmann.

Google Scholar

Nixon, M. S., Carter, J. N., Cunado, D., Huang, P. S., and Stevenage, S. V. (2006). “Automatic gait recognition,” in Biometrics: Personal Identification in Networked Society, eds A. K. Jain, R. Bolle, and S. Pankanti, (New York, NY: Springer), 231–250.

Google Scholar

Nuzzo, R. (2014). Scientific method: statistical errors. Nature 506, 150–152. doi: 10.1038/506150a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlović, R., and Idrizović, K. (2017). Factor analysis of world record holders in athletic decathlon. Sport Sci. 10, 109–116.

Google Scholar

Rein, R., Davids, K., and Button, C. (2010). Adaptive and phase transition behavior in performance of discrete multi-articular actions by degenerate neurobiological systems. Exp. Brain Res. 201, 307–322. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2040-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, A. (2012). Movement pattern recognition in basketball free-throw shooting. Hum. Mov. Sci. 31, 360–382. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A., and Young, D. E. (1991). Methodology for motor learning: a paradigm for kinematic feedback. J. Mot. Behav. 23, 13–24. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1991.9941590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnabel, G., Harre, D., and Borde, A. (1997). Trainingswissenschaft: Leistung - Training - Wettkampf. Berlin: Sportverlag.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I. (1993). Biomechanische Einzelfallanalyse im Diskuswurf. Frankfurt: Harri Deutsch.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I. (1998). Systemdynamische Betrachtung komplexer Bewegungsmuster im Lernprozess - Prozessorientierte Strukturierung der Entwicklung eines Bewegungsablaufs mit Hilfe biomechanischer Beschreibungsgrößen. Frankfurt: Peter Lang Verlag.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I., and Bauer, H.-U. (1997). “Linear vs nonlinear classification of complex time course patterns,” in Proceedings of the Second Annual Congress of the European College of Sport Science, eds J. Bangsbo, B. Saltin, H. Bonde, Y. Hellsten, B. Ibsen, M. Kjaer, et al. (Copenhagen: University of Copenhagen), 308–309.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I., and Bauer, H. U. (1998). “Identifying individual movement styles in high performance sports by means of self organizing Kohonen maps,” in Proceedings of the XVI International Symposium on Biomechanics in Sports, eds H. J. Riehle, and M. M. Vieten, (Konstanz: Universitätsverlag), 574–577.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I., Nigg, B. M., Stefanyshyn, D. J., and Liu, W. (2002). Identification of individual walking patterns using time discrete and time continuous data sets. Gait Posture 15, 180–186. doi: 10.1016/s0966-6362(01)00193-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I., Peham, C., Licka, T., and Scheidl, M. (2006). A pattern recognition approach for the quantification of horse and rider interactions. Equine Vet. J. Suppl. 38, 400–405. doi: 10.1111/j.2042-3306.2006.tb05576.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I., Röber, F., Jaitner, T., Hellstern, W., and Käubler, W. (2001). “Discrete and continuous effects of traditional and differential sprint training,” in Proceedings of the Sixth Annual Congress of the European College of Sport Science, eds J. Mester, G. King, H. Strüder, E. Tsolakidis, and A. Osterburg, (Köln: Sport und Buch Strauss), 331.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. I., Stefanysbyn, D. J., Nigg, B. M., and Liu, W. (1999). Recognition of individual walking patterns by means of artificial neural nets. Gait Posture 10, 85–86. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(99)90454-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schöner, G., Zanone, P. G., and Kelso, J. A. (1992). Learning as change of coordination dynamics: theory and experiment. J. Mot. Behav. 24, 29–48. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schorer, J., Baker, J., Fath, F., and Jaitner, T. (2007). Identification of interindividual and intraindividual movement patterns in handball players of varying expertise levels. J. Mot. Behav. 39, 409–421. doi: 10.3200/jmbr.39.5.409-422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, C., and Schöllhorn, W. I. (1995). “Verlaufsorientierte Strukturierung verschiedener Stützphasen des Sprintlaufs mit Hilfe der P- und S-Faktorenanalyse und Referenzfunktionen,” in Sport im Lebenslauf, eds D. Schmidtbleicher, K. Bös, and A. Müller (Hamburg: Czwalina), 299–302.

Google Scholar

Stevenage, S. V., Nixon, M. S., and Vince, K. (1999). Visual analysis of gait as a cue to identity. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 13, 513–526. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199912)13:6<513::aid-acp616>3.0.co;2-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, S. A., Stevenson, E., Spear, A., and Weeks, D. L. (1994). Scanning the dynamics of reaching and grasping movements. Hum. Mov. Sci. 13, 255–289. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(94)90040-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: motor learning, pattern recognition, high-performance sports, machine learning, support vector machine, individuality, transdisciplinary individuality

Citation: Horst F, Janssen D, Beckmann H and Schöllhorn WI (2020) Can Individual Movement Characteristics Across Different Throwing Disciplines Be Identified in High-Performance Decathletes? Front. Psychol. 11:2262. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02262

Received: 13 May 2020; Accepted: 11 August 2020;
Published: 18 September 2020.

Edited by:

Ana Filipa Silva, Polytechnic Institute of Maia, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Ana Paulo, Université d’Orléans, France
Howard N. Zelaznik, Purdue University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Horst, Janssen, Beckmann and Schöllhorn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wolfgang I. Schöllhorn, schoellw@uni-mainz.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.