Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 08 February 2017
Sec. Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances in Facet Theory Research: Developments in Theory and Application and Competing Approaches View all 12 articles

Commentary: Wild psychometrics: Evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Gloucestershire, Cheltenham, UK
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
  • 3School of Communication, Emerson College, Boston, MA, USA

A commentary on
Wild psychometrics: Evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes

by Shaw, R. C., Boogert, N. J., Clayton, N. S., and Burns, K. C. (2015). Anim. Behav. 109, 101–111. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.001

Studies into the psychometric structure of human intelligence and cognitive ability have identified a “g” or general factor, e.g., Buckhalt (2002), and in comparative psychology, in primates: Reader et al. (2011) and in rodents: Matzel et al. (2003). As well as this “g” factor, intelligence is conceptualized as possessing multiple specific intelligences. Due perhaps to more recent discoveries regarding the complexity of birds cognitive abilities (see, Emery, 2016), birds have become the subject of psychometric investigation (e.g., Ackerman, 2016).

Seminally, Shaw et al. (2015) assembled a test battery for avian cognition to assess the cognitive abilities in New Zealand robins (Petroica longipes). Their test battery was comprised of a series of, “tasks based on established measures of avian cognitive performance: a motor task, color and shape discrimination, reversal learning, spatial memory and inhibitory control.” (Shaw et al., 2015, p. 101). These scientists found robins to vary greatly in their ability to solve tasks. They also discovered weak, positive, non-significant correlations between performances on most tasks. They analyzed test performance using principle component analysis (PCA) using the criteria for extracting components as being the possession of an eigenvalue of unity or above. All sub-tests were found to load positively on the first component, which explained >34% of between-test variance. As a consequence of this finding, the authors identified this first factor aa a “g” factor, and concluded that New Zealand robins, tested in their wild habitat, displayed a general cognitive factor analogous to the human “g” factor. They continue to draw similarities between their own research and the body of literature on human general intelligence. In their article, Shaw et al. (2015) state that: “In human psychometric testing, individuals' scores in tests of diverse cognitive processes are positively correlated, and a “g” factor typically accounts for at least 40% of total variance” (Shaw et al., 2015, p 101).

However, several researchers, (e.g., Guttman, 1965, 1981; Koop, 1985; Guttman and Levy, 1991) have expressed concerns with the use of PCA to analyze tests of cognition as this procedure embodies specific assumptions regarding the data being analyzed that may not be met. For example, PCA requires that the data being subjected to PCA possesses the following characteristics: that the data is parametric in nature; that there is a correspondence between the data and the psychological structure of the construct being assessed; that comparability exists between the measurement scales in different sub-tests, etc.

Within human psychometrics questions have been asked regarding the nature of the relationship between general and specific forms of intelligence. Rimoldi (1951) hypothesized upon the nature of a “g” factor in intelligence. Koop (1985) notes how Rimoldi proposed that the “g” component was better understood as being a second-order factor (as being a product of specific intelligences) rather than as a primary factor with secondary specific intelligences. A psychometric approach that has been employed to analyse human cognition (and many other forms of behavior and experience) is that of facet theory (see Hackett, 2014, 2016). Typically, when a facet theory perspective is utilized, a definitional framework, known as a mapping sentence, is developed and research instruments are designed to investigate this initial propositional definition. Data reduction techniques (similar to PCA, but having different assumptions about the data), such as smallest space analysis (SSA) and partial order scalogram analysis (POSA) are used to test the veracity of the mapping sentence. Another way in which facet theory has been used is to analyse existing data sets. The data set produced through Shaw et al.'s research into New Zealand robin's cognitive performance is a data set that would be suited to such analysis.

An example of how a facet theory approach has been sued to interrogate an existing data set is Koop's (1985) reanalysis of Rimoldi's (1951) data using facet theory's analytic procedure: SSA and found a radial (qualitative) array of sub-tests (geometrical; numerical and verbal tests). Furthermore, he discovered that each of these sub-types of intelligence sub-tests were modified in terms of whether a specific sub-test required: inference, application or learning. Other researchers have divided the “g” intelligence into two forms: fluid (gf) and crystalized (gc) general intelligence (Cattell, 1963; Horn, 1988). Beauducel et al. (2001) used the facet theory approach to investigate the structure of fluid and crystalized general intelligence. They discovered support for the discrete existence of the tests of: verbal, numerical and figural abilities, with these being divided into: fluid (gf) and crystalized (gc) general intelligence, in a 2 × 3 structure.

Shaw et al.'s (2015) psychometric test battery and framework has demonstrated cognitive structures analogous to human intelligence and their findings are an extremely important steps toward better understanding how birds cognitively experience and process their world. Of particular relevance to this commentary is the fact that Shaw and colleagues identification of a complex structure to exist within avian intelligence offers a new research area for facet theory.

To conclude, facet theory approach has allowed the depiction and modeling of human cognitive processes and for understanding human intelligence. It is the contention of this author that facet theory provides a way to conceptualize and design avian cognition research. The approach also provides a way to analyse data that arises from research designed in this way using non-parametric techniques (e.g., SSA and POSA) to reveal the structure of avian intelligence in the form of a mapping sentence. By using a mapping sentence to design research and non-parametric statistical analyses this allows the investigation of the structure of avian intelligence in a way that does not make unsubstantiated assumptions of the psychometric qualities of the data. Furthermore, facet theory based research produces cumulative and directly comparable results across studies employing a faceted design. Thus, the use of facet theory could potentially develop greater understanding of avian cognitive processes.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank the authors of the article upon which this commentary is based (Shaw, R. C., Boogert, N. J., Clayton, N. S., and Burns, K. C.) for their co-operation and for making their work available to me.

References

Ackerman, J. (2016). The Genius of Birds. London: Penguin Press HC.

Beauducel, A., Brocke, B., and Liepmann, D. (2001). Perspectives on fluid and crystallized intelligence: facets for verbal, numerical, and figural intelligence. Pers. Indiv. Diff. 30, 977–994. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00087-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckhalt, J. A. (2002). A short history of g: psychometrics' most enduring and controversial construct. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 13, 101–114. doi: 10.1016/S1041-6080(02)00074-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: a critical experiment. J. Educ. Psychol. 54, 1–22. doi: 10.1037/h0046743

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, N. (2016). Bird Brain: An Exploration of Avian Intelligence. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Guttman, L. (1965). “A faceted definition of intelligence,” in Studies in Psychology, Scripta Hierosolymitana, ed R. Eiferman (Jerusalem: The Hebrew University), 166–181.

Guttman, L. (1981). “What is not what in statistics,” in Multidimensional Data Representations: When and Why, ed I. Borg (Ann Arbor: Mathesis Press), 20–46.

Guttman, L., and Levy, S. (1991). Two structural laws for intelligence tests. Intelligence 15, 79–103. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(91)90023-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackett, P. M. W. (2014). Facet Theory and the Mapping Sentence: Evolving Philosophy, Use and Application. Basingstoke: Palgrave.

Google Scholar

Hackett, P. M. (2016). Facet theory and the mapping sentence as hermeneutically consistent structured meta-ontology and structured meta-mereology. Front. Psychol. 7:471. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, J. (1988). “Thinking about human abilities,” in Handbook of Multivariate Experimental Psychology, eds J. R. Nesselroade and R. B. Cattell (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 645–685.

Koop, T. (1985). “Replication of Guttman's structure of intelligence,” in Facet Theory: Approaches to Social Research, ed D. Canter (New York, NY: Springer Verlag), 237–244.

Matzel, L. D., Han, Y. R., Grossman, H., Karnik, M. S., Patel, D., Scott, N., et al. (2003). Individual differences in the expression of a “general” learning ability in mice. J. Neurosci. 23, 6423–6433.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., and Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1017–1027. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimoldi, H. J. A. (1951). The central intellective factor. Psychometrica 16, 75–101. doi: 10.1007/BF02313429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, R. C., Boogert, N. J., Clayton, N. S., and Burns, K. C. (2015). Wild psychometrics: evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes. Anim. Behav. 109, 101–111. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: facet theory, cognition, avian intelligence, comparative psychology, “g” factor in intelligence, psychometrics

Citation: Hackett PMW (2017) Commentary: Wild psychometrics: Evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes. Front. Psychol. 8:165. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00165

Received: 12 December 2016; Accepted: 25 January 2017;
Published: 08 February 2017.

Edited by:

Erica Cosentino, Ruhr University Bochum, Germany

Reviewed by:

Jonas Rose, University of Tübingen, Germany

Copyright © 2017 Hackett. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Paul M. W. Hackett, paul@hackettspace.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.