Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 May 2016
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Phonological and visual processing, reading and writing skills in students with dyslexia and ADHD. View all 28 articles

Brazilian Normative Data on Letter and Category Fluency Tasks: Effects of Gender, Age, and Geopolitical Region

\r\nIzabel Hazin*Izabel Hazin1*Gilmara LeiteGilmara Leite2Rosinda M. OliveiraRosinda M. Oliveira3Joo C. AlencarJoão C. Alencar4Helenice C. FichmanHelenice C. Fichman5Priscila d. N. MarquesPriscila d. N. Marques3Claudia Berlim de MelloClaudia Berlim de Mello2
  • 1Departmento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
  • 2Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 3Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
  • 4Departamento de Biociências da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
  • 5Departamento de Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Verbal fluency is a basic function of language that refers to the ability to produce fluent speech. Despite being an essentially linguistic function, its measurements are also used to evaluate executive aspects of verbal behavior. Performance in verbal fluency (VF) tasks varies according to age, education, and cognitive development. Neurodevelopmental disorders that affect the functioning of frontal areas tend to cause lower performance in VF tasks. Despite the relative consensus that has been reached in terms of the use of VF tasks for the diagnosis of dyslexia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, few studies have considered regional variations in Brazil. The present study sought to provide normative data on VF tasks in children by considering gender, age, education, and geopolitical region of origin with auxiliary purposes in neuropsychological diagnosis of disorders that occur with executive changes The study included 298 participants, 7–10 years of age of both genders, who performed three letter fluency tasks and three category fluency tasks. The data were subjected to correlational and variance analyses, with age and gender as factors. No effect of gender on the children's performance was found. However, significant differences between age groups were observed, with better performance in letter tasks in older children and better performance in letter tasks compared with category tasks. Significant regional differences in performance on the letter VF task were observed. These results reinforce the importance of regional normative data in countries with high regional cultural variations, such as Brazil.

Introduction

Verbal fluency (VF) is a language function that is related to the ability to produce fluent speech and is typically tested in the letter and category domains (Lezak et al., 2012). VF tasks require the spontaneous generation of words according to specific criteria, under time constraints. In a letter fluency task, the subject is required to produce words that begin with specific letters (e.g., F, A, and S). In a category fluency task, the subject is required to say words that belong to specific semantic categories (e.g., animals, fruits, and clothing; Strauss et al., 2006; Moura et al., 2013).

VF tasks provide measurements of a wide range of cognitive functions, such as (1) executive function (e.g., systematic search, cognitive flexibility, and processing speed), (2) working memory and semantic memory, (3) language, and (4) verbal ability, which requires speaking and knowledge of words (Moura et al., 2015).

Performance on these tasks is particularly dependent on frontal and temporal regions of the left cerebral hemisphere, providing information about executive function and lexical skills (Perret, 1974; Miller, 1984; Ruff et al., 1997; Troyer et al., 1998; Baldo et al., 2006; Davidson et al., 2008; Birn et al., 2010). Letter fluency tasks seem to rely more on regions of the frontal cortex, whereas category fluency tasks rely more on temporal lobe functioning (Moura et al., 2015).

Studies of individuals with typical development have shown that the number of words that are produced in VF tasks is associated with changes in lexical skills and executive function that are especially related to the maturation of frontal lobe regions and may be an important marker of improvements in these functions during infancy and adolescence (Chan and Poon, 1999; Korkman et al., 2001; Riva et al., 2001; Nieto et al., 2008).

Children who are diagnosed with neurodevelopmental disorders that affect frontal areas, such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Barkley, 1997; Castellanos et al., 2006; Coghill et al., 2014), usually present lower performance in VF tasks compared with typically developing children (Cohen et al., 1999; Puentes-Rozo et al., 2008; Banerjee et al., 2011). Consistent with such findings, letter fluency is particularly efficient in discriminating ADHD from typical development in adolescents (Abreu et al., 2013).

In individuals with dyslexia, most studies have reported lower VF scores compared with controls (Booth et al., 2010; Lima et al., 2013; Varvara et al., 2014; Moura et al., 2015). However, although letter fluency impairment is almost always found, category fluency was shown to be preserved in some studies (e.g., Frith, 1995; Lima et al., 2013).

Such performance differences in VF are presumably attributable to impairments in executive function that are characteristic of ADHD (Brosnan et al., 2002; Lima et al., 2013; Varvara et al., 2014) and have been systematically identified in developmental dyslexia (Booth et al., 2010). Children with these clinical conditions tend to utilize different strategies when performing VF tasks. Such strategies appear to be associated with potential deficits in executive components of initiation, self-monitoring, organization, and verbal planning, as well as verbal and mnemonic skills (Silveira et al., 2009; Fernández et al., 2010; Gonçalves et al., 2013; Abreu et al., 2013; Takács et al., 2014; Gonçaves, 2015).

Despite the relative consensus that has been reached with regard to the utility of VF tasks for the diagnosis of ADHD and dyslexia, controversy has arisen in terms of the impact of sociodemographic variables on performance. Inconsistent results have been reported when the influence of gender is considered. The gender variable has been assumed by some researchers to not be influential (Riva et al., 2001; Brucki and Rocha, 2004; Hurks et al., 2010). However, other studies have suggested that gender is a significant factor, with females performing better than males (Albuquerque et al., 2007; Moura et al., 2015). Overall consensus has been reached with regard to significant improvements in performance that are observed as age and education increase (Abreu et al., 2013; Esteves et al., 2015).

Few studies have considered the impact of regional differences on performance in VF tasks. Accounting for regional differences in cognitive performance needs to go beyond the type of school or a family's socioeconomic status and should consider broad cultural population variables, such as access to information, quality of education, parents' education level, social values, and customs, among others (Ardila, 2007). In developing countries, such as Brazil, large differences are observed in children's performance on formal educational achievement tests, depending on the region of the country (Instituto Nacional de Estudos e Pesquisas Educacionais Anísio Teixeira, 2013). Such evidence suggests that normative data for neuropsychological tests in Brazil should consider cultural variables. In the clinical context, such as procedures for diagnosing neurodevelopmental disorders, such caution in sample composition may reduce the risk of false positives.

According to Mitrushina et al. (2005), normative reports should provide information about exclusion criteria with regard to sample composition and should stipulate not only conditions whose influence is well known (e.g., conditions of a psychiatric nature) but also conditions whose influence is still unclear, such as geographic region, ethnicity, and occupation. Strauss et al. (2006) stated that clinicians should be cautious when using norms that are collected from different locations because variability in demographic factors that are associated with geographic regions may interfere with performance. These authors concluded, “…local norms are therefore an asset when available” (p. 54).

The aim of the present study was to provide normative data on letter fluency (i.e., the letters F, A, and M) and category fluency (i.e., the categories animals, fruits, and clothing) in Brazilian children based on a sample of healthy children, 7–10 years of age and with different education levels and geopolitical regions of origin (southeastern, northeastern, and northern Brazil).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study included 298 children, 7–10 years of age who were students from private schools in the following geopolitical regions of Brazil: northeast (cities located in the states of Paraíba and Rio Grande do Norte), north (city of Belém), and southeast (city of Rio de Janeiro). All of the children belonged to socioeconomic classes C and D (based on family monthly income). Only children who were placed in regular education (elementary school) and whose first language was Portuguese were selected for the sample. Children were excluded if (1) they needed some adaptation in the curriculum to assist with their learning (e.g., due to sensory, motor, or intellectual impairments that might influence performance or due to specific learning disorders that impact reading and/or writing), (2) they presented medical conditions with neurological or psychiatric manifestations, or (3) they were using psychotropic medications. The information for establishing these criteria were collected using questionnaires that were administered to the children's parents or legal guardians.

Materials

Three letter fluency tasks and three category fluency tasks were used. For letter fluency, the letters were “F,” “A,” and “M.” For category fluency, the categories were “animals,” “fruits,” and “clothing.” The words that were produced were recorded on protocols or tape-recorded.

Procedures

The fluency tasks were administered in the following fixed order: letter (F, A, and M) and category (animals, fruits, and clothing). For the letter fluency tasks, the participants were instructed to say words that began with the letters F, A, and M in three separate 1-min trials. The participants were instructed not to say proper nouns or derivations of the same word. Similarly, for the category fluency tasks, the participants were instructed to generate exemplars of animals, fruits, and clothing, each within 1 min. The total number of correct words that were produced in each of the six trials was counted, according to the correction criteria that were proposed by Charchat-Fichman et al. (2011). The letter fluency score was the sum of the number of correct words in the three letter trials, and the category fluency score was the sum of the number of correct words in the three category trials. The study followed all ethical principles according to Resolution No. 466/2012 of the National Health Council that governs research with humans.

Statistical Analysis

The data were analyzed using SPSS 20 software for Windows. Homogeneity among samples regarding gender and age (categorized, 7–10 years old) was tested using the χ2 test of independence, and Spearman's rank correlations were used to assess associations between age and grade level. Potential main effects of the demographic variables on VF and interactions were examined using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), followed by univariate analysis (ANOVA) and post hoc tests with Bonferroni correction. Effect sizes in the MANOVA were calculated and are presented as the partial eta-squared (ηp2). Pearson correlation coefficients between age and VF scores and between the various VF scores were calculated. The level of statistical significance for all of the analyses was 5%.

Results

The standardization sample in this study comprised 298 children, 7–10 years of age and residents in three Brazilian regions (southeast, northeast, and north). No difference was found between these regions in terms of the age distribution (χ2 = 7.24, p = 0.299) and gender (χ2 = 1.34, p = 0.51). Age and education level showed a strong positive correlation (ρ = 0.85, p < 0.001). Thus, age was elected as the variable of interest for the norms. The characterization of the participants is described in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characterization of the normative sample.

The MANOVA revealed effects of region (southeast, northeast, north; Pillai's trace = 0.157, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.079) and age (7, 8, 9, and 10 years; Pillai's trace = 0.313, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.104) on letter fluency and category fluency scores. No effect of gender was found, with no significant interactions.

Significant univariate effects of region were observed (ANOVA) for each letter: “F” [F(2, 298) = 3.75, p = 0.025, ηp2 = 0.027], “A” [F(2, 298) = 3.89, p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.028], and “M” [F(2, 298) = 2.99, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.028]. An effect of region on total letter fluency was found [F(2, 298) = 4.25, p < 0.015, ηp2 = 0.03]. For the letters “F” and “M,” children from the southeastern region presented better performance, whereas children from the northeastern region presented lower performance compared with children from the other regions studied. No effect of region was found on category fluency in the three separate trials or on total category fluency (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparisons of verbal fluency scores between regions.

The univariate ANOVA indicated that age significantly affected all letter fluency and category fluency tasks: “F” [F (3, 298) = 17.09, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.158], “A” [F(3, 298) = 10.86, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.106], “M” [F(3, 298 = 16.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.152], “animals” [F(3, 298) = 7.01, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.071], “clothes” [F(3, 298) = 9.71, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.096], “fruits” [F(3, 298) = 10.97, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.107], letter fluency [F(3, 298) = 23.29, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.203], category fluency [F(3, 298) = 14.61, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.138], and total fluency [F(3, 298) = 22.7, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.199]. The Bonferroni post hoc test revealed significant differences between several pairs of age groups (Figure 1). Seven-year-old children underperformed 9- and 10-year-old children in the letter fluency and category fluency tasks and differed from 8-year-old children only in total letter fluency scores. Eight-year-old children underperformed 9- and 10-year-old children in total category fluency scores and total letter fluency scores. No differences were found between 9- and 10 year-old children in category fluency or letter fluency.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Average number of words produced according to age and fluency task group for each letter fluency variable. Different letters over the bars indicate significant differences between groups (Bonferroni post hoc test, p < 0.05).

There were positive correlations between age and VF scores. Moderate correlations (r > 0.4) were found between the letter fluency variables (“F,” “A,” and “M”) and total VF scores. We also found correlations between VF variables (“F,” “A,” and “M”) and letter fluency and between semantic fluency variables (“animals,” “clothing,” and “fruits”) and category fluency, with moderate or weak correlations between letter fluency variables and semantic fluency variables (Table 3). These associations between VF variables provide evidence of the theoretical coherence of the proposed tasks.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between age and levels of letter fluency.

Considering that the main objective of the present study was to provide normative data, Table 4 summarizes means, standard deviations (SDs), and scores for low (–2 SD), low average (–2 to –1 SD), average (–1 to +1 SD), high average (+1 to +2 SD), and superior (> 2 SD) VF scores for each age group. Table 5 shows the means and SDs for each VF score according to age and region.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Brazilian standards for the verbal fluency test between 7 and 10 years of age.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Brazilian standards for the verbal fluency test between regions.

Discussion

The main goal of the present study was to provide normative data on performance in category and letter VF tasks in healthy Brazilian children, 7–10 years of age, adjusted for the following sociodemographic variables: gender, age, education, and region of origin. VF tasks are sensitive to different processes and cognitive abilities, particularly executive function and language, and are very relevant in both experimental and clinical contexts.

In the present study, no associations were found between performance in VF tasks (letter and category) and gender, corroborating the results of other studies (Riva et al., 2001; Brucki and Rocha, 2004; Hurks et al., 2010; Abreu et al., 2013; Moura et al., 2015). Thus, this variable was not used to stratify the normative data.

With regard to the effect of age, the findings confirmed the sensitivity of the VF tasks in identifying neurodevelopmental age markers (Matute et al., 2004; Martins et al., 2007; Moura et al., 2013). Seven-year-old children significantly underperformed 9- and 10-year-old children with regard to total letter fluency and total category fluency and differed from 8-year-old children only in the total number of words generated in the VF tasks. Eight-year-old children underperformed 9- and 10-year-old children in total category fluency scores and total letter fluency scores. No differences were found between 9- and 10-year-old children in category fluency or letter fluency, suggesting the establishment of a plateau in performance. If so, then such data would be different from those reported in some studies that considered that adequate performance in letter fluency tasks depends on frontal lobe maturation, the stabilization of which occurs around 12 years of age (Moura et al., 2013).

Based on functional imaging studies, we consider the existence of five periods of rapid growth of the frontal lobes; three of these periods can be identified in childhood. Five years of age is a milestone with regard to the first neuronal growth spurt in this area, with significant improvements in the level of attentional control processes. Additionally, the period from 7 to 9 years of age corresponds to the second growth spurt, consistent with the development of three other executive domains: information processing, cognitive flexibility, and the establishment of goals. During the third growth spurt, from 12 to 13 years of age (during the transition from childhood to adolescence), the aforementioned executive domains mature further, and executive control emerges (Diamond, 2002).

The long maturation period of the frontal cortex is one of the elements that contribute to the vulnerability to developmental disorders. The evolutionary recentness of the emergence of these abilities in humans appears to contribute equally to the susceptibility of this area to genetic and environmental variations. Consequently, a high level of comorbidity is found between alterations in executive function and the presence of invasive and global developmental disorders (e.g., autism spectrum disorder; Happe et al., 2006), maladaptive behavioral disorders (e.g., ADHD; Vital and Hazin, 2008), learning disorders (e.g., dyslexia; Gooch et al., 2011), neurological conditions (e.g., epilepsy; Luton et al., 2010), and disorders that are characterized by chromosomal alterations (e.g., Down syndrome; Lott and Dierssen, 2010).

One of the peculiarities of this study was the consideration of individual regions of origin as an independent variable. Children from northeastern Brazil presented lower performance in letter fluency compared with children from the other two regions. The participants were from three distinct regions of Brazil (northeast, north, and south), with important social and economic differences that are marked by social inequality in the country. The highest Human Development Index (HDI) are found for cities in the south and southeast of the country, whereas the northeastern cities have the worst HDI in Brazil. In the northeast region, 78% of the municipalities are characterized by low income (per capita income of R$624) and a high rate (60%) of income concentration (Instituto de Pesquisa Econômica Aplicada et al., 2013).

The impact of socioeconomic family variables and the dimensions that are considered in its rankings (e.g., parental education and family income) on the cognitive development of children, particularly regarding language and executive skills, has been reported in studies from different countries (Ardila et al., 2005; Nuru-Jeter et al., 2010; Baird et al., 2011; Sbicigo et al., 2013). Specific contexts associated with vulnerability and a lack of adequate stimulation impact the development and function of the prefrontal cortex. Although our entire sample was composed of students from private schools, these children belonged to socioeconomic classes C and D, suggesting disparities in access to different sources of information. These findings indicate that normative studies in child neuropsychology should consider the socioeconomic and cultural variability of a population when composing research samples. In the clinical context, such as procedures for diagnosing neurodevelopmental disorders, such caution in sample composition may reduce the risk of false positives.

Author Contributions

IH, GL, CB, HF, and RO make substantial contributions to conception and design, analysis and interpretation of data; GL, JA, and PM participate in acquisition of data.

Funding

This work was funded by CNPq (Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa) and UFRN (Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abreu, N., Argollo, N., Oliveira, F., Cardoso, A. L., Bueno, J. L. O., and Xavier, G. F. (2013). Semantic and phonologic verbal fluency tests for adolescents with ADHD. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 10, 63–71. Available online at: http://www.clinicalneuropsychiatry.org/pdf/03abreu.pdf

Albuquerque, C. P., Martins, C., and Simões, M. R. (2007). Testes de consciência fonológica da Bateria de Avaliação Neuropsicológica de Coimbra [Phonological awareness tests of Coimbra's Neuropsychological Assessment Battery]. Educação: Temas e Problemas 4, 101–118. Available online at: http://www.oei.es/14206.htm

Ardila A. (2007). “The impact of culture on neuropsychological test performance,” in International Handbook of Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology, eds B. P. Uzzell, M. O. Ponton, A. Ardila (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 23–44.

Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., and Guajardo, S. (2005). The influence of the parents' educational level on the development of executive functions. Dev. Neuropsychol. 28, 539–560. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2801_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baird, S., Friedman, S., and Schady, N. (2011). Aggregate income shocks and infant mortality in the developing world. Rev. Econ. Stat. 93, 847–856. doi: 10.1162/REST_a_00084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldo, J. V., Schwartz, S., Wilkins, D., and Dronkers, N. F. (2006). Role of frontal versus temporal cortex in verbal fluency as revealed by voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 12, 896–900. doi: 10.1017/s1355617706061078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banerjee, P., Grange, D. K., Steiner, R. D., and White, D. A. (2011). Executive strategic processing during verbal fluency performance in children with phenylketonuria. Child Neuropsychol. 17, 105–117. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.525502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birn, R. M., Kenworthy, L., Case, L., Caravella, R., Jones, T. B., Bandettini, P. A., et al. (2010). Neural systems supporting lexical search guided by letter and semantic category cues: a self-paced overt response fMRI study of verbal fluency. Neuroimage 49, 1099–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. N., Boyle, J. M. E., and Kelly, S. W. (2010). Do tasks make a difference? Accounting for heterogeneity of performance of children with reading difficulties on tasks of executive function: findings from a meta-analysis. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 28, 133–176. doi: 10.1348/026151009X485432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M., Demetre, J., Hamill, S., Robson, K., Shepherd, H., and Cody, G. (2002). Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 40, 2144–2155. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00046-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brucki, S. M. D., and Rocha, M. S. G. (2004). Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 37, 1771–1777. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2004001200002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Milham, M. P., and Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, A. S., and Poon, M. W. (1999). Performance of 7- to 95-year old individuals in a Chinese version of the category fluency test. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 5, 525–533. doi: 10.1017/S135561779956606X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charchat-Fichman, H. C., Oliveira, R. M., and Silva, A. M. (2011). Performance of Brazilian children on phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks. Dement. Neuropsychologia 5, 78–84. Available online at: http://www.demneuropsy.com.br/detalhe_artigo.asp?id=269

Coghill, D. R., Seth, S., and Matthews, K. (2014). A comprehensive assessment of memory, delay aversion, timing, inhibition, decision making and variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: advancing beyond the three-pathway models. Psychol. Med. 44, 1989–2001. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. J., Morgan, A. M., Vaughn, M., Riccio, C. A., and Hall, J. (1999). Verbal fluency in children: developmental issues and differential validity in distinguishing children with attention-deficit hyperactiviety disorder and two subtypes of dyslexia. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 14, 433–443.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Davidson, P. S. R., Gao, F. Q., Mason, W. P., Winocur, G., and Anderson, N. D. (2008). Verbal fluency, trail making, and wisconsin card sorting test performance following right frontal lobe tumor resection. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 30, 18–32. doi: 10.1080/13803390601161166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2002). “Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood (cognitive functions, anatomy and biochemistry),” in The Frontal Lobes, eds D. T. Stuss and R. G. Knight (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 466–503.

Esteves, C. S., Oliveira, C. R., Moret-Tatayb, C., Navarro-Pardoc, X. X. X., Carlia, G. A., et al. (2015). Phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks: normative data for elderly Brazilians. Psicol. Reflexão e Crítica 28, 350–355. doi: 10.1590/1678-7153.201528215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, A. Y., Miranda, B. R., and Casas, A. M. (2010). Funciones ejecutivas, comprensión de historias y coherencia narrativa en niños con trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad. R. Logopedia Foniatría Audiol. 30, 151–161. doi: 10.1016/S0214-4603(10)70163-7

CrossRef Full Text

Frith, U. (1995). Dyslexia: can we have a shared theoretical framework? Educ. Child Psychol. 12, 6–17.

Google Scholar

Gonçalves, H. A., Mohr, R. V., Moraes, A. L., Siqueira, L. S., Prando, M. L., and Fonseca, R. P. (2013). Componentes atencionais e de funções executivas em meninos com TDAH: dados de uma bateria neuropsicológica flexível. Braz. J. Psyquiatry 62, 13–21. doi: 10.1590/s0047-20852013000100003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonçaves, H. A. (2015). Clustering e Switching na Fluência Verbal Infantil: Idade, Tipo De Escola E TDAH. Porto Alegre: Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul.

Gooch, D., Snowling, M., and Hulme, C. (2011). Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 195–203. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02312.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happe, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., and Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain Cogn. 61, 25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurks, P., Schrans, D., Meijs, C., Wassenberg, R., Feron, F., and Jolles, J. (2010). Developmental changes in semantic verbal fluency: analyses of word productivity as a function of time, clustering, and switching. Child Neuropsychol. 16, 366–387. doi: 10.1080/09297041003671184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korkman, M., Kemp, S. L., and Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age and neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: a cross-sectional study on 800 children from the United States. Dev. Neuropsychol. 20, 331–354. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Bigler, E. D., and Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological Assessment, 5th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Lima, R. F., Azoni, C. A. S., and Ciasca, S. M. (2013). Attentional and executive deficits in Brazilian children with developmental dyslexia. Psychology 10A, 1–6. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.410A001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lott, I. T., and Dierssen, M. (2010). Cognitive deficits and associated neurological complications in individuals with down's syndrome. Lancet Neurol. 9, 623–633. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70112-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luton, L. M., Burns, T. G., and DeFilippis, N. (2010). Frontal lobe epilepsy in children and adolescents: a preliminary neuropsychological assessment of executive function. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 25, 762–770. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martins, I. P., Vieira, R., Loureiro, C., and Santos, M. E. (2007). Speech rate and fluency in children and adolescents. Child Neuropsychol. 13, 319–332. doi: 10.1080/09297040600837370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matute, E., Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., and Morales, G. (2004). Verbal and non-verbal fluency in Spanish-speaking children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 26, 647–660. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. (1984). Verbal fluency as a function of a measure of verbal intelligence and in relation to different types of cerebral pathology. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 23, 53–57. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1984.tb00626.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitrushina, M. N., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., and D'Elia, L. F. (2005). Handbook of Normative Data for Neuropsychological Assessment, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Moura, O., Simões, M. R., and Pereira, M. (2013). Fluência verbal semântica e fonêmica em crianças: Diferentes funções cognitivas e análise dos desempenhos ao longo dos 60 segundos [Different cognitive functions and performance analysis during 60 seconds]. Aval. Psicol. 12, 167–177.

Moura, O., Simões, M. R., and Pereira, M. (2015). Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. Clin. Neuropsychol. 28, 20–41. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2014.964326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Instituto Nacional de Estudos e Pesquisas Educacionais Anísio Teixeira (2013). Boletins com Resultados da Prova Brasil 2013. Available online at: portal.inep.gov.br/web/saeb/resultados-2013 (Accessed April 7, 2016).

Instituto de Pesquisa Econômica Aplicada, Empoderando Vidas–Fortalecendo Nações, Fundaçāo Joāo Pinheiro. (2013). Atlas do Desenvolvimento Humano no Brasil - Série Atlas do Desenvolvimento Humano no Brasil. Available online at: www.pnud.org.br/arquivos/idhm-brasileiro-atlas-2013.pdf (Accessed April 7, 2016).

Nieto, A., Galtier, I., Barroso, J., and Espinosa, G. (2008). Fluencia verbal en niños españoles en edad escolar: estudio normativo piloto y análisis de las estrategias organizativas. R. Neurol. 46, 2–6. Available online at: http://aidyne12.tizaypc.com/contenidos/contenidos/3/Modulo3-Ficha6-FluenciaVerbalNormativaNinios-Espaniola.pdf

Nuru-Jeter, M., Sarsour, K., Jutte, D. P., and Boyce, W. T. (2010). Socioeconomic predictors of health and development in middle childhood: variations by socioeconomic status measure and race. Issues Compr. Pediatr. Nurs. 33, 59–81. doi: 10.3109/01460861003663953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior. Neuropsychologia 12 323–330. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(74)90047-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puentes-Rozo, P. J., Barceló-Martínez, E., and Pineda, D. A. (2008). Características conductuales y neuropsicológicas de niños de ambos sexos, de 6 a 11 años, con trastorno por déficit de atención/hiperactividad. R. Neurol. 47, 175–184.

Riva, D., Nichelli, F., and Devoti, M. (2001). Developmental aspects of verbal fluency and confrontation naming in children. Brain Lang. 71, 267–284. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., and Levin, H. S. (1997). The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain Lang. 57, 394–405. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sbicigo, J. B., Abaid, J. L. W., Dell'Aglio, D. D., and Salles, J. F. (2013). Nível socioeconômico e funções executivas em crianças/adolescentes: revisão sistemática. Arq. Bras. Psicol. 65, 51–69. Available online at: http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/pdf/arbp/v65n1/v65n1a05.pdf

Silveira, D. C., Passos, L. M. A., Santos, P. C., and Chiappetta, A. L. M. L. (2009). Avaliação da fluência verbal em crianças com transtorno da falta de atenção com hiperatividade: um estudo comparativo. Revista CEFAC 11, 208–216. doi: 10.1590/s1516-18462009000600011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Takács, Á., Kóbor, A., Tárnok, Z., and Csépe, V. (2014). Verbal fluency in children with ADHD: strategy using and temporal properties. Child Neuropsychol. 20, 415–429. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.799645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyer, A. K., Moskovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M. P., and Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: the effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 36, 449–504. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00152-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varvara, P., Varuzza, C., Sorrentino, A. C. P., Vicari, S., and Menghini, D. (2014). Executive functions in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:120. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vital, M., and Hazin, I. (2008). Avaliação do desempenho escolar em matemática de crianças com transtorno de déficit de atenção/hiperatividade (TDAH): um estudo piloto. Ciências Cognição 13, 19–36. Available online at: http://www.cienciasecognicao.org/revista/index.php/cec/article/view/59/56

Keywords: letter fluency, category fluency, dyslexia, ADH

Citation: Hazin I, Leite G, Oliveira RM, Alencar JC, Fichman HC, Marques PdN and de Mello CB (2016) Brazilian Normative Data on Letter and Category Fluency Tasks: Effects of Gender, Age, and Geopolitical Region. Front. Psychol. 7:684. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00684

Received: 30 October 2015; Accepted: 23 April 2016;
Published: 10 May 2016.

Edited by:

Michael S. Dempsey, Boston University Medical Center, USA

Reviewed by:

Thomas James Lundy, virtuallaboratory.net, inc, USA
Qiuying Wang, Oklahoma State University, USA

Copyright © 2016 Hazin, Leite, Oliveira, Alencar, Fichman, Marques and de Mello. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Izabel Hazin, izabel.hazin@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.