Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 November 2017
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00550

Commentary: The Code for Facial Identity in the Primate Brain

Bruno Rossion1,2* Jessica Taubert3
  • 1Face Categorization Lab, Psychological Sciences Research Institute and Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
  • 2Service de Neurologie, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Nancy, Nancy, France
  • 3School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

A commentary on
The Code for Facial Identity in the Primate Brain

by Chang, L., and Tsao, D.Y. (2017). Cell 169, 1013.e14–1028.e14. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.011

A recent study analyzed the variation in firing rates among neurons of the monkey brain in response to a large set of humanoid face images (Chang and Tsao, 2017). The paper has received considerable attention, especially in the popular press, because it claims to have “cracked the code” underlying individual face recognition in primates. Here, we put this study in its historical scientific context and explain why this claim is not substantiated.

Variations in the firing rate of face-selective neurons to images of different individual faces has long been reported in the monkey infero-temporal (IT) cortex, with population coding proposed as a mechanism for the recognition of individual faces (Baylis et al., 1985; Rolls, 1992). Hence, the alternative account that a single neuron fires for a single face (a “grandmother cell”) is a straw man, which has never been seriously considered by this scientific community. Furthermore, an earlier study already showed that physically similar faces were represented by similar patterns of discharges across neurons sampled in monkey IT, with coding along two axes explaining most of the variance in the response pattern (Young and Yamane, 1992). The lack of reference to these previous studies might be because, at that time, functional magnetic resonance imaging in monkeys was not available to guide single cell recordings. Yet, specifically targeting face-selective patches, does not prevent the study of Chang and Tsao (2017) suffering from other methodological shortcomings. For instance, the study relies on a high-dimensional (n = 50) psychometric face identity space, testing neurons with monotonic response profiles, but at no point establishes a null hypothesis; i.e., what conditions or stimulus manipulations would result in poor decoding/encoding performance? If another linear space was created via the same process but with round fruits as the input images rather than faces, would we expect a different pattern of responses from face-selective neurons? Is a different pattern even possible when using such a high-dimensional space? Why would face identity decoding benefit from pooling neurons' responses across two monkey brains?

There are, however, more fundamental issues. As in previous studies cited above, these findings are thought of as revealing face identity coding in humans. Yet, there is no evidence that, like humans, monkeys rely primarily on facial information to individualize conspecifics in their natural environment, let alone are experts at individualizing human faces. In laboratory settings, monkeys need extensive training (i.e., hundreds of trials) to reach above chance performance at discriminating individual conspecific faces (Parr et al., 2000; Pokorny and de Waal, 2009). In these studies, even after conditioning, significant drops of performance are observed with novel images, indicating a reliance on image-based cues. Monkeys' viewing preference for novel vs. habituated images of conspecifics might also be entirely due to low-level image attributes and/or non-identity related cues (e.g., eye gaze direction) (Pascalis and Bachevalier, 1998). The argument that monkeys (also other nonhuman primates; Martin-Malivel et al., 2006) rely on image-based information is supported by the lack of a performance cost for inverted faces—arguably the clearest marker of expert face recognition in humans (Rossion, 2008)—when monkeys individualize conspecific faces (Rosenfeld and Van Hoesen, 1979; Bruce, 1982). Note that ambiguity regarding the inversion cost in monkeys could be attributed to confusion of inversion with rotation effects (Parr et al., 2000), or comparing trained upright to untrained inverted stimuli (Dahl et al., 2013). Other similar studies also measuring responses to transformed face stimuli do not support the view that monkeys rely on qualitatively similar processes as humans when individualizing faces (Parr et al., 2000, 2012).

If monkeys lack human-like expertise at individual face recognition, why would variations in single neuron spike rates in their brain successfully decode human faces? This issue concerns both the limitations of anatomico-functional brain homologies across species, and the interpretation of a classifier performance. In humans, the cortical face network is primarily located in the ventral occipito-temporal cortex (VOTC; Rossion et al., 2012). However, the temporal lobe in monkeys is much smaller and relatively thinner than in humans even after considering relative body size (Rilling and Seligman, 2002), their VOTC lacks a (right lateralized) fusiform gyrus critical for individual face recognition in humans (Meadows, 1974), and does not include face-selective regions. Rather, monkeys' cortical face network, where face-selective neurons are typically recorded, is in the superior temporal sulcus (STS), in addition to a small region in the lateral anterior temporal lobe. Strikingly, complete bilateral STS lesion in monkeys only has a small unselective effect on individual face recognition (Heywood and Cowey, 1992). However, it causes a severe deficit in discriminating eye gaze direction, a biologically relevant cue in monkeys, coded in synchrony with head orientation in their STS (Perrett et al., 1985). Interestingly, humans also have STS face-selective regions involved in coding dynamic aspects of faces such as deviations in eye gaze direction rather than face identity (Puce and Perrett, 2003). Hence, the STS face-selective network might be partly homologous across these two primate species sharing a common ancestor 25–30 million year ago. In contrast, a right lateralized VOTC face-selective network may have evolved selectively in the human lineage in response to an increase in morphologic and genetic interindividual variability in the face (Sheehan and Nachman, 2014) and social pressure to rapidly discriminate among many individuals.

In summary, the recently reported study (Chang and Tsao, 2017) does not constitute a step forward in our understanding of the neural mechanisms of individual face recognition in the human species. Instead, it serves as a timely reminder that successful pattern decoding of stimuli in a psychometric space with a linear classifier might be observed even when the units sampled are not important for the presumed function. To conclude with an analogy, although different complex sounds, such as different spoken words, would likely elicit different patterns of activity across neurons in the monkey auditory cortex, could high decoding performance and subsequent reconstruction of the words be used to understand language comprehension mechanisms in humans?

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Talia Retter, Michael Webster, and Jacques Jonas for helpful comments on a previous version of this commentary. BR is supported by the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research (FNRS).

References

Baylis, G. C., Rolls, E. T., and Leonard, C. M. (1985). Selectivity between faces in the responses of a population of neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey. Brain Res. 342, 91–102.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bruce, C. (1982). Face recognition by monkeys: absence of an inversion effect. Neuropsychologia 20, 515–521.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chang, L., and Tsao, D. Y. (2017). The code for facial identity in the primate brain. Cell 169, 1013.e14–1028.e14. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahl, C. D., Rasch, M. J., Tomonaga, M., and Adachi, I. (2013). The face inversion effect in non-human primates revisited - an investigation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Sci. Rep. 3:2504. doi: 10.1038/srep02504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heywood, C. A., and Cowey, A. (1992). The role of the “face-cell” area in the discrimination and recognition of faces by monkeys. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 335, 31–37.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Martin-Malivel, J., Mangini, M. C., Fagot, J., and Biederman, I. (2006). Do humans and baboons use the same information when categorizing human and baboon faces? Psychol. Sci. 17, 599–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01751.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meadows, J. C. (1974). The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychol. 37, 489–501.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Parr, L. A., Heintz, M., and Pradhan, G. (2000). Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) lack expertise in face processing. J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 390–402. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.122.4.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parr, L. A., Taubert, J., Little, A. C., and Hancock, P. J. (2012). The organization of conspecific face space in nonhuman primates. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 2411–2434. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.693110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascalis, O., and Bachevalier, J. (1998). Face recognition in primates: a cross-species study. Behav. Process. 43, 87–96.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Smith, P. A., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Milner, A. D., et al. (1985). Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 223, 293–317.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pokorny, J. J., and de Waal, F. B. (2009). Face recognition in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Comp. Psychol. 123, 151–160. doi: 10.1037/a0014073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puce, A., and Perrett, D. (2003). Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 435–445. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Rilling, J. K., and Seligman, R. A. (2002). A quantitative morphometric comparative analysis of the primate temporal lobe. J. Hum. Evol. 42, 505–533. doi: 10.1006/jhev.2001.0537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (1992). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 335, 11–20.

PubMed Abstract

Rosenfeld, S. A., and Van Hoesen, G. W. (1979). Face recognition in the monkey. Neuropsychologia 17, 503–509.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rossion, B. (2008). Picture-plane inversion leads to qualitative changes of face perception. Acta Psychol. 128, 274–289. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., Hanseeuw, B., and Dricot, L. (2012). Defining face perception areas in the human brain: a large-scale factorial fMRI face localizer analysis. Brain Cogn. 79, 138–157. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, M. J., and Nachman, M. W. (2014). Morphological and population genomic evidence that human faces have evolved to signal individual identity. Nat. Commun. 5:4800. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5800

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, M. P., and Yamane, S. (1992). Sparse population coding of faces in the inferotemporal cortex. Science 256, 1327–1331.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: monkey brain, individual face recognition, human specificity, ventral temporal cortex, decoding

Citation: Rossion B and Taubert J (2017) Commentary: The Code for Facial Identity in the Primate Brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:550. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00550

Received: 12 September 2017; Accepted: 31 October 2017;
Published: 14 November 2017.

Edited by:

Paul Fredrick Sowman, Macquarie University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Davide Rivolta, Università degli studi di Bari Aldo Moro, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Rossion and Taubert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bruno Rossion, bruno.rossion@uclouvain.be

Download