Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 04 April 2018
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 10 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00091

Two Behavioral Tests Allow a Better Correlation Between Cognitive Function and Expression of Synaptic Proteins

  • 1Center for Neurobiology of Aging, INRCA, IRCCS, Ancona, Italy
  • 2Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Section of Neuroscience and Cell Biology, Università Politecnica delle Marche, Ancona, Italy
  • 3Fondazione di Medicina Molecolare, Università Politecnica delle Marche, Ancona, Italy

The molecular substrate of age-associated cognitive decline (AACD) is still elusive. Evidence indicates that AACD is related to synaptic impairment in hippocampus, but different hippocampal regions play different roles, with the dorsal hippocampus (DH) associated to spatial learning, and the ventral hippocampus (VH) crucial for emotionality. If changes in hippocampal function contributes to AACD, this contribution may be reflected in alterations of synaptic protein levels. A commonly used approach to investigate this issue is western blotting. When this technique is applied to the entire hippocampus and the cognitive impairment is evaluated by a single task, changes in expression of a protein might undergo a “dilution effect”, as they may occur only in a given hippocampal region. We show that two behavioral tests yield more accurate results than one test in evaluating the function of the whole rat hippocampus by studying the expression of synaptotagmin 1 (SYT1), a vesicular protein whose expression in aged hippocampus is reportedly inconsistent. Analysis of SYT1 levels in the whole hippocampus of rats selected by the Morris water maze (MWM) test only failed to highlight a difference, whereas analysis of SYT1 levels in the whole hippocampus of rats categorized by both the MWM and the step-through passive avoidance (STPA) tests demonstrated a significant increase of SYT1 level in impaired rats. These findings, besides showing that SYT1 increases in impaired aged rats, suggest that using the whole hippocampus in blotting studies may prevent false negative results only if animals are categorized with tests exploring both DH and VH.

Memory dysfunction is a hallmark of brain aging. Although numerous studies have been focused on this phenomenon over the last decades (Park et al., 2002; Salthouse, 2003, 2012; Butler et al., 2004; Dixon and De Frias, 2014; Pudas et al., 2014), the molecular substrates of age-related cognitive deficits are still elusive.

Mounting evidence indicates that age-associated cognitive decline (AACD) can be related to synaptic impairment in hippocampus (e.g., Vanguilder and Freeman, 2011; Vanguilder et al., 2011), and investigations on synaptic structure and function and on synaptic proteins have flourished in the recent past (e.g., Burke and Barnes, 2006; Bishop et al., 2010; Bano et al., 2011). Indeed, the hippocampus is involved in important aspects of learning and memory (e.g., Bettio et al., 2017), but different hippocampal regions play different roles. In their 1995 article, Moser et al. (1995) showed that removal of 40% of the dorsal hippocampus (DH) impaired spatial learning, whereas removal of 70% of the ventral part did not affect performance in this test. On the contrary, the ventral hippocampus (VH), as part of a network comprising prefrontal cortex, amygdala and subcortical structures associated with hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, is crucial for emotionality, i.e., fear, anxiety and depression (Kjelstrup et al., 2002; Fanselow and Dong, 2010; McLaughlin and Gobbi, 2012; Bannerman et al., 2014).

If changes in hippocampal synaptic function contribute to AACD, this contribution may be determined by or reflected in alterations of synaptic protein levels. A commonly used approach to investigate such an issue is western blotting. Indeed, it is informative, easy, inexpensive and non-time consuming. However, when this tecnique is applied to the entire hippocampus in order to obtain enough material to study a broad pannel of structurally and functionally linked proteins and the cognitive impairment is evaluated by a single task, significant changes in expression of a given protein might undergo a “dilution effect”, as they may occur only in a given hippocampal subregion underpinnig a specific functional domain (Shimohama et al., 1998; Chen et al., 2007; Cao et al., 2013).

The Two Behavioral Tests Approach

To overcome this problem, we reasoned that the hippocampal regions analyzed by western blotting, hence the amount of tissue examined and the ability of the method to detect alterations in proteins levels, can be increased by employing a larger number of behavioral tests. Here, we report some methodological considerations gathered in the course of our ongoing investigations on the correlation between the level of synaptic proteins, studied by western blotting, and behavioral analysis. We administrated in series the Morris water maze (MWM) and the step-through passive avoidance (STPA) tests. We considered these tests suitable to validate/reject our hypothesis for three reasons: (i) together, they allow to assess the performance of both the DH and the VH (Moser et al., 1995; Lorenzini et al., 1996; Ambrogi Lorenzini et al., 1997; Wang et al., 2017); (ii) the MWM test is the preferred test for assessing core aspects of spatial learning and memory (Vorhees and Williams, 2014), while one-trial inhibitory avoidance is the best studied task concerning the molecular post-training processes of hippocampal cellular memory consolidation (Izquierdo et al., 2016); and (iii) evidence exists that spatial and aversive memories, both crucial for a proper interaction with the environment, undergo age-related changes (Lovatel et al., 2013; Beaudet et al., 2014; Leffa et al., 2014).

We first administered the MWM test to 40 old male Sprague-Dawley albino rats (24 months of age). Thirty-four animals were categorized as Impaired (n = 19) and Non-impaired (n = 15), while six (15%) were excluded because they were unable to learn the task (Figures 1A–C). The addition of the STPA test determined the exclusion of further 16 animals, because we selected only animals that exhibited the same results in both tests. Indeed, only 18 of the 34 rats that were categorized by the MWM test could be included after categorization with the STPA test (12 Impaired, 6 Non-impaired), whereas a further 16 did not comply the inclusion criteria. Altogether, 55% of the cohort was excluded (Figures 1A,D). Therefore, in our experimental conditions, performing two tests implied that less than half of the animals passed the selection and were categorized as Impaired and Non-impaired. Notably, the calculation does not take into account physiological mortality.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Diagram showing animal categorization and numbers. The initial cohort included 40 male Sprague-Dawley albino rats aged 24 months. Performing the Morris water maze (MWM) test according to Vorhees and Williams (2006) yielded 19 Impaired and 15 Non-impaired rats, while 6 rats were excluded as they were unable to significantly reduce the distance moved to find the platform during the 5-day learning phase. These 34 rats were then subjected to the step-through passive avoidance (STPA) test according to Platano et al. (2008). This step produced 12 Impaired and 6 Non-impaired rats according to both tests; 16 rats had opposite result in the two tests, and were excluded. (B) Percent time spent in the target quadrant at probe day of the 34 rats classified by the MWM test. Animals were classified as Non-Impaired if the time spent in the target quadrant was >30% and superior to that spent in each non-target quadrant; rats where categorized as Impaired if their permanence in the target quadrant was <25%. To exclude any categorization bias due to visual deficits, a post hoc control was applied and the distance moved by Non-Impaired and Impaired rats to find a multiple-located visible platform was compared. (C) Examples of performances at probe day. The first panel represents a Non-Impaired rat with a percentage of time spent in the target quadrant of 51; the second and the third panels represent the performance of two Impaired rats with percentages of time spent in the target quadrants of 24 (random swimming equally distributed in all the quadrants) and 17 (preferential searching in non-target quadrants), respectively. (D) Latency at test day of the STPA task of the 34 rats classified by the MWM test. A value <100 s to enter the dark compartment 24-h after the shock release was taken to indicate Impairment and a latency >200 s was taken to indicate Non-impairment. Horizontal lines indicate mean ± SEM values.

Next, we tested the hypothesis that two behavioral tasks provide more accurate results than one test alone in evaluating the function of the whole rat hippocampus by studying the expression of the vesicular protein synaptotagmin 1 (SYT1). We specifically selected SYT1 because its expression in the aged hippocampus provided inconsistent results to date. Indeed, Chen et al. (2007), who categorized their animals with the MWM test and used only dorsal hippocampal tissue for western blotting analysis, reported that cognitive impairment correlated with increased SYT1 levels, whereas Nicolle et al. (1999), who categorized the animals by the same test but examined the whole hippocampus found no difference in SYT1 levels. In our study, analysis of SYT1 levels in the whole hippocampus of rats selected by the MWM test alone, as in the study of Nicolle et al. (1999), failed to highlight a difference (Figure 2), whereas analysis of SYT1 levels in the whole hippocampus of rats categorized with both the MWM and the STPA tests demonstrated a significant increase of SYT1 level in rats with cognitive impairment (117.2% ± 5.64% vs. Non-Impaired rats; p = 0.029, Mann Whitney test; Figure 2). Therefore, using the entire hippocampus to analyze proteins levels requires classifying animals by at least two tests, exploring the dorsal and ventral portion of the hippocampus, if false negative results are to be avoided.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Western blotting analysis of hippocampal tissue from male Sprague-Dawley albino rats categorized only with the Morris water maze (MWM) test or with the MWM and the step-through passive avoidance (STPA) tests (MWM+STPA). Rats were anesthetized with intraperitoneal chloral hydrate (300 mg/kg), decapitated, and the hippocampi were quickly collected. Homogenization and crude synaptic plasma membrane preparation were as in Danbolt et al. (1990) and Marcotulli et al. (2017). The Bio-Rad Protein Assay (Bio-Rad Laboratories GmbH, Munich, Germany) was used to determine the total amount of protein in each homogenate (3–4 measurements/ homogenate). For quantitative analysis, standard curves with increasing total protein concentrations were drawn to define a linear range for immunoblot densitometric analysis (Bragina et al., 2006). For optimal resolution of synaptotagmin 1 (SYT1; 105011 clone 41.1; 1:500; Synaptic System, Göttingen, Germany; RRID AB_887832) concentrations, western blotting analysis was performed in crude synaptic membranes using 7 μg of total protein. Immunoblot densitometric analysis of MWM group rats was conducted in tissue from 11 Non-impaired (green) and 13 Impaired subjects (red; 108.5% ± 5.72% of Non-impaired rats), whereas the MWM+STPA group included 5 Non-impaired (green) and 9 Impaired (red) rats (117.2% ± 5.64% of Non-impaired rats; *p = 0.029, Mann Whitney test). Circles represent animals that were Non-Impaired (green) or Impaired (red) at both MWM and STPA tests, while squares represent animals that had discordant performances at the two tests (i.e., Non-Impaired at MWM test but Impaired at STPA test (green) and Impaired at MWM test but Non-Impaired at STPA test (red)). Horizontal lines indicate mean ± SEM values. Graphs include representative western blottings of Non-Impaired and Impaired animals classified by MWM (top) or MWM+STPA (bottom) tests.

How to Choose?

Our findings raise the question whether, when investigating cognitive decline, information should be obtained from a single hippocampal sub-region or from the whole structure. The answer to this question is important for the proper interpretation of results. Rats categorization with two behavioral tests allows using the entire hippocampus for western blotting studies, i.e., about 300 μg of protein in crude synaptic plasma membrane preparations (Danbolt et al., 1990; see also Marcotulli et al., 2017). Since 4–5 measurements and 7 μg of protein per measurement are generally required for each antigen, the whole hippocampus allows testing 8–10 antigens. In contrast, the use of the MWM test alone involves that although a larger number of categorized animals is available (meaning that one could employ half the animals that are needed for two tests protocol), only the upper third of the hippocampus should be evaluated (Chen et al., 2007; Cao et al., 2013). This amount of tissue is sufficient to test only 2–3 antigens. If the western blotting study will involve only few proteins, it would be preferable to select animals using exclusively one test, and to analyze only the involved part of the hippocampus, thus allowing to employ fewer animals. If, however, more proteins are to be investigated, the two-tests approach will allow to use all the hippocampal tissue, thus maximizing the ratio between the amount of tissue and the number of animals used.

Conclusion

Our findings show a relationship between AACD, assessed by the MWM and the STPA tests, and increased SYT1 levels in the whole hippocampus of aged rats. They also suggest that the use of the whole hippocampus in western blotting analysis may avoid false negative results if animals have been categorized with behavioral tests that explore both the DH and the VH. Moreover, these findings might shed new light in the field of cognitive dysfunctions, in particular Alzheimer’s disease. Indeed, synaptotagmins seem to play a role as APP interactors in promoting Aβ generation (Gautam et al., 2015; Kuzuya et al., 2016): the application of the present methodology to assess their level could help to better clarify their possible involvement in Alzheimer’s disease etiopathogenesis.

Ethics Statement

All experimental procedures involving animals and their care were carried out in accordance with the European Community Council Directive guidelines (2010/63/UE) and approved by the Italian Ministry of Health (code 336/2016-PR).

Author Contributions

FC and GF conceived the project. MB, GF, AP, DM and LB performed the experiments. MB and GF gathered and analyzed the data. FC supervised the project and discussed the data. GF, MB and FC wrote the article.

Funding

This work was supported by grants provided by Università Politecnica delle Marche to GF and FC; by Ministero dell’Istruzione, dell’Università e della Ricerca (PRIN grant 2010JFYFY2) and INRCA, IRCCS to FC.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Belinda Giorgetti (Center for Neurobiology of Aging, INRCA, IRCCS) for technical assistance.

References

Ambrogi Lorenzini, C. G., Baldi, E., Bucherelli, C., Sacchetti, B., and Tassoni, G. (1997). Role of ventral hippocampus in acquisition, consolidation and retrieval of rat’s passive avoidance response memory trace. Brain Res. 768, 242–248. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00651-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannerman, D. M., Sprengel, R., Sanderson, D. J., McHugh, S. B., Rawlins, J. N., Monyer, H., et al. (2014). Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 181–192. doi: 10.1038/nrn3677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bano, D., Agostini, M., Melino, G., and Nicotera, P. (2011). Ageing, neuronal connectivity and brain disorders: an unsolved ripple effect. Mol. Neurobiol. 43, 124–130. doi: 10.1007/s12035-011-8164-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaudet, G., Bouet, V., Jozet-Alves, C., Schumann-Bard, P., Dauphin, F., Paizanis, E., et al. (2014). Spatial memory deficit across aging: current insights of the role of 5-HT7 receptors. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:448. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettio, L. E. B., Rajendran, L., and Gil-Mohapel, J. (2017). The effects of aging in the hippocampus and cognitive decline. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 79, 66–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, N. A., Lu, T., and Yankner, B. A. (2010). Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature 464, 529–535. doi: 10.1038/nature08983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bragina, L., Melone, M., Fattorini, G., Torres-Ramos, M., Vallejo-Illarramendi, A., Matute, C., et al. (2006). GLT-1 down-regulation induced by clozapine in rat frontal cortex is associated with synaptophysin up-regulation. J. Neurochem. 99, 134–141. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04030.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, S. N., and Barnes, C. A. (2006). Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 30–40. doi: 10.1038/nrn1809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, K. M., McDaniel, M. A., Dornburg, C. C., Price, A. L., and Roedinger, H. L. III. (2004). Age differences in veridical and false recall are not inevitable: the role of frontal lobe function. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 921–925. doi: 10.3758/bf03196722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, L., Jiang, W., Wang, F., Yang, Q. G., Wang, C., Chen, Y. P., et al. (2013). The reduced serum free triiodothyronine and increased dorsal hippocampal SNAP-25 and Munc18–1 had existed in middle-aged CD-1 mice with mild spatial cognitive impairment. Brain Res. 1540, 9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G. H., Wang, Y. J., Qin, S., Yang, Q. G., Zhou, J. N., and Liu, R. Y. (2007). Age-related spatial cognitive impairment is correlated with increase of synaptotagmin 1 in dorsal hippocampus in SAMP8 mice. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 611–618. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Danbolt, N. C., Pines, G., and Kanner, B. I. (1990). Purification and reconstitution of the sodium- and potassium-coupled glutamate transport glycoprotein from rat brain. Biochemistry 29, 6734–6740. doi: 10.1021/bi00480a025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, R. A., and De Frias, C. M. (2014). Cognitively elite, cognitively normal, and cognitively impaired aging: neurocognitive status and stability moderate memory performance. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 36, 418–430. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.903901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and Dong, H. W. (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65, 7–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautam, V., D’Avanzo, C., Berezovska, O., Tanzi, R. E., and Kovacs, D. M. (2015). Synaptotagmins interact with APP and promote Aβ generation. Mol. Neurodegener. 10:31. doi: 10.1186/s13024-015-0028-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izquierdo, I., Furini, C. R., and Myskiw, J. C. (2016). Fear memory. Physiol. Rev. 96, 695–750. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00018.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjelstrup, K. G., Tuvnes, F. A., Steffenach, H. A., Murison, R., Moser, E. I., and Moser, M. B. (2002). Reduced fear expression after lesions of the ventral hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 99, 10825–10830. doi: 10.1073/pnas.152112399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuzuya, A., Zoltowska, K. M., Post, K. L., Arimon, M., Li, X., Svirsky, S., et al. (2016). Identification of the novel activity-driven interaction between synaptotagmin 1 and presenilin 1 links calcium, synapse, and amyloid β. BMC Biol. 14:25. doi: 10.1186/s12915-016-0248-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leffa, D. D., Damiani, A. P., Damazio, D. D., Guerra, N. P., Moretti, M., de Brito, G. G., et al. (2014). Long-term effects of ageing and ovariectomy on aversive and recognition memory and DNA damage in the hippocampus of female rats. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 26, 161–169. doi: 10.1017/neu.2013.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzini, C. A., Baldi, E., Bucherelli, C., Sacchetti, B., and Tassoni, G. (1996). Role of dorsal hippocampus in acquisition, consolidation and retrieval of rat’s passive avoidance response: a tetrodotoxin functional inactivation study. Brain Res. 730, 32–39. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(96)00427-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovatel, G. A., Elsner, V. R., Bertoldi, K., Vanzella, C., Moysés Fdos, S., Vizuete, A., et al. (2013). Treadmill exercise induces age-related changes in aversive memory, neuroinflammatory and epigenetic processes in the rat hippocampus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 101, 94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcotulli, D., Fattorini, G., Bragina, L., Perugini, J., and Conti, F. (2017). Levetiracetam affects differentially presynaptic proteins in rat cerebral cortex. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 11:389. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, R. J., and Gobbi, G. (2012). Cannabinoids and emotionality: a neuroanatomical perspective. Neuroscience 204, 134–144. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.07.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moser, M. B., Moser, E. I., Forrest, E., Andersen, P., and Morris, R. G. (1995). Spatial learning with a minislab in the dorsal hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 92, 9697–9701. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.21.9697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolle, M. M., Gallagher, M., and McKinney, M. (1999). No loss of synaptic proteins in the hippocampus of aged, behaviorally impaired rats. Neurobiol. Aging 20, 343–348.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hadden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., and Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 17, 299–320. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Platano, D., Fattoretti, P., Balietti, M., Giorgetti, B., Casoli, T., Di Stefano, G., et al. (2008). Synaptic remodeling in hippocampal CA1 region of aged rats correlates with better memory performance in passive avoidance test. Rejuvenation Res. 11, 341–348. doi: 10.1089/rej.2008.0725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pudas, S., Persson, J., Nilsson, L. G., and Nyberg, L. (2014). Midlife memory ability accounts for brain activity differences in healthy aging. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2495–2503. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2003). Memory aging from 18 to 80. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 17, 162–167. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200307000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2012). Are individual differences in rates of aging greater at older ages? Neurobiol. Aging 33, 2373–2381. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimohama, S., Fujimoto, S., Sumida, Y., Akagawa, K., Shirao, T., Matsuoka, Y., et al. (1998). Differential expression of rat brain synaptic proteins in development and aging. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 251, 394–398. doi: 10.1006/bbrc.1998.9577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanguilder, H. D., Farley, J. A., Yan, H., Van Kirk, C. A., Mitschelen, M., Sonntag, W. E., et al. (2011). Hippocampal dysregulation of synaptic plasticity-associated proteins with age-related cognitive decline. Neurobiol. Dis. 43, 201–212. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanguilder, H. D., and Freeman, W. M. (2011). The hippocampal neuroproteome with aging and cognitive decline: past progress and future directions. Front. Aging Neurosci. 3:8. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vorhees, C. V., and Williams, M. T. (2006). Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat. Protoc. 1, 848–858. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vorhees, C. V., and Williams, M. T. (2014). Value of water mazes for assessing spatial and egocentric learning and memory in rodent basic research and regulatory studies. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 45, 75–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2014.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G.-W., Liu, J., and Wang, X.-Q. (2017). Post-training reversible disconnection of the ventral hippocampal-basolateral amygdaloid circuits impairs consolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory in rats. Learn. Mem. 24, 602–606. doi: 10.1101/lm.044701.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: brain aging, cognitive decline, hippocampus, synaptic proteins, synaptotagmin 1, Morris water maze test, step-through passive avoidance test

Citation: Balietti M, Fattorini G, Pugliese A, Marcotulli D, Bragina L and Conti F (2018) Two Behavioral Tests Allow a Better Correlation Between Cognitive Function and Expression of Synaptic Proteins. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:91. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00091

Received: 27 December 2017; Accepted: 19 March 2018;
Published: 04 April 2018.

Edited by:

Nibaldo C. Inestrosa, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Chile

Reviewed by:

Mikhail Yu. Stepanichev, Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology (RAS), Russia
Luigia Trabace, University of Foggia, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Balietti, Fattorini, Pugliese, Marcotulli, Bragina and Conti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giorgia Fattorini, g.fattorini@univpm.it

Download