Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Physical activity on executive function in sedentary individuals: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials

Abstract

Physical activity has been demonstrated to promote cognitive performance. However, the relationship between physical activity and executive function (EF) in sedentary individuals is not fully understood. This meta-analysis examined the impact of physical activity on EF in sedentary individuals and evaluated potential moderators of the relationship between physical activity and EF. In accordance with the PRISMA guidelines, the electronic databases MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO and Web of Science were searched. Included studies had to report sedentary individuals randomized to either a physical activity group or a control group. Subgroup analyses of EF sub-domains, exercise prescription and age were conducted alongside the overall meta-analysis. Thirteen RCT studies were included, with a total of 752 participants. Results showed a small to moderate beneficial effect of physical activity on EF (SMD = 0.24, 95% CI 0.08 to 0.40). In subgroup analysis by EF sub-domains, physical activity enhanced inhibitory control (SMD = 0.38, 95% CI 0.12 to 0.63) and working memory (SMD = 0.22, 95% CI -0.05 to 0.49), but not cognitive flexibility (SMD = 0.11, 95% CI -0.18 to 0.41). Interventions with an intervention length > 12 weeks improved overall EF (SMD = 0.26, 95% CI 0.06 to 0.46), but intervention length ≤ 12 weeks did not (SMD = 0.20, 95% CI -0.08 to 0.47). Interventions with session time ≥ 45 minutes improved overall EF (SMD = 0.47, 95% CI 0.22 to 0.77), but session time < 45 minutes did not (0.17, 95% CI -0.11 to 0.44). Physical activity improves EF for older adults (age ≥ 60 years) (SMD = 0.25, 95% CI 0.08 to 0.42), but not for younger individuals (age < 60 years) (SMD = 0.17, 95% CI -0.25 to 0.59). Overall, physical activity has a beneficial effect on EF in sedentary individuals, although the influence may be domain specific and influenced by exercise prescription and age. These findings have practical implications for those seeking to improve EF in sedentary individuals through physical activity.

Introduction

Executive function (EF) is a top-down, higher-order cognitive functions that are responsible for reasoning, planning, regulating, controlling information processes and behaviors. Core EFs consist of inhibitory control, working memory and cognitive flexibility. The capacity to suppress strong or inappropriate impulses from within or from the outside is referred to as inhibitory control. Working memory is the ability to store information in the mind for short periods of time and is essential for reasoning and problem solving. Cognitive flexibility represents the ability to adjust flexibly to changing needs or priorities [1]. EF is relevant to multiple aspects of life, including physical and mental health as well as social, cognitive and psychological development, and can predict quality of daily life and health [2]. A systematic review included 12 cross-sectional studies that investigated the relationship between sedentary behavior and EF [3]. The studies concluded that sedentary behavior was associated with poorer EF across the lifespan. In addition, physical activity is not only good for physical health but also produces positive effects on EF [4,5]. However, to our knowledge, there is currently no systematic review that examines the effects of physical activity on brain health using improvements EF sub-domains in sedentary individuals as a research endpoint.

Sedentary behavior (SB) and physical activity are two separate but related lifestyles that take up all the waking hours of a day. Sedentary behavior was defined as any form of waking activity that involves sitting or lying down and results in energy expenditure of less than 1.5 metabolic equivalents (METs) [6]. Accumulating evidence showed that high levels of sedentary behavior are linked to an increased risk of chronic conditions, such as obesity [7], type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular disease [8,9], and mental illness [10]. Despite this, nearly 67% of adults worldwide are still sedentary for more than 8.5 hours during waking hours [11]. Physical activity is characterized as any movement of the body that elevates energy expenditure beyond that at rest [12]. Being more active and less sedentary is essential for maintaining a healthy lifestyle because of its beneficial effects on skeletal [13], cardiovascular [7] and metabolic levels [14]. A previous meta-analysis study have found small effect of physical activity on inhibitory control in old sedentary individuals [15]. Notably, this study only included specific exercise prescriptions, cognitive components (i.e., inhibitory control) and age groups (i.e., older adults) [15]. However, research has found that physical activity characteristics (duration, frequency and intensity) and different cognitive components contribute to differences in the impact of physical activity on people’s cognitive performance. In addition, characteristics of participants, such as age, physical fitness and cognitive ability, were also found to affect the relationship [16].

Individual demographic characteristics may be a moderator of the effect of physical activity on EF in sedentary individuals [17]. For example, age may be an important moderating factor due to differences in the maturation of the prefrontal cortex at different ages [18]. Studies have found that EF is usually associated with the healthy functioning of the prefrontal cortex [19]. In addition, the results of the meta-analysis showed that older adults could achieve greater EF benefits after physical activity compared to other adult groups [20]. This finding suggests greater adaptive reserve in older people experiencing significant cognitive recession. However, the generalizability of these findings to sedentary individuals is yet to be established. In addition to individual characteristics, the relationship between physical activity and EF in sedentary individuals may also be influenced by exercise prescriptions, including session time, intervention length and exercise frequency [21,22]. Due to the dose-response relationship, the volume of physical activity may be another important moderator [23]. Therefore, the benefits of EF may vary depending on the session time. Experimental studies have shown that 20 minutes of physical activity have a greater impact on EF than 45 minutes and 10 minutes [24]. However, some studies report still inconsistent results in the performance of EFs in sedentary individuals after 40 minutes of physical activity [25,26]. Last but not least, EF sub-domains may play an important moderating role, each with a different degree of sensitivity to physical activity [20]. Observational studies have shown that physical activity in individuals is positively correlated with EF, with particularly pronounced effects on inhibitory control and working memory [27]. However, some studies report inconclusive evidence regarding the cognitive benefits of physical activity in healthy populations. For instance, Verburgh et al. (2014) [18] found no significant impact of physical activity on cognitive function in cognitively healthy older adults. Additionally, Singh et al. (2019) [28] revealed insufficient evidence to support the notion that physical activity improves cognitive performance in children.

In summary, there is currently limited evidence on the effects of physical activity interventions on the cognitive performance of sedentary individuals. Therefore, the purpose of this meta-analysis review was to examine the effects of physical activity on EF in sedentary individuals. Furthermore, we evaluated its potential moderation by examining relevant moderators. Specifically, determine the moderating effects of EF sub-domains (i.e., inhibitory control, working memory and cognitive flexibility), exercise prescription (i.e., intervention length, exercise frequency and session time) and individual characteristics (i.e., age) on physical activity to improve EF in sedentary individuals.

Materials and methods

The methodology employed in this meta-analysis was carried out in strict accordance with the guidelines established by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) [29] as well as the Cochrane Collaboration Handbook Statemen [30]. This review was prospectively registered with PROSPERO (registration number CRD42023433776).

Literature search strategy

Sample two review authors (S.D. and Z.D.) conducted electronic article searches in the Web of Science, PsycINFO, Embase and MEDLINE databases between January 2000 and January 2023. The titles and abstracts of the relevant studies were carefully reviewed by two separate authors, with any discrepancies resolved through mediation by a third author (X.L.) until consensus was reached. All possible search terms (from MeSH) contained in each search string are concatenated using the Boolean operators “AND” and “OR”. The combination of four sets of search terms were used to locate the studies, respectively the physical activity terms (“physical activity (PA)” OR “aerobic exercise” OR “chronic exercise” OR “exercise” OR “training”) AND sedentary behavior terms (“sedentary behavior” OR “sedentary lifestyle” OR “screen time” OR “sitting time”) AND EF terms (“executive function” OR “executive control” OR “cognition” OR “working memory” OR “inhibitory control” OR “cognitive flexibility” OR “cognitive function”) AND selected terms regarding study designs (“randomized controlled trial”). To ensure the thoroughness of this study, a comprehensive search of the Web of Science was conducted to include any potentially eligible studies from previous meta-analyses. All database specific search queries are available in S1 Appendix.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria were established for the selection of studies in this review: (1) Studies to be included in the review must be randomized controlled trials (RCT). (2) The study must focus on the effects of physical activity interventions on EF in sedentary individuals. (3) Participants must be sedentary or inactive healthy individuals. (4) Articles examining the relationship between physical activity and/or sedentary behavior and outcomes in at least one sub-domain of EF (inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) must be reported. (5) Studies must report the results of pre- and post-intervention testing of EF. (6) All included studies are peer-reviewed published articles written in English. Regarding the exclusion criteria, we did not include conference abstracts in the study because their quality reporting was not feasible [31]. Secondly, studies with multiple behavioral health interventions (e.g., diet programs or sleep programs combined with physical activity) were excluded because no independent effects of physical activity on EF could be obtained for sedentary behavioral health individuals. The eligibility criteria for the relevant studies are shown in Table 1.

Data extraction

Two authors performed independent extraction of study background information (first name of author, year of publication, study site), sample characteristics, physical activity protocol, and EF sub-domains of eligible studies. To calculate the pre-post intervention differences, we followed the guidelines outlined in the Cochrane Collaboration Handbook, using Mean and Standard Deviation (SD) values (SD difference = SQRT (SD pre 2 + SD post 2)– 2 * CRC * SD pre* SD post, Mean difference = Mean post−Mean pre), with a correlation coefficient (CRC) of 0.5 [30].

Risk of bias assessment

Two review authors independently assessed the methodological quality and risk of bias of included studies using the Cochrane Collaboration Risk of Bias tool [30] and the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guidelines [32]. The Cochrane Collaboration guidelines identified seven domains, including random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of participants, blinding of outcome assessments, incomplete outcome data, selective outcome reporting, and other sources of bias. The potential risk of bias in each domain was assessed as “low risk”, “high risk”, or “unclear bias”. In instances where there were discrepancies between the two authors conducting the quality evaluation, a third author (F.H.) was consulted to facilitate resolution and ensure consensus. The quality assessment data are summarized in S1 Fig in S1 Appendix.

Statistical analysis

Considering the variations in the outcomes and indicators of EF measures utilized across the included studies, standardized mean differences (SMD) were calculated by means of the pre- and post-intervention means, and weights were given by their inverse variance. The magnitude of the effect sizes was assessed using Cohen’s d values, with SMD values of < 0.2, 0.2 ≤ SMD < 0.5, and 0.5 ≤ SMD < 0.8 indicating small, moderate, and large effect sizes, respectively [30]. Quantitative pooled analysis based on a fixed-effects model, and a random-effects model if heterogeneity exists. To assess the degree of statistical heterogeneity in the meta-analysis, we employed the I2 statistic and p-value. Specifically, I2 values of 25%, 50%, and 75% were used to indicate mild, moderate, and high levels of heterogeneity, respectively [33]. Sensitivity analyses was performed to identify the sources of heterogeneity, and we utilized the literature-by-deletion method to perform a sensitivity analysis. Potential bias in the study is assessed using an adjusted funnel plot, which can be visualized and used to detect the presence of any dominant type of potential bias, such as publication bias, selective reporting, or other bias. We also assessed publication bias with the Egger bias test [34].

Following a meta-analysis of overall EF, a series of subgroup analyses were conducted in order to explain any observed heterogeneity or to explore potential moderating effects. Our variables of interest included EF sub-domains (inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility), exercise frequency (≤ 2 times/week or ≥ 3 times/week), intervention length (≤ 12 weeks or > 12 weeks), session time (≤ 45 min or > 45 min), and age (young-old/ ≤ 60 years or > 60 years). All the above analysis sequences were performed in STATA version 15.1 (Stata, Corp, College Station, Texas, USA).

Results

An initial search of 1267 peer-reviewed articles were conducted, of which two were from published meta-analyses and 1265 were obtained through a search strategy search. Duplicate articles were removed and filtered by title to get 378 articles. After careful examination of these abstracts, 33 articles were deemed eligible for in-depth review. Finally, 20 of these 33 articles were excluded because they did not have a control group, did not test EF, did not have data on EF measured before and after, did not have effect size data, and did not have a physical activity intervention (see Fig 1).

Characteristics of included studies

To summarize, the meta-analysis included 13 studies, the characteristics of which are detailed in Table 2. The total sample included 752 participants aged from 14 to 75 years. Specifically, the average age of participants in five studies was less than 40 years and in 10 studies the average age was greater than 60 years. In terms of the sub-domains of EF, nine studies evaluated inhibitory control, seven evaluated working memory, and seven evaluated cognitive flexibility. With regards to the geographic location of the included studies, 6 were conducted in the Americas, 6 in Europe, and 1 in Oceania. With regards to the exercise intervention protocols, only 4 studies had interventions lasting longer than 45 minutes. Ten studies had more than three exercise sessions per week, and eight studies had exercise interventions lasting longer than 12 weeks. The primary modifying factors considered in the study were age, session time, exercise frequency, exercise length and sub-domains of EF.

Results indicate that a significant proportion of the included studies (more than 50%) had high or unclear risks of bias with regards to allocation concealment, blinding of participants and personnel and other biases had low quality (S2 Fig in S1 Appendix).

Overall analysis and sensitivity analysis

As depicted in Fig 2, the results of the meta-analysis demonstrate a small but statistically significant positive standardized mean difference in overall EF (SMD = 0.24, 95% CI 0.08 to 0.40) with medium heterogeneity (I2 = 41.7%, p<0.05). The sensitivity analysis revealed that one study was the primary source of elevated heterogeneity [37]. Exclusion of this study resulted in a reduction of heterogeneity (I2 = 21.8%, p = 0.181), with a pooled SMD of 0.17 (95% CI 0.03 to 0.31) for overall EF measures. Results from Egger’s test and a visual assessment of the funnel plot did not provide significant indications of publication bias in the current study (p = 0.387). The funnel plot is shown in S3 Fig in S1 Appendix.

Subgroup analysis

Subgroup analyses revealed that relationship between physical activity and EF was significantly moderated by the sub-domains of EF. The results indicated that physical activity had the largest effect on inhibitory control (SMD = 0.38, 95% CI 0.12 to 0.63, I2 = 23.0%, p = 0.238) in sedentary individuals, followed by working memory (SMD = 0.22, 95% CI -0.05 to 0.49, I2 = 42.8%, p = 0.105) and cognitive flexibility (SMD = 0.11, 95% CI -0.18 to 0.41, I2 = 50.2%, p = 0.061) (S4 Fig in S1 Appendix). Furthermore, the impact of physical activity on EF was found to be significantly influenced by intervention length, session time, and age, but not by the frequency of exercise. As illustrated by Table 3, the SMD for intervention length > 12 weeks was 0.26 (95% CI 0.06 to 0.46) indicating medium heterogeneity (I2 = 41.4%, p = 0.047), whereas the SMD for intervention length ≤ 12 weeks was 0.20 (95% CI -0.08 to 0.47), with medium heterogeneity (I2 = 43.4%, p = 0.089) (S5 Fig in S1 Appendix). With respect to the exercise frequency, the SMD for exercise frequency ≥ 3 times/week was 0.24 (95% CI 0.04 to 0.43) with medium heterogeneity (I2 = 45.0%, p = 0.026), and the SMD for < 3 times/week was 0.25 (95% CI -0.05 to 0.52) with medium heterogeneity (I2 = 31.9%, p = 0.184) (S6 Fig in S1 Appendix). With respect to the session time, the SMD for session time > 45 min was 0.47 (95% CI 0.22 to 0.77) with small heterogeneity (I2 = 22.8%, p = 0.256), whereas the SMD for ≤ 45 min was 0.17 (95% CI -0.11 to 0.44) with medium heterogeneity (I2 = 38.0%, p = 0.115) (S7 Fig in S1 Appendix). With respect to the age, the SMD for participants older than 60 years was 0.25 (95% CI 0.08 to 0.42), with low heterogeneity (I2 = 0%, p = 0.628), whereas the SMD for participants 60 years and younger was 0.17 (95% CI -0.25to 0.59), with medium heterogeneity (I2 = 46.3%, p = 0.012) (S8 Fig in S1 Appendix).

Discussion

The primary purpose of this meta-analysis was to investigate the influence of physical activity on executive function in sedentary individuals. Specifically, this meta-analysis investigated the key moderating variables of physical activity on sedentary individuals, including executive function domain, intervention length, session time, frequency, and age. In the 13 studies included in the meta-analysis, the overall effect revealed a small to moderate benefit of physical activity on executive function in sedentary individuals compared to controls (SMD = 0.24). The current study is the first meta-analysis to explore the effects of a physical activity intervention on executive function in sedentary individuals, with a particular focus on key moderating variables that may influence subsequent cognitive outcomes, such as the EF subdomain and exercise prescription.

EF Sub‑Domains as a moderator

Although previous studies have shown that physical activity has a favorable influence on EF in sedentary individuals [15]. This meta-analysis offers a valuable contribution to the existing literature by examining the effect of physical activity on sub-domains of EF. The results of the subgroup analysis showed that sedentary individuals who participated in the physical activity showed small to moderate significant improvements in inhibitory control (SMD = 0.38) and working memory (SMD = 0.22), but did not cognitive flexibility (SMD = 0.11). In terms of inhibitory control, our study is consistent with the findings of a recent meta-analysis review investigating the effects of physical activity on inhibitory control in sedentary older adults. A small to moderate positive effect of physical activity on inhibitory control in sedentary individuals was observed [15]. These findings highlight the potential benefits of physical activity in improving inhibitory control in sedentary individuals and emphasize the importance of promoting physical activity in this population. Previous literature suggests that physical activity exhibits a particularly strong association with inhibitory control, as compared to the other two core EFs, in children and adolescents [46,47]. Moreover, our findings also provide evidence that physical activity interventions for sedentary individuals yielded favorable outcomes in terms of enhancing working memory. However, consistent with the results of the present study, previous empirical studies likewise failed to find any effect of physical activity on cognitive flexibility [48]. The results overall suggest that physical activity has a positive effect on two dimensions of EF in sedentary individuals. This supports the potential utility of physical activity interventions as a non-pharmacological approach to improve specific domains of EF in sedentary individuals.

Exercise prescription variable as a moderator

This meta-analysis review further assessed the important moderating effect of another set of moderating factors (e.g., exercise prescription) on physical activity measures on EF in sedentary individuals. The findings indicated that intervention length is a moderating factor in the effects of EF, with greater benefits associated with longer duration (>12 weeks) physical activity (SMD = 0.26) compared to shorter duration (≤12 weeks) physical activity (SMD = 0.20). This result is similar to the findings of a review by Yan et al. (2022) [49], which highlighted that interventions lasting over 12 weeks were more effective in improving cognitive outcomes in sedentary older adults compared to shorter interventions. These consistent observations reinforce the notion that longer physical activity interventions may be more beneficial for cognitive function in this population. In addition, Colcombe and Kramer (2003) [50] found that studies with intervention durations longer than 6 months had moderate effect sizes that were greater than those for interventions of moderate and short duration. The reason for this may be that prolonged regular physical activity is associated with greater grey matter volume in the prefrontal cortex [51] and increased functional connectivity of the frontal parietal network compared to shorter periods of physical activity [52], which may be associated with greater improvements in EF.

In addition to intervention length, session time was also recognized as moderators in our review. Our results suggest that interventions with >45 minutes of physical activity (SMD = 0.50) have a positive effect on overall EF, while interventions with ≤45 minutes (SMD = 0.17) have no significant effect on EF. This is similar to a previous meta-analysis which showed that >45 minutes of physical activity improved EF [17]. However, the effect of physical activity session time remains controversial. A meta-analysis by Xue et al (2019) [47] showed that physical activity had no significant effect on overall EF when session time was >90 minutes, while physical activity improved overall EF when session time was ≤90 minutes. It should be noted that even participants with a high level of fitness would be anticipated to experience declines in cognitive performance after prolonged physical activity (i.e., 2 h), possibly due to central fatigue caused by heat stress, dehydration, or hypoglycemia [53,54], or with adverse effects on information processing [55]. Interestingly, our review is splitting studies with session time less than 90 minutes into >45 minutes and ≤45 minutes. The duration of physical activity is a key aspect of creating an effective intervention. Our study further extends the understanding of the positive impact of intervention duration and has important implications for future intervention designs targeting sedentary populations.

Age as a moderator

The results of this review revealed significant differences in improvement of EF after physical activity between individuals with different, and these differences emphasize that physical activity is a beneficial strategy for EF in sedentary older adults (SMD = 0.25). This is consistent with a previous meta-analysis which demonstrated that physical activity can improve EF in sedentary older adults [15]. The reason for this is that normal ageing usually leads to atrophy of the whole brain, changes in functional response of brain, and cognitive ability decline [56]. Therefore, due to their greater cognitive reserve, older people are better able to benefit from physical activity than those in an optimal functional state [20]. Physical activity is associated with larger brain gray matter volumes in brain regions including the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in older adults [57], and retains white matter integrity, decreased severity of white matter lesions, and improved white matter microstructure [58]. When studies were conducted with children and adolescents, greater benefits were observed among younger participants. Greater benefits were observed in younger participants. This suggests that physical activity may have a stronger and more lasting effect on general cognitive skills during a sensitive period [59]. In addition, previous meta-analyses have found that physical activity has a positive impact on executive function and academic performance in prepubescent children [60]. However, these findings should be interpreted to be cautious, although the current meta-analysis suggests age-related variations in the influence of physical activity on EF in sedentary individuals, there are limited studies with prepubescent children and adolescents to determine. Whether the impact of physical activity is consistent across different developmental stages, including prepubescent children, adolescents, young adults, and older adults, remains to be determined.

Strengths and limitations

The exclusion of observational studies and the inclusion of only RCT studies is a strength of this meta-analysis. This stringent inclusion criterion enhances the reliability of causal inferences drawn from the study. This systematic review has additional strengths, including the examination of crucial moderators, such as sub-domains of EF, exercise prescription variables, and sample age. Compared to previous reviews, the data in this systematic review allow us to understand the correlation between physical activity and EF in sedentary individuals at a more refined level.

A limitation of the current review was that the included studies had limited control for potential sources of bias and there was no consistent reporting of the extent to which these risks were controlled for, for example, education, cognitive performance at baseline and BMI. Therefore, it is necessary to exercise caution when interpreting the results of the meta-analysis. Another limitation is the insufficient number of studies that include prepubescent children, adolescents and young adults. Therefore, further studies are necessary to establish the effect of physical activity on EF in sedentary individuals of different age groups. Moreover, the third limitation lies in the utilization of gain scores, which overlooks between-group baseline differences. In future research, we intend to employ multilevel models to account for the correlated structure of effects, avoiding mere data averaging. Additionally, we aim to reduce heterogeneity in study outcomes by introducing moderating variables into the model [61]. A fourth limitation is that we did not include exercise type and intensity as moderators. Including them as moderators would have significantly reduced the number of studies that could be included in certain analyses, potentially compromising the robustness of our findings. We prioritized maintaining a more comprehensive data set over introducing these additional moderators. Future research could consider these factors with larger datasets or in more targeted analyses.

Conclusions

This study is the first meta-analysis to investigate the effects of physical activity on the EF subdomain in sedentary individuals. In addition, we examined how moderating variables, such as exercise prescription variables and age, influenced these relationships. Our findings suggest that physical activity is a potentially effective strategy for improving EF in sedentary individuals, with improvements in inhibitory control and working memory. Based on the findings of this review, it is recommended that future intervention sessions should not exceed 45 minutes, and the exercise length needs to last more than 12 weeks, as this has been shown to have the greatest benefit for executive function in sedentary individuals. Whether the frequency of exercise was 1–2 or 3–4 times per week, physical activity had a positive effect on executive function and there was no significant difference between the two frequencies. Sedentary older individuals appeared to benefit more from physical activity. These results may be used as the foundation for suggestions on how to encourage sedentary people to engage in physical exercise on a regular basis. These could consequently be interesting to healthcare providers.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Extended results on physical activity to improve executive function in sedentary individuals.

It includes a search strategy for literature organization, an assessment framework for evaluating the quality of the literature (S1 and S2 Figs), funnel plots (S3 Fig) and subgroup forest plots for assessing publication bias (S4-S8 Figs), and PRISMA Checklist.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294251.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all authors of studies included in this systematic review and meta-analysis.

References

  1. 1. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–68. pmid:23020641
  2. 2. Blair C. Developmental Science and Executive Function. Current directions in psychological science. 2016;25(1):3–7. pmid:26985139
  3. 3. Li S, Guo J, Zheng K, Shi M, Huang T. Is Sedentary Behavior Associated With Executive Function in Children and Adolescents? A Systematic Review. Front Public Health. 2022;10:832845. pmid:35186852
  4. 4. Ludyga S, Gerber M, Pühse U, Looser VN, Kamijo K. Systematic review and meta-analysis investigating moderators of long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals. Nature human behaviour. 2020;4(6):603–12. pmid:32231280
  5. 5. Tian S, Mou H, Qiu F. Sustained Effects of High-Intensity Interval Exercise and Moderate-Intensity Continuous Exercise on Inhibitory Control. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2021;18(5). pmid:33800004
  6. 6. Pate RR, O’Neill JR, Lobelo F. The evolving definition of "sedentary". Exercise and sport sciences reviews. 2008;36(4):173–8. pmid:18815485
  7. 7. Lavie CJ, Ozemek C, Carbone S, Katzmarzyk PT, Blair SN. Sedentary Behavior, Exercise, and Cardiovascular Health. Circ Res. 2019;124(5):799–815. pmid:30817262
  8. 8. Dempsey PC, Biddle SJH, Buman MP, Chastin S, Ekelund U, Friedenreich CM, et al. New global guidelines on sedentary behaviour and health for adults: broadening the behavioural targets. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2020;17(1). pmid:33239026
  9. 9. Katzmarzyk PT, Powell KE, Jakicic JM, Troian RP, Piercy K, Tennant B, et al. Sedentary Behavior and Health: Update from the 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2019;51(6):1227–41. pmid:31095080
  10. 10. Vancampfort D, Firth J, Schuch FB, Rosenbaum S, Mugisha J, Hallgren M, et al. Sedentary behavior and physical activity levels in people with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder: a global systematic review and meta-analysis. World Psychiatry. 2017;16(3):308–15. pmid:28941119
  11. 11. Harvey JA, Chastin SF, Skelton DA. Prevalence of sedentary behavior in older adults: a systematic review. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2013;10(12):6645–61. pmid:24317382
  12. 12. Caspersen CJ, Powell KE, Christenson GM. Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public health reports (Washington, DC: 1974). 1985;100(2):126–31. pmid:3920711
  13. 13. Ubaida-Mohien C, Gonzalez-Freire M, Lyashkov A, Moaddel R, Chia CW, Simonsick EM, et al. Physical Activity Associated Proteomics of Skeletal Muscle: Being Physically Active in Daily Life May Protect Skeletal Muscle From Aging. Frontiers in physiology. 2019;10. pmid:30971946
  14. 14. Janssen I, LeBlanc AG. Systematic review of the health benefits of physical activity and fitness in school-aged children and youth. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7. pmid:20459784
  15. 15. Hoffmann CM, Petrov ME, Lee RE. Aerobic physical activity to improve memory and executive function in sedentary adults without cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Preventive medicine reports. 2021;23:101496. pmid:34377632
  16. 16. Williams RA, Hatch L., & Cooper S. B. A review of factors affecting the acute exercise-cognition relationship in children and adolescents. Integrative and Complementary Medicine. 2019;4:1–24.
  17. 17. Chen FT, Etnier JL, Chan KH, Chiu PK, Hung TM, Chang YK. Effects of Exercise Training Interventions on Executive Function in Older Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sports medicine (Auckland, NZ). 2020;50(8):1451–67.
  18. 18. Verburgh L, Königs M, Scherder EJ, Oosterlaan J. Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2014;48(12):973–9. pmid:23467962
  19. 19. Friedman NP, Robbins TW. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47(1):72–89. pmid:34408280
  20. 20. Ludyga S, Gerber M, Brand S, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Pühse U. Acute effects of moderate aerobic exercise on specific aspects of executive function in different age and fitness groups: A meta-analysis. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(11):1611–26. pmid:27556572
  21. 21. Álvarez-Bueno C, Pesce C, Cavero-Redondo I, Sánchez-López M, Pardo-Guijarro MJ, Martínez-Vizcaíno V. Association of physical activity with cognition, metacognition and academic performance in children and adolescents: a protocol for systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2016;6(6):e011065. pmid:27354073
  22. 22. van der Fels IM, Te Wierike SC, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4–16 year old typically developing children: A systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2015;18(6):697–703. pmid:25311901
  23. 23. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Research. 2012;1453:87–101. pmid:22480735
  24. 24. Chang YK, Chen FT, Kuan G, Wei GX, Chu CH, Yan J, et al. Effects of Acute Exercise Duration on the Inhibition Aspect of Executive Function in Late Middle-Aged Adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2019;11:227. pmid:31551753
  25. 25. Chapman SB, Aslan S, Spence JS, Defina LF, Keebler MW, Didehbani N, et al. Shorter term aerobic exercise improves brain, cognition, and cardiovascular fitness in aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013;5:75. pmid:24282403
  26. 26. Leckie RL, Oberlin LE, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Szabo-Reed A, Chaddock-Heyman L, et al. BDNF mediates improvements in executive function following a 1-year exercise intervention. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:985. pmid:25566019
  27. 27. Cox EP, O’Dwyer N, Cook R, Vetter M, Cheng HL, Rooney K, et al. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: A systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(8):616–28. pmid:26552574
  28. 28. Singh AS, Saliasi E, van den Berg V, Uijtdewilligen L, de Groot RHM, Jolles J, et al. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive and academic performance in children and adolescents: a novel combination of a systematic review and recommendations from an expert panel. Br J Sports Med. 2019;53(10):640–7. pmid:30061304
  29. 29. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JP, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2009;339:b2700. pmid:19622552
  30. 30. Cumpston M, Li TJ, Page MJ, Chandler J, Welch VA, Higgins JPT, et al. Updated guidance for trusted systematic reviews: a new edition of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2019(10). pmid:31643080
  31. 31. Kho ME, Eva KW, Cook DJ, Brouwers MC. The Completeness of Reporting (CORE) index identifies important deficiencies in observational study conference abstracts. Journal of clinical epidemiology. 2008;61(12):1241–9.e2. pmid:18599268
  32. 32. von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. International journal of surgery (London, England). 2014;12(12):1495–9. pmid:25046131
  33. 33. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in medicine. 2002;21(11):1539–58. pmid:12111919
  34. 34. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 1997;315(7109):629–34. pmid:9310563
  35. 35. Albinet CT, Boucard G, Bouquet CA, Audiffren M. Increased heart rate variability and executive performance after aerobic training in the elderly. European journal of applied physiology. 2010;109(4):617–24. pmid:20186426
  36. 36. Albinet CT, Abou-Dest A, André N, Audiffren M. Executive functions improvement following a 5-month aquaerobics program in older adults: Role of cardiac vagal control in inhibition performance. Biological psychology. 2016;115:69–77. pmid:26812613
  37. 37. Antunes HK, Santos-Galduroz RF, De Aquino Lemos V, Bueno OF, Rzezak P, de Santana MG, et al. The influence of physical exercise and leisure activity on neuropsychological functioning in older adults. Age (Dordrecht, Netherlands). 2015;37(4):9815. pmid:26169946
  38. 38. Bliss ES, Wong RHX, Howe PRC, Mills DE. The Effects of Aerobic Exercise Training on Cerebrovascular and Cognitive Function in Sedentary, Obese, Older Adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:892343. pmid:35663579
  39. 39. Edwards MK, Loprinzi PD. Effects of a Sedentary Intervention on Cognitive Function. American journal of health promotion: AJHP. 2018;32(3):595–605.
  40. 40. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):3017–22. pmid:21282661
  41. 41. Gothe NP, Keswani RK, McAuley E. Yoga practice improves executive function by attenuating stress levels. Biological psychology. 2016;121(Pt A):109–16. pmid:27794449
  42. 42. Prehn K, Lesemann A, Krey G, Witte AV, Köbe T, Grittner U, et al. Using resting-state fMRI to assess the effect of aerobic exercise on functional connectivity of the DLPFC in older overweight adults. Brain and cognition. 2019;131:34–44. pmid:28844505
  43. 43. Schwartz B, Kapellusch JM, Baca A, Wessner B. Medium-term effects of a two-desk sit/stand workstation on cognitive performance and workload for healthy people performing sedentary work: a secondary analysis of a randomised controlled trial. Ergonomics. 2019;62(6):794–810. pmid:30762479
  44. 44. Torbeyns T, de Geus B, Bailey S, Decroix L, Van Cutsem J, De Pauw K, et al. Bike Desks in the Classroom: Energy Expenditure, Physical Health, Cognitive Performance, Brain Functioning, and Academic Performance. Journal of physical activity & health. 2017;14(6):429–39. pmid:28169561
  45. 45. Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Alves H, Chaddock L, et al. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Human brain mapping. 2013;34(11):2972–85. pmid:22674729
  46. 46. Álvarez-Bueno C, Pesce C, Cavero-Redondo I, Sánchez-López M, Martínez-Hortelano JA, Martínez-Vizcaíno V. The Effect of Physical Activity Interventions on Children’s Cognition and Metacognition: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2017;56(9):729–38.
  47. 47. Xue Y, Yang Y, Huang T. Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2019;53(22):1397–404. pmid:30737201
  48. 48. Coles K, Tomporowski PD. Effects of acute exercise on executive processing, short-term and long-term memory. J Sports Sci. 2008;26(3):333–44. pmid:18074301
  49. 49. Ishihara T, Drollette ES, Ludyga S, Hillman CH, Kamijo K. The effects of acute aerobic exercise on executive function: A systematic review and meta-analysis of individual participant data. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;128:258–69. pmid:34147558
  50. 50. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(2):125–30. pmid:12661673
  51. 51. Erickson KI, Leckie RL, Weinstein AM. Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiology of aging. 2014;35:S20–S8. pmid:24952993
  52. 52. Voss MW, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Basak C, Chaddock L, Kim JS, et al. Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2.
  53. 53. Wittbrodt MT, Sawka MN, Mizelle JC, Wheaton LA, Millard-Stafford ML. Exercise-heat stress with and without water replacement alters brain structures and impairs visuomotor performance. Physiol Rep. 2018;6(16). pmid:30136401
  54. 54. Lieberman HR. Hydration and cognition: a critical review and recommendations for future research. Journal of the American College of Nutrition. 2007;26(5 Suppl):555s–61s. pmid:17921465
  55. 55. Grego F, Vallier JM, Collardeau M, Bermon S, Ferrari P, Candito M, et al. Effects of long duration exercise on cognitive function, blood glucose, and counterregulatory hormones in male cyclists. Neurosci Lett. 2004;364(2):76–80. pmid:15196681
  56. 56. Boraxbekk CJ, Salami A, Wåhlin A, Nyberg L. Physical activity over a decade modifies age-related decline in perfusion, gray matter volume, and functional connectivity of the posterior default-mode network-A multimodal approach. Neuroimage. 2016;131:133–41. pmid:26702778
  57. 57. Erickson KI, Leckie RL, Weinstein AM. Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiology of aging. 2014;35 Suppl 2:S20–8. pmid:24952993
  58. 58. Tian Q, Glynn NW, Erickson KI, Aizenstein HJ, Simonsick EM, Yaffe K, et al. Objective measures of physical activity, white matter integrity and cognitive status in adults over age 80. Behav Brain Res. 2015;284:51–7. pmid:25655514
  59. 59. Rafael RC, Daniels M, Luis F C. Let’s go beyond “the effect of”: Reappraising the impact of ordinary activities on cognition. Psicologica,. 2023;44(1): e15144.
  60. 60. de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Oosterlaan J, Visscher C, Hartman E. Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport. 2018;21(5):501–7. pmid:29054748
  61. 61. Ciria LF, Román-Caballero R, Vadillo MA, Holgado D, Luque-Casado A, Perakakis P, et al. An umbrella review of randomized control trials on the effects of physical exercise on cognition. Nature human behaviour. 2023;7(6):928–41. pmid:36973359