Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Seasonal variation in the balance and strength of cooperative and competitive behavior in patches of blue mussels

  • Jacob J. Capelle ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    jacob.capelle@wur.nl

    Affiliation Wageningen University & Research -Wageningen Marine Research, Yerseke, The Netherlands

  • Eva Hartog,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation HZ University of Applied Sciences, Vlissingen, The Netherlands

  • Tony Wilkes,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Wageningen University & Research -Wageningen Marine Research, Yerseke, The Netherlands

  • Tjeerd J. Bouma

    Roles Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Netherlands Institute for Sea Research, Yerseke, The Netherlands, Faculty of Geosciences, Department of Physical Geography, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands

Abstract

Aggregation into groups may affect performance of individuals through the balance and strength of facilitative versus competitive interactions. We studied in situ how seasonal variation in abiotic environment affects this balance for blue mussels, a semi-sessile species. We hypothesize that seasonal variation in stresses and resources affects the strength of the interaction. We expected that, in benign conditions (here: high food availability, medium temperatures, low hydrodynamic stress), performance is dominated by growth and is better at low densities, while at adverse conditions (here: low food availability, low or high temperatures, high hydrodynamic stress), performance is dominated by survival and higher at high densities. Mussels were kept in shallow subtidal exclosures at 10 different densities for a one-month period. This exact procedure was repeated seven times at the same location within a one-year period. We measured development in mussel patch shape, performance, and environmental parameters. Environmental conditions for mussels were most benign in summer and most adverse in winter. Patches developed into less complex shapes at lower densities, but also after stronger hydrodynamic disturbances. Towards summer, mussels became more active, aggregation behavior increased, and interactions became more pronounced. Towards winter, mussels became less active: aggregation behavior and growth rates declined and at the lowest temperatures survival started to decrease with mussel density. Survival and growth (by proxy of mussel condition) were both density-dependent; however, contrary to our expectations we found positive interactions between density and survival at the most benign conditions in summer and negative interactions at the most adverse conditions in winter. In between the two seasons, the strength of the interactions increased towards summer and decreased towards winter following a bell-shaped pattern. This pattern might be explained by the environmental mediated aggregation behavior of the mussels. The obvious seasonal pattern in balance and strength of density-dependent interactions demonstrates that strength and direction of intra-specific interactions are both strongly affected by environmental context.

Introduction

Animals group together, from pairs up to large-scale aggregations, to gain potential communal benefits [1, 2], such as safety in numbers [3, 4], locating and defending food sources [5], thermoregulation [6, 7], defending offspring [8], and increasing immunoregulation [9, 10]. Group living may also have disadvantages for individuals within groups, when resources are limited and competition for food is high [11]. Therefore, the question of whether it is advantageous for an animal to join or to live in a group is highly context-dependent and may change as a function of environmental condition [12]. For most organisms, neither the communal benefits nor the competition strength is constant over different life stages and seasons, while conditions that are experienced in one period can have carry-over effects for the organism into the following period [13]. In mobile species, environmental mediated behavior is a strong determinant for the dynamics of group formation [14, 15], and densities may vary rapidly over time [16, 17]. In contrast, sedentary or semi-sedentary species have a limited ability to change group size at best, which means it is vital to optimize the density for facilitation and minimize competition.

Numerous studies have been designed to investigate effects of environmental conditions on the balance between competition and cooperation, since this is recognized as an important contributor to ecological theory [18, 19]. The stress gradient hypothesis (SGH) predicts that the role of positive interactions will be greater at increasing levels of stress [20]. A number of studies have underlined the importance of this hypothesis, but also stressed the conditional effect of environment and species [2124]. Surprising few studies have addressed temporal effects, even though various parameters that contribute to environmental variation, are strongly affected by season [25]. We address this topic using blue mussels (Mytilus edulis) in a shallow subtidal environment as the model system.

Blue mussels are ideal model organisms in that they are semi-sedentary species that typically live and are adapted to dynamic and challenging environments. That is, the local environment may shift between benign and adverse, depending on the prevalent conditions that are often subjected to strong seasonal patterns. For mussels, benign can be defined as a situation with high food availability [26], medium temperatures [27], low turbidity [28], and low current and wave exposure [29], while adverse can be defined as the opposite to benign: low food availability, low or high temperatures, higher turbidity, and a higher exposure to current- and wave dynamics. Mussels show strong aggregation behavior, causing them to self-organize into spatial patterns. The dense patches that they form offer protection against dislodgement [30]. However, the formation of dense mussel patches can induce strong intra-specific competition [3133]. Even in very small patches e.g., 21–28 individuals per patch it was already observed that mussels that were located in the center had a reduced growth and reproductive output, compared to mussels located at the edges or in isolated groups [33]. The extent to which patches are being formed depends strongly on the overall mussel density [30, 34]. By looking at mussel patch dynamics, we study how seasonal variation in the abiotic environment affects the balance between cooperation and competition, and how this depends on the overall mussel density.

Aggregation behavior in mussels is density-dependent; sparse clumping occurs at low densities and dense clumping at high densities [35, 36]. Therefore, we expect that competition (for resources), and cooperation (resilience against threats) increases with clump size. We hypothesize that seasonal variation in stresses and resources affect the strength between competition and cooperation: in more benign conditions, performance will be dominated by growth and mussels are expected to perform better at low densities, while in more adverse conditions, performance will be dominated by survival and is expected to be higher at high densities. The present study takes an experimental approach by studying how seasonal variation in the abiotic environment affect the balance and strength between cooperation and competition in a (semi-)sessile species. The main abiotic parameters that affect mussel performance are chlorophyll-a (a proxy for food), turbidity (a proxy for food quality and a proxy for hydrodynamic stress [37]), and temperature. Mussel performance was measured in the absence of predation over a period of about one month for a range of mussel densities and this procedure was repeated several times throughout the year. Positive density interactions–or cooperation–was indicated when either survival or condition increased with mussel density, while negative density interactions–or competition–was indicated when either survival or condition decreased with mussel density. Fig 1 illustrates a conceptual diagram depicting the hypotheses and the methods employed to test them.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Conceptual drawing of hypotheses and methodology.

A. Hypothetical interactions in subtidal mussel patches; in summer interactions are expected to be dominated by growth due to high level of resources and temperature (benign conditions). In winter interactions are expected to be dominated by survival or mortality, because of the high level of environmental stress, low resources and high turbidity (adverse conditions). Note that temperature is not considered a stressor under the subtidal conditions at the experimental area. We anticipate that in the balance between competition and facilitation, competition will play a more significant role when growth is dominant, while facilitation will be more important when survival is dominant and B. Method to map density dependent interactions; to map density-dependent interactions, mussels will be placed in cages at 10 different densities (in triplicate) from low to high and performance was monitored over approximately one month, at seven different time periods within a year (M1-7, indicated by the grey bars in A.). Photos illustrate this development (at the highest density of 19 kg/m2) and the binary image shows the mussel cover used to calculate the perimeter-to-area ratio.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.g001

Material and methods

Mussels (Mytilus edulis) were kept in 10 different densities in predation-exclosures in the field for a period of about one month. This procedure was repeated seven times (runs) over a one-year period to cover all seasons (see Table 1). Relevant environmental parameters at the experimental site (that is, chlorophyl-a, turbidity, and temperature) were measured continuously for the duration of the experiments. Mussel growth, survival, and patch shape were calculated for each run.

thumbnail
Table 1. Mussel performance was evaluated in an experimental procedure that was repeated seven times over a one-year period.

This table shows when the experiments took place and summarizes the conditions, by displaying the average and standard deviation over the respective period (W0 = average mussel weight at start, Temp = temperature, Chl-a = chlorophyll-a, Turb = turbidity).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.t001

Experimental set up, mussel response, and environmental variables

The experiment was conducted at a shallow subtidal soft sediment area in the Oosterschelde estuary, The Netherlands (51°33’29.8"N 3°54’02.1"E), at a tidal amplitude of about 3.5 m. The area is in use as mussel lease site and with permission of the owner of the plot no permits were required for the fieldwork. At low tide, water height above the sediment was 0.5 m on average. The set-up consisted of 30 randomly placed cages (0.55 m x 0.60 m x 0.25 m) at least 2 meters apart, completely covered with a 25 mm mesh. The mesh was placed to exclude predators and enclose mussels. The bottom of the cage was buried approximately 10 cm into the sediment to avoid the mussels attaching to the cage, rather than living on a soft bottom. At the onset of each run, mussels were homogeneously placed in 10 different densities (1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 17 & 19 kg m-2 in an average density of 578, 1730, 2904, 4049, 5223, 6375, 7531, 8683, 9849 and 10981 mussels m-2, or 52, 156, 261, 364, 470, 574, 678, 781, 886, 988 mussels plot-1). We used a 0.3 x 0.3 m rectangle in the middle of each cage, to demark the area where mussels were added to create a buffer zone around the edges to prevent the mussels from attaching to the mesh. Each density was replicated three times. Densities were based on the range typically observed in the field, including the extremes [38], and were randomly assigned to the cages. Different mussels were used for each consecutive run. All mussels were collected at nearby locations and originated from seeded mussel culture plots in the same bay as where the experiment took place. Mussels were cleansed and sorted (in a range between 2 and 4 cm) prior to placement in the field.

Average wet weight, shell length, ash free dry weight (AFDW), and number of mussels were obtained from a subsample of all the mussels at the start of the experiment, to translate the starting weight of the mussels to number of individuals. The duration of each run was set for 30 days, but varied slightly because of weather and tide conditions (see Table 1). In previous studies, we found that the first month after relay is a critical period for survival and spatial organization is generally defined within one month [36, 38]. Growth rates during periods of low growth may be challenging to measure over a one month period. Therefore, we use mussel condition as a proxy for growth. Additionally, our previous research has shown a strong correlation between mussel meat weight and growth rate (see Fig 2 in [38]).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Environmental parameters measured at the experimental location, with daily averages calculated from 10-minute intervals.

A. Water temperature (°C), B. Chlorophyll A (μg/l) and C. turbidity (FTU). Colors indicate the environmental parameters during the consecutive experimental runs, with in 2017: 1 = Mar 10th–Apr 9th, 2 = Apr 18th–May 15th, 3 = Jun 1st-27th, 4 = Jul 27th–Aug 28th, 5 = Sep 20th–Oct 19th, 6 = Nov 9th–Dec 11th and in 2018: 7 = Feb 21st–Mar 19th.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.g002

At the end of an experimental run a top view photograph was taken from each cage. Mussels were collected and cleansed and total mussel mass was measured. Similar to the start of the experiment, from a homogenized subsample per cage wet weight, shell length, AFDW, and number of mussels were obtained.

A calibrated logger-type chlorophyll and turbidity meter (ACLW2-USB, JFE Advantech) measured chlorophyll-a (μg l-1), turbidity (FTU) and temperature (°C) in a 10-minute interval at the site during the entire experimental period (Fig 2). Chlorophyll-a is a reliable proxy for food quantity in the Oosterschelde [39]. In the analysis we did not consider effects of food quality.

Calculations

Mussel density at the start (D0) and at the end (D1) was calculated by extrapolating the density:weight relations from subsamples to the total weight per cage. The condition index (CI) was calculated with the AFDW (mg) and the shell length (cm) as: (1)

Condition index is used as a proxy for growth, because it is a more instantaneous measure than growth in weight or shell length over such a short time period. Top view photographs were analyzed in ImageJ [40]. Mussel-covered area was selected by hand, and mussels physically connected to each other were included in the same patch. The selection was saved as a binary image (see example in Fig 1). We placed a red piece of iron (15.3 cm) alongside the mussels when taking the picture as a size reference. Mussel covered area (mm2) and circumference (mm) were calculated from each picture and the perimeter-to-area ratio (PtoA) was defined as: (2)

Proportion of mussels that survived per cage was calculated for each run as: (3) where N0 and N1 are the number of mussels at the beginning and the end of an experiment, respectively. There were some occasions where N1 was slightly greater than N0. Since there was no mussel recruitment, this is considered an estimation error and the analysis whenever N1 > N0, N1 was replaced with the number from N0.

Statistical analysis

We corrected for differences in experimental duration assuming that the daily death rate within an experiment is constant throughout the experiment. The daily death rate was calculated by dividing the total number of deaths during an experiment by the duration of the experiment: (4)

The number of mussels that survived the experiment, out of the number of mussels that the experiment started with, was modeled with a quasi-binomial Generalized Linear Model (GLM) [41] using a logit link function (Model 1). With the assumption of a constant death rate, binomial failures, successes, and probabilities (p) are defined as follows: (5) (6) (7)

Using the assumption of a constant death rate, the daily death rate was multiplied by 22 (days) for mathematical consistency. New binomial successes and failures could become non-integer counts, but a quasi-binomial GLM can handle non-integer counts. The perimeter-to-area ratio (Model 2) and the Condition Index (Model 3) were both ratios and were modeled with linear models with log-transformed responses (see S1 File for details).

Models 2 used the main effects: D0, mean turbidity, mean chlorophyll-a, and mean temperature. No interaction terms were present for these models, as that resulted in multicollinearity. The quasi-binomial model (Model 1) had these same main effects, as well as pair-wise interactions between mean turbidity, mean chlorophyll-a and mean temperature. Model 3 (CI) used the main effects D0, mean turbidity, mean chlorophyll-a and temperature direction. Here, temperature direction is a categorical variable indicating how the temperature changed during the experiment. It has the following levels: “0” = the temperature was both high and stable. This is also the reference level, there was no “low and stable” level, as that never occurred during this study. “+” = the temperature increased from low to high; “-” = the temperature decreased from high to low. Temperature direction was used instead of mean temperature, as a model with mean temperature resulted in considerable non-linearity (correlogram of the data that was used for this model is attached as S3 Fig). No interaction terms were present for the final models as that resulted in multicollinearity. Pearson residuals were used to assess the model diagnostics for all linear and log-linear models. For the quasi-binomial GLMs, deviance residuals were used. The log-linear ratio models (models for the perimeter-to-area ratio and the Condition Index) were expected to have some heteroskedasticity. Therefore, robust standard errors, calculated using the “sandwich” R package [4244], were used on the log-linear ratio models to correct the effects that heteroskedasticity may have on the variances and thus p-values of the covariates. There were no clear violations of the model assumptions, and the fit of the models was reasonably good.

Holm’s correction for multiple comparisons [45] was applied to p-values of the coefficient significance tests to correct for multiple testing. Corrected p-values lower than 0.05 were considered significant. All statistical analysis were performed in R and RStudio [46, 47].

Results

Environmental characterization

Environmental patterns for Water temperature, chlorophyll-a, and turbidity over the period in which the consecutive experimental runs took place followed a typical annual trend for the region where the experiment was performed (Fig 2). Water temperature differences between summer and winter were about 20°C. Turbidity showed higher values from November to March, which is consistent with more storms causing upstirring of sediment in the fall-winter season. Chlorophyll-a increases sharply in spring due to the spring bloom that occurs in the North Sea-dominated region [48], then remains elevated over summer and decreases over autumn to reach lowest levels in winter. Because the mussels were continuously submerged no extreme temperatures were recorded, impact of heatwaves in the same region is limited to tidal flats [49].

Mussel survival

In line with our hypothesis, we found that overall an increase in density corresponds to an increase in the odds of survival if all other covariates remain the same (Table 2). The strength of the density effect was quantified using the slope (Msurvival) of the log-log relation between end-density (Dend) and starting-density (D0), with a slope below 1 indicating a negative density effect and a slope above 1 indicating a positive density effect. The slope for sequential experimental runs throughout the season shows a bell-shaped pattern (Fig 3) indicating the presence of a strong seasonal effect. Underlying plots are attached as supplementary material (S1 Fig).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Slope coefficient (Msurvival± SE, n = 30, a proxy for the strength of a density-dependent effect) of the log-log relation between end density and starting density for each consecutive experimental run: ln[D1] = Msurvival • ln[N0]+b.

Each run consisted of 10 different densities in triplicate. When the slope is below 1, a negative interaction between the individuals is expected (indication of competition), because the chance of survival decreases with mussel density. When the slope is above 1, positive interactions are expected (indication of cooperation) because the chance of survival increases with mussel density.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.g003

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary results of Model 1: Survival of mussels.

Dispersion parameter is 17.37, D0 = starting density, Temp = temperature (°C), Chl-a = chlorophyll a, Turb = turbidity, used as proxy for hydrodynamics stress, n.s. = non-significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.t002

Contrary to our expectations, survival chances were lower at higher temperatures and at higher chlorophyll-a values (Table 2). Also, the interaction effects between temperature with respectively chlorophyll-a and turbidity and between chlorophyll-a and turbidity were significant (Table 2). Fig 4 shows both the marginal and the interaction effects of the temperature, chlorophyll-a, and turbidity on survival chances. Chance of survival is higher when extremes in temperature and chlorophyll-a levels concur; that is, at the combination of low temperatures and low chlorophyll-a levels and at the combination of high temperatures and high chlorophyll-a levels. The latter combination refers to monitoring moment 4 (Fig 2) and although a strong density effect is observed at that moment (Fig 3) overall mortality was lower. Survival chance was lower at higher turbidity levels, which is a proxy for hydrodynamic stress, especially at higher temperatures and lower chlorophyll-a levels.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Marginal and interaction effects of the temperature, chlorophyll-a, and turbidity on the odds of survival.

The color indicates the relative effect of the interaction terms on the linear predictions: more green means a higher survival chance (higher log-odds), more red means a lower survival chance (lower log-odds). More white means the relative effect is closer to 0 (neither increase nor decrease in log-odds).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.g004

Perimeter-to-area ratio

An increase in starting density corresponds to a decrease in perimeter-to-area ratio, which confirms our hypothesis (Table 3). This indicates that, at higher starting densities, mussels aggregated quickly into larger and more complex patches (Fig 5). Of all the environmental parameters, only turbidity had a positive effect on this ratio: a higher turbidity resulted in an increase in the perimeter-to-area ratio, indicating that mussels aggregated less into larger complex patches. Since turbidity is a proxy for hydrodynamic stress, our findings indicate that an increase in hydrodynamic stress, as occurs during storms driving turbidity, results in the breaking up of mussel patches.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Perimeter: Area ratio ± SE (n = 21) of the mussel patches at the end of the experimental run as a function starting density.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.g005

thumbnail
Table 3. Summary results of Model 2: Perimeter-to-Area ratio, D0 = starting density, Temp = temperature (°C), Chl-a = chlorophyll a, Turb = turbidity, used as proxy for hydrodynamics stress, n.s = non-significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.t003

Mussel condition

An increase in density corresponds to a decrease in the condition, as represented by the condition index (CI; Table 4). A stable and high temperature during the experiment (TempDirection = 0) corresponds to a higher condition index than an increasing or decreasing temperature. Experiments where the temperature decreased from high to low had a higher condition index than experiments where the temperature increased from low to high. Temperature direction is more an indication of season than it is of actual temperature (see also Fig 2). Turbidity also had a significant negative effect on the mussel condition (Table 4). The slope between CI and starting density indicates, for all significant cases, a negative density dependence of the condition index (S2 Fig, Fig 6). This relation means that mussels are less fit when growing at higher densities and is valid across the various periods of the year.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Slope coefficient MCI ± SE (n = 30) between the log-log relation between mussel condition index (mg/cm3) and starting biomass for the different consecutive runs, as ln[CI] = MCI • ln[D0]+b., each run consisted of 10 different densities (ns = not significant, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001).

Run 2 is not included due to a lack of data. At negative MCI values mussel conditions decreases with mussel density (negative interactions) indicating competition for food, at positive MCI values mussel condition increases with mussel density (positive interactions—not observed).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.g006

thumbnail
Table 4. Summary results of Model 3: Condition index (mg/cm3), D0 = starting density, Temp Dir = temperature (°C) direction (- = decreasing, 0 = stable, + = increasing), Chl-a = chlorophyll a, Turb = turbidity, used as proxy for hydrodynamics stress, n.s = non-significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.t004

Discussion

The density-dependent patterns in mussel growth, mussel survival, and mussel aggregation were affected by environmental context in that the strength of the density effect followed a seasonal pattern that mimics the environmental trend in temperature and chlorophyll-a (a proxy for the available food quantity). We expected that, at conditions considered benign for mussels, such as when food availability is high and at medium temperatures, a negative density effect would occur; that is, a high growth rate and some degree of competition. Surprisingly, although growth (mussel condition) was indeed higher at more benign conditions, survival increased with mussel density. At more adverse conditions for mussels that typically occur more frequent in winter, such as low food conditions, low temperatures, and a higher frequency of hydrodynamic stress, we observed at the lowest temperature that mussel survival decreased at higher densities.

Balance between competition and cooperation

Species will experience concurrent cooperation and competition throughout their development [50] and at different environmental conditions [51]. There is a body of empirical evidence that, in intra-specific interactions, the balance between cooperation and competition shifts from positive (cooperation) to negative (competition) when the environment shifts from benign to harsh (e.g., [1921, 23, 52, 53]. These findings seem to oppose the basics of the original stress gradient hypothesis (SGH), which predicts that cooperation will dominate under stressful environments and competition will dominate under benign environments [18, 20]. Ever since this theory was harnessed in the SGH, discussions have arisen regarding the conditional validity of this theory, particularly about the role of cooperation and several studies refined the SGH with the addition that cooperation is underestimated under benign conditions [22, 5456]. For instance, organisms are more sensitive to stress under more benign conditions, while at high stress levels species tend to be more dependent on their feedback mechanisms. Our study deals with typical semi-sessile ecosystem engineering species. Ecosystem engineers have evolved to ameliorate stress under harsh conditions and alleviate limiting abiotic and biotic stresses for a range of species [57]. At adverse conditions, feedback from the engineer, such as aggregation into patterns [58], promotes survival, although it also lowers production. When the environment becomes more benign, activity increases and, in our study, resulted in a behavior that increases mortality, due to density-dependent competition for food and density-dependent cooperation for survival, all at the same time.

Effects of intra-specific interactions on survival

Which factors might be responsible for this observed behavior? In order to test only for the effect of the abiotic environmental parameters, predation was excluded. The main abiotic environmental parameters that do affect bivalve population dynamics in the subtidal zone are change in temperature [59], food availability [60], sediment dynamics [61], and hydrodynamics [62]. All of those parameters were measured directly or by proxy. Apparently, competition for food reduced mussel condition, but it did not accelerate mussel mortality. In soft sediment habitats, mussels need each other to prevent burial, a risk that increases with mussel cover [63]. Observations during retrieving the mussels from the experiment suggested that, in the low densities, where the mussels were organized in very small patches, mussels were often covered with sediment. At higher densities, mussels crawled on top of each other or aggregated in an interconnected pattern that is elevated from/laying on top of the sediment (see also Fig 5). This hypothesis is strengthened by the finding that M. edulis mortality caused by burial increases with temperature [64], probably due to higher oxygen demands at higher temperatures and is therefore expected to be higher in summer.

Effect of environmental context on the cooperation and competition

Mussel condition declined with mussel density, and, at the same time, survival interacted positively with density, meaning that competition concurred with cooperation. Density also affected the spatial structure of the mussel patches; at lower densities, patches were more complex (as in a higher perimeter:area, see also [65]). Furthermore, higher turbidity related to more complex patches, an effect that is expected when patches are broken up by hydrodynamic disturbances, such as during storms.

Environmental patterns followed a normal annual trend for the region in which the experiment was performed. The pronounced effects of temperature on the balance between cooperation and competition are interesting parameters to consider in scenario studies on climate change. In our experiment, the temperature did not reach levels that can be considered stressful for the mussels; instead, it increased cooperation between mussels. At higher temperatures, intra-specific interactions might buffer mortality at higher densities. But what will happen when temperature becomes a stressor? Will the balance between competition and cooperation shift towards more competitiveness, as observed in our study where conditions become more adverse, in that case Fig 2 may become bimodal. For instance, in a recent study on Baltic mussels, density dependency was presented as an accelerator of reduced population growth under increasing temperature scenarios [66].

It would also be interesting to include inter-specific effects. For example, the two most common predators for subtidal mussels in the study area are starfish and shore crabs, both of which show temperature-dependent feeding rates [67, 68] and interact with the (density-mediated) level of aggregation of the prey [69, 70]. That leads to the hypothesis that the effects we found in our study might even become more pronounced when inter-specific effects are included.

Sessile ecosystem engineers are unable to move to benign environments and depend on their feedback to optimize environmental dependent growth and survival. The coastal environment is highly dynamic over time and the species have adapted to short time variation in the environment, such as tidal effects [71], but also to longer term variation, such as in seasonal changes [72]. When conditions are harsh for the species (such as in winter, when temperatures are low, food is scarce, and disturbances by storms are frequent), ecosystem engineers show low levels of activity and increase survival through their feedback mechanisms [29]. When conditions get better in spring, activity will increase and will peak during summer when the environment is most benign (that is, when there are high temperatures, medium food levels, and low disturbance probability). Our results show that a high level of activity increases the competition for food, but also increases behavioral cooperation. In our case this is most likely done by actively using conspecifics to maneuver to a beneficial position within the patch.

An important implication of our study is that it is vital to take the environmental context into account when studying and discussing density-dependent patterns and performance. The strength of the cooperation-competition balance changes over the season. We repeated the same set-up seven times over a year and would jump to very different conclusions from isolated experiments when performed in summer (density-dependent facilitation in survival), than when performed in winter (density-dependent competition in survival).

Supporting information

S1 File. Modelling ratios where numerator and denominator are correlated.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Log-log relation between end density and starting density per run.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.s002

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Log-log relation between Condition index and starting biomass per run.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.s003

(DOCX)

S3 Fig. Correlogram for data used in model 3.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293142.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the assistance of Niels Wagenaar, Jesse van der Pool, Loes de Jong, Rein Krielen, and Phiel de Koeijer, who carried out most of the field and lab work as part of their study at HZ University of Applied Sciences.

References

  1. 1. Jolles JW, King AJ, Killen SS. The Role of Individual Heterogeneity in Collective Animal Behaviour. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2020;35(3):278–91. pmid:31879039
  2. 2. Krause J, Ruxton GD, Ruxton G, Ruxton IG. Living in groups: Oxford University Press; 2002.
  3. 3. Turchin P, Kareiva P. Aggregation in Aphis varians: an effective strategy for reducing predation risk. Ecology. 1989;70(4):1008–16.
  4. 4. Connell SD. Is there safety‐in‐numbers for prey? Oikos. 2000;88(3):527–32.
  5. 5. Cocroft RB. Vibrational communication and the ecology of group-living, herbivorous insects. American Zoologist. 2001;41(5):1215–21.
  6. 6. Fletcher LE. Examining potential benefits of group living in a sawfly larva, Perga affinis. Behavioral Ecology. 2009;20(3):657–64.
  7. 7. Nicastro KR, Zardi GI, McQuaid CD, Pearson GA, Serrao EA. Love thy neighbour: group properties of gaping behaviour in mussel aggregations. PloS ONE 2012;7(10):e47382. pmid:23091620
  8. 8. Solomon NG, Crist TO. Estimates of reproductive success for group-living prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster, in high-density populations. Animal Behaviour. 2008;76(3):881–92.
  9. 9. Lombardo MP. Access to mutualistic endosymbiotic microbes: an underappreciated benefit of group living. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2008;62(4):479–97.
  10. 10. Ezenwa VO, Ghai RR, McKay AF, Williams AE. Group living and pathogen infection revisited. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2016;12:66–72.
  11. 11. Ford JR, Swearer SE. Two’s company, three’s a crowd: Food and shelter limitation outweigh the benefits of group living in a shoaling fish. Ecology. 2013;94(5):1069–77. pmid:23858647
  12. 12. Song C, Von Ahn S, Rohr RP, Saavedra S. Towards a probabilistic understanding about the context-dependency of species interactions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2020;35(5):384–96. pmid:32007296
  13. 13. Ratikainen II, Gill JA, Gunnarsson TG, Sutherland WJ, Kokko H. When density dependence is not instantaneous: theoretical developments and management implications. Ecology Letters. 2008;11(2):184–98. pmid:17979979
  14. 14. Paul MJ, Zucker I, Schwartz WJ. Tracking the seasons: the internal calendars of vertebrates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2008;363(1490):341–61. pmid:17686736
  15. 15. Pittendrigh CS. Temporal organization: reflections of a Darwinian clock-watcher. Annual Review of Physiology. 1993;55(1):17–54. pmid:8466172
  16. 16. Bonabeau E, Dagorn L, Freon P. Scaling in animal group-size distributions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1999;96(8):4472–7. pmid:10200286
  17. 17. Griesser M, Ma Q, Webber S, Bowgen K, Sumpter DJ. Understanding animal group-size distributions. PLOS ONE. 2011;6(8):e23438. pmid:21912596
  18. 18. Callaway RM, Walker LR. Competition and facilitation: A synthetic approach to interactions in plant communities. Ecology. 1997;78:1958–65.
  19. 19. Bertness MD, Grosholz E. Population dynamics of the ribbed mussel, Geukensia demissa: The costs and benefits of an aggregated distribution. Oecologia. 1985;67:192–204.
  20. 20. Bertness MD, Callaway R. Positive interactions in communities. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 1994;9:191–3. pmid:21236818
  21. 21. Maestre F, Valladares F, Reynolds J. Is the change of plant–plant interactions with abiotic stress predictable? A meta-analysis of field results in arid environments. Journal of Ecology. 2005;93(4):748–57.
  22. 22. Holmgren M, Scheffer M. Strong facilitation in mild environments: the stress gradient hypothesis revisited. Journal of Ecology. 2010;98(6):1269–75.
  23. 23. Meysick L, Ysebaert T, Jansson A, Montserrat F, Valanko S, Villnäs A, et al. Context-dependent community facilitation in seagrass meadows along a hydrodynamic stress gradient. Journal of Sea Research. 2019;150–151:8–23.
  24. 24. Barrio IC, Hik DS, Bueno CG, Cahill JF. Extending the stress-gradient hypothesis–is competition among animals less common in harsh environments? Oikos. 2013;122(4):516–23.
  25. 25. Gastaldi M, Firstater FN, Romero MA, Pereyra PJ, Narvarte MA. Seasonality dictates changes in the ecological interactions among spatial dominants. Marine Biology. 2020;167(12):176.
  26. 26. Bayne B, Newell R. Physiological energetics of marine molluscs. The mollusca: Academic Press; 1983. P. 407–515.
  27. 27. Widdows J. The effects of temperature on the metabolism and activity of Mytilus edulis. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research. 1973;7:387–98.
  28. 28. Loosanoff V. Effects of turbidity on some larval and adult bivalves. 1962. Proceedings of the Gulf and Caribbean Fisheries Institute 14th Annual Session.
  29. 29. Schotanus J, Capelle JJ, Paree E, Fivash GS, van de Koppel J, Bouma TJ. Restoring mussel beds in highly dynamic environments by lowering environmental stressors. Restoration Ecology. 2020;28(5):1124–34.
  30. 30. Bertolini C, Cornellissen B, Capelle JJ, van de Koppel J, Bouma TJ. Putting self‐organization to the test: labyrinthine patterns as optimal solution for persistence. Oikos. 2019;128: 1805–15.
  31. 31. Van de Koppel J, Rietkerk M, Dankers N, Herman PMJ. Scale-dependent feedback and regular spatial patterns in young mussel beds. The American naturalist. 2005;165:66–77. pmid:15729660
  32. 32. Gascoigne JC, Beadman HA, Saurel C, Kaiser MJ. Density dependence, spatial scale and patterning in sessile biota. Oecologia. 2005;145:371–81. pmid:15968539
  33. 33. Okamura B. Group living and the effects of spatial position in aggregations of Mytilus edulis. Oecologia. 1986;69:341–7.
  34. 34. Van De Koppel J, Gascoigne JC, Theraulaz G, Rietkerk M, Mooij WM, Herman PMJ. Experimental evidence for spatial self-organization and its emergent effects in mussel bed ecosystems. Science. 2008;322:739–42. pmid:18974353
  35. 35. Zardi GI, Nicastro KR, McQuaid CD, de Jager M, van de Koppel J, Seuront L. Density-dependent and species-specific effects on self-organization modulate the resistance of mussel bed ecosystems to hydrodynamic stress. The American Naturalist. 2021;197(5):615–23. pmid:33908830
  36. 36. Capelle JJ, Wijsman JWM, Schellekens T, van Stralen MR, Herman PMJ, Smaal AC. Spatial organisation and biomass development after relaying of mussel seed. Journal of Sea Research. 2014;85:395–403.
  37. 37. Fettweis M, Francken F, Van den Eynde D, Verwaest T, Janssens J, Van Lancker V. Storm influence on SPM concentrations in a coastal turbidity maximum area with high anthropogenic impact (southern North Sea). Continental Shelf Research. 2010;30(13):1417–27.
  38. 38. Capelle JJ, Wijsman JWM, Van Stralen MR, Herman PMJ, Smaal AC. Effect of seeding density on biomass production in mussel bottom culture. Journal of Sea Research. 2016;110:8–15.
  39. 39. Prins TC, Smaal AC, Pouwer AJ, Dankers N, 1996. Filtration and resuspension of particulate matter and phytoplankton on an intertidal mussel bed in the Oosterschelde estuary (SW Netherlands). Marine Ecology Progress Series. 142, 121–134.
  40. 40. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9(7):676–82. pmid:22743772
  41. 41. McCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized linear models. 2 ed. London: Chapman and Hall; 1989.
  42. 42. Zeileis A. Object-oriented computation of sandwich estimators. Journal of Statistical Software. 2006;16:1–16.
  43. 43. Zeileis A. Econometric computing with HC and HAC covariance matrix estimators. Vienna: Institut für Statistik und Mathematik, WU Vienna University of Economics and Business; 2004.
  44. 44. Zeileis A, Köll S, Graham N. Various versatile variances: An object-oriented implementation of clustered covariances in R. Journal of Statistical Software. 2020;95:1–36.
  45. 45. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics. 1979:65–70.
  46. 46. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org. 2020.
  47. 47. Rstudio T. Rstudio: integrated development for R. Rstudio Team, PBC, Boston, MA URL http://www.rstudio.com. 2020.
  48. 48. Philippart CJM, Van Iperen JM, Cadée GC, Zuur AF. Long-term field observations on seasonality in chlorophyll-a concentrations in a shallow coastal marine ecosystem, the Wadden Sea. Estuaries and Coasts. 2010;33:286–94.
  49. 49. Zhou Z, Bouma TJ, Fivash GS, Ysebaert T, van Ijzerloo L, van Dalen J, et al., 2022. Thermal stress affects bioturbators’ burrowing behavior: A mesocosm experiment on common cockles (Cerastoderma edule). Science of The Total Environment. 824, 153621.
  50. 50. Valiente-Banuet A, Verdú M. Temporal shifts from facilitation to competition occur between closely related taxa. Journal of Ecology. 2008;96(3):489–94.
  51. 51. Brooker RW, Callaghan TV. The Balance between Positive and Negative Plant Interactions and Its Relationship to Environmental Gradients: A Model. Oikos. 1998;81(1):196–207.
  52. 52. Lortie C, Callaway R. Meta-analysis and rejection of the stress-gradient hypothesis? Analytical recommendations. Journal of Ecology. 2006;94:7–16.
  53. 53. He Q, Bertness MD, Altieri AH. Global shifts towards positive species interactions with increasing environmental stress. Ecology Letters. 2013;16(5):695–706. pmid:23363430
  54. 54. Soliveres S, Smit C, Maestre FT. Moving forward on facilitation research: response to changing environments and effects on the diversity, functioning and evolution of plant communities. Biological Reviews. 2015;90(1):297–313. pmid:24774563
  55. 55. Scrosati RA. Community-level facilitation by macroalgal foundation species peaks at an intermediate level of environmental stress. Algae. 2017;32(1):41–6.
  56. 56. Takimoto G. Direct and Indirect Effects of Facilitation and Competition in Ecological Communities: Population Persistence, Coexistence, and Species Diversity Along the Stress Gradients. In: Mougi A, editor. Diversity of Functional Traits and Interactions: Perspectives on Community Dynamics. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2020. P. 81–108.
  57. 57. Crain CM, Bertness MD. Ecosystem Engineering across Environmental Gradients: Implications for Conservation and Management. BioScience. 2006;56(3):211–8.
  58. 58. Liu Q-X, Doelman A, Rottschäfer V, de Jager M, Herman PMJ, Rietkerk M, et al. Phase separation explains a new class of self-organized spatial patterns in ecological systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(29):11905–10. pmid:23818579
  59. 59. Widdows J, Bayne BL. Temperature acclimation of Mytilus edulis with reference to its energy budget. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom, 1971, 51, pp 827–843. 1971.
  60. 60. Smaal AC, Haas HA. Seston dynamics and food availability on mussel and cockle beds. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science. 1997;45(2):247–59.
  61. 61. Jordan-Cooley WC, Lipcius RN, Shaw LB, Shen J, Shi J. Bistability in a differential equation model of oyster reef height and sediment accumulation. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2011;289 (Supplement C):1–11. pmid:21871900
  62. 62. Zardi GI, Nicastro KR, McQuaid CD, Rius M, Porri F. Hydrodynamic stress and habitat partitioning between indigenous (Perna perna) and invasive (Mytilus galloprovincialis) mussels: constraints of an evolutionary strategy. Marine Biology. 2006;150(1):79–88.
  63. 63. Widdows J, Lucas JS, Brinsley MD, Salkeld PN, Staff FJ. Investigation of the effects of current velocity on mussel feeding and mussel bed stability using an annular flume. Helgoland Marine Research. 2002;56:3–12.
  64. 64. Hutchison ZL, Hendrick VJ, Burrows MT, Wilson B, Last KS. Buried Alive: The Behavioural Response of the Mussels, Modiolus modiolus and Mytilus edulis to Sudden Burial by Sediment. PLOS ONE. 2016;11(3):e0151471.
  65. 65. Bertolini C, Capelle JJ, ter Veld JWD, van de Koppel J, Bouma TJ. Understanding scales of density-dependence to improve the use of resources in benthic mussel aquaculture. Aquaculture. 2020;528:Article: 735477.
  66. 66. Jaatinen K, Westerbom M, Norkko A, Mustonen O, Koons DN. Detrimental impacts of climate change may be exacerbated by density-dependent population regulation in blue mussels. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2021;90(3):562–73. pmid:33073861
  67. 67. Agüera A, Trommelen M, Burrows F, Jansen JM, Schellekens T, Smaal A. Winter feeding activity of the common starfish (Asterias rubens L.): The role of temperature and shading. Journal of Sea Research. 2012;72:106–12.
  68. 68. Elner RW. The influence of temperature, sex and chela size in the foraging strategy of the shore crab, Carcinus maenas (L.). Marine & Freshwater Behaviour & Physiology. 1980;7(1):15–24.
  69. 69. Dolmer P. The interactions between bed structure of Mytilus edulis L. and the predator Asterias rubens L. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology. 1998;228:137–50.
  70. 70. Whitton TA, Jenkins SR, Richardson CA, Hiddink JG. Aggregated prey and predation rates: Juvenile shore crabs (Carcinus maenas) foraging on post-larval cockles (Cerastoderma edule). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology. 2012;432–433:29–36.
  71. 71. Bertness MD, Leonard GH. The role of positive interactions in communities: Lessons from intertidal habitats. Ecology. 1997;78:1976–89.
  72. 72. Widdows J. Combined effects of body size, food concentration and season on the physiology of Mytilus edulis. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom. 1978;58(01):109–24.