Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Susceptibility to movement-evoked pain following resistance exercise

Abstract

Objective

To investigate the: (1) role of basic muscle pain sensitivity and psychological factors in the prediction of movement-evoked pain (MEP) following delayed onset muscle soreness (DOMS), and (2) association of MEP with changes in systemic muscle pain sensitivity following DOMS induction.

Methods

Fifty-one participants were assigned to either eccentric resistance exercise or control groups. They completed questionnaires evaluating psychological distress and underwent muscle pain sensitivity evaluation by the pressure pain threshold (PPT) test at the exercised and remote muscles, before and 24 hours following the intervention. MEP intensity was determined in response to lifting a 3kg canister using a visual analogue scale (VAS).

Results

The exercise group demonstrated MEP intensity of 5/10 on VAS and reduced PPTs at the main exercised muscle (p<0.001). A regression tree analyses revealed that the level of anxiety trait predicted a higher MEP intensity. A secondary analysis showed that 53% participants who were DOMS responders (MEP > mild intensity; ≥ 3/10 VAS) exhibited decreased PPTs in the exercised (p<0.001) and remote (p = 0.027) muscles following eccentric exercise. Characterization of DOMS responders revealed that, at baseline, they had lower PPTs in the exercised (p = 0.004) and remote (p = 0.001) muscles and reported higher psychological distress i.e., anxiety trait and depression symptoms (p<0.05), compared to non-responders. A regression analysis revealed that lower PPT or high levels of anxiety trait increased the probability to become a responder (p = 0.001).

Conclusions

Susceptibility to MEP following DOMS is determined by muscle pain hypersensitivity and high levels of anxiety trait. MEP at the early stage of DOMS is linked with an increase in systemic muscle pain sensitivity suggestive of central mechanisms. This knowledge is valuable in translating science into clinical musculoskeletal pain management.

Introduction

Delayed onset muscle soreness (DOMS) is an acute muscle pain condition that reaches its peak at 24–72 hours, usually following eccentric contractions [14]. In order to evaluate DOMS intensity, previous studies used pain recalled through self-report questionnaires or spontaneous pain ratings. However, an assessment of pain in general, and not specifically in relation to evoked movement, does not evaluate the main clinical characteristic of DOMS, which is commonly referred to as movement-evoked pain (MEP). Because DOMS is often used as a model of acute muscle pain in experimental pain research in which the effects of interventions on muscle pain are tested [5], it is important to use an objective test such as MEP as an outcome [68].

Mechanisms implicated in MEP associated with musculoskeletal pains are multifactorial including peripheral and central mechanisms [7]. Yet to the best of our knowledge, only one study investigated MEP mechanisms following DOMS induction and it focused on brain-related activity [9]. MEP in the exercised muscle following DOMS induction is a type of mechanical allodynia i.e., pain evoked in response to movement only and not under normal conditions. In fact, large diameter (myelinated) afferents [10, 11] or C-tactile (CT) fibers [12] might underlie the mechanical allodynia observed in DOMS, presumably via centrally mediated mechanisms i.e., central sensitization. This central neuroplasticity accompanies the peripheral sensitization of nociceptive neurons [13, 14]. The latter is due to inflammatory processes caused by ultrastructural muscle damage in the exercised muscles [15, 16]. In detail, biopsy analysis of eccentric strained muscle tissue shows that there is a disruption of sarcomeres which leads to protein degradation and a local inflammatory response [2, 17]. Furthermore, a higher cytokine concentration also acts to mediate inflammation and increase vascular permeability [18]. In addition, the ultrastructural muscle damage is associated with increased cytosolic calcium concentrations, which activate proteolytic enzymes and contribute further to vascular permeability [19]. The accumulation of interstitial fluid is accompanied by intramuscular edema which is responsible for nociceptor activation and pain sensation. The poor correlation observed between perceived pain intensity and the degree of change of biological markers of muscle damage [2022] further supports the key role of central mechanisms in mechanical allodynia following DOMS induction. Furthermore, central mechanisms have been implicated in MEP in individuals with other musculoskeletal pains (e.g. knee osteoarthritis) as evident of its association with temporal summation [23, 24] and not with spontaneous pain [23]. Thus, one aim of this study was to understand whether MEP following DOMS induction is associated with potential central neuroplastic changes manifested as a reduction in systemic pain sensitivity i.e., a decrease in pressure pain thresholds (PPT) in remote areas from the exercised muscles.

In addition to the neurophysiological mechanisms, MEP also involves complex relationships and interactions with negative psychological factors (e.g., pain catastrophizing, fear-avoidance, depression) [6, 7]. These factors may contribute to inter-subject variability in the severity of DOMS. Indeed, DOMS intensity, not evaluated specifically in relation to MEP, varies from slight stiffness in the muscles that disappears rapidly with the carrying out of routine daily activities to severe debilitating pain that interferes with movement [2]. Therefore the severity of DOMS is substantially influenced by both individual differences in pain sensitivity [25, 26] and by psychological constructs [27, 28]. However, these factors have not been tested in relation to DOMS evaluated by the MEP test. Accordingly, this study focused on MEP as a procedure for DOMS assessment and aimed at investigating: 1) the predictive role of individual pain sensitivity and psychological constructs on MEP intensity, and 2) the association of DOMS-induced MEP with changes in systemic pain sensitivity.

Material and methods

Study population

Healthy sedentary individuals aged 18–40 years were recruited. Participants were excluded from the study if they 1) engaged in exercise or sporting activities more than once a week within the previous six months; 2) reported physiological, psychiatric, cognitive or neurological disorders, or any acute or chronic pain condition; 3) used medications on a regular basis; or 4) were pregnant. Participants were asked to avoid analgesic medication at least 24 hours prior to session 1 through to the end of session 2.

Study procedure

The study was approved by the Institutional Research Board of the University of Haifa (ethics approval # 070/16) and took place at the university’s Physical Therapy Laboratories. Potential study participants were screened via a telephone interview and then those that met the inclusion criteria were asked to attend two experimental sessions. In the first session participants gave their signed informed consent and were assigned to either the eccentric resistance exercise group or the control group based on stratified randomization using Microsoft Excel software and controlling for age and sex. Demographic and anthropometric information was collected, and participants underwent familiarization with the PPT test. Thereafter, they were asked to lift a 3 kg canister for the MEP evaluation and to rate the level of pain using the 10 cm visual analogue scale (VAS) scale in the McGill Pain Questionnaire-short form (SF-MPQ) [29]. Following this, all participants completed psychological questionnaires and the PPT tests. Finally, those allocated to the exercise group only underwent the eccentric exercise protocol for DOMS induction.

All participants returned to the laboratory 24 hours after the first session. The exercise group who had undergone the DOMS protocol filled out the DOMS-related interference questionnaire and then again underwent the MEP evaluation. Thereafter, all participants, regardless of their group assignment, completed the PPT tests in the same order as in the first session (Fig 1).

Eccentric exercise protocol

Participants underwent a brief familiarization of the eccentric exercise protocol where they were shown the correct way to carry out the exercises. Each participant then carried out the exercise protocol. Dumbbell weight was adjusted to each participant and for each exercise. Three unilateral [30, 31] exercises for right upper extremity was performed in a standing position: 1) biceps curl to target the biceps muscle; 2) shoulder flexion to target the anterior deltoid muscle; and 3) chest press to target the pectoralis major.

To determine the appropriate weight load for each participant, a 5-repetition maximum (5RM; i.e. the maximum weight lifted with 5 concentric contractions) strength test was conducted. Participants were asked to estimate their 5RM and this weight then served as the starting weight. The load was then adjusted until the participant could not complete more than 5 repetitions; this load then served as the 5RM load. This 5RM strength test was conducted for each of the three exercises. The trials carried out until finding each participant’s 5RM load were separated by 3-minute rest periods. Participants were asked to perform 10 sets of 5RM for each exercise [30] The exercise was performed while the investigator assisted the participant in lifting the load in the concentric phase. In order to emphasize eccentric contractions, participants were asked to control the eccentric phase repetition for approximately 10 seconds while attempting to maintain a constant velocity throughout the movement. One-minute rest breaks were taken between sets, and two-minute rest breaks were taken between exercises [32]. After each set, participants rated their perceived exertion on a scale between 6 "no effort " to 20 "maximal effort" [33].

DOMS intensity was determined in response to lifting a 3 kg canister (i.e. the MEP test) filled with sand [32] using the 10 cm VAS scale from the SF-MPQ [29]. This measure represents the pain evoked by a physical task performed at one point in time, 24 hours after conducting the eccentric exercise protocol. In detail, the canister was positioned on a table that its height was adjusted so that the handle of the canister was at standing elbow height. The participant stood erect with his or her elbow bent at 90 degrees and he was asked to lift the canister with arm fully extended.

DOMS intensity measures

DOMS-related interference questionnaire.

We used a modified version of the questionnaire used by Trost and colleagues [34] which refers to back pain. DOMS-related interference was assessed by asking the participant the following question: “Did the pain in your upper extremity prevent you from performing your usual activities in the past day?”. The participant was asked to provide their responses on a 1–4 Likert scale.

McGill Pain Questionnaire-short form (SF-MPQ).

MEP intensity was quantified using the SF-MPQ in response to lift a 3 kg load to the maximum reach prior (1st session) and 24 hours following (2nd session) DOMS protocol implementation. In the SF-MPQ three pain assessment tools are included: the pain rating index (PRI), the present pain intensity index (PPI) and a VAS [29]. The PRI contains a total of 15 descriptors, which are rated on a 4-point Likert scale for pain intensity with answers ranging from 0 (‘none’) to 3 (‘severe’). For each session, a pain score was derived by calculating the sum of the intensity rank values for all 15 descriptors. For the PPI, participants were asked to indicate the intensity of pain on a 6-point Likert scale, with answers ranging from 0 (‘no pain’) to 5 (‘excruciating pain’). The VAS was measured on a scale ranging from 0 to 10. The VAS scores were used for quantification of the MEP intensity.

Muscle pain sensitivity measurement.

We used the PPT test for evaluation of muscle pain sensitivity [35]. All participants completed a training session. This included familiarization with the PPT test in which three repetitions of the PPT test were applied to the exercised hand thenar eminence. The study PPTs were tested at the exercised muscles (i.e., right deltoid and biceps) and in a remote muscle (i.e., left forearm flexors) using a pressure algometer (Somedic Algometer, Sweden). At each of the three selected sites [right anterior deltoid (PPTd), right biceps (PPTb) and left flexor (PPTf)], 3 pressure stimuli were administered, with an increasing rate of 30 kPa/sec and an inter-stimulus interval of 30 sec. The participant was asked to push a button to indicate the point at which the pressure stimulus was perceived as minimally painful. PPT was computed as the mean of the 3 measures.

Pain-related psychological questionnaires

Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS).

The 13 item PCS [36] was used in order to assess catastrophic thinking related to past painful experiences. A 5-point Likert scale was used to rate their responses where 0 = ‘not at all’ to 4 = ‘all the time’. The PCS total score is computed by summing responses to all 13 items and may range from 0–52. The PCS has been shown to have high internal consistency (coefficient alpha = .87) and the Hebrew version has demonstrated good reliability (coefficient alpha = .86) [37].

Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI).

Anxiety level was assessed by Spielberger’s STAI using the validated Hebrew version [38]. Part I of the questionnaire assesses the level of state anxiety and Part II assesses the level of trait anxiety. Each part includes 20 statements which describe various emotional states. Participants were asked to rate each item based on a 4-point Likert scale (1–4), ranging from 1-‘not at all’ to 4-"very much so’ for the STAI state scale and from ‘almost never’ to ‘almost always’ for the STAI trait scale. Scores range from 20 to 80, with higher scores suggesting greater levels of anxiety. The STAI has good internal consistency (coefficient alpha >.89), and the STAI trait scale has evidenced good test–retest reliability (average r = .88) at multiple time intervals [39].

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI).

The BDI is a 21-item self-report measure of the severity of depressive symptoms [40]. Participants rated the severity of each symptom on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 to 3. A total score (range 0–63) was computed by summing individual items’ scores and was interpreted as follows: normal (0–9), light depression (10–18), mild depression (19–26). A total score above 26 is considered to be indicative of severe depression. The BDI has demonstrated good reliability and validity (coefficient alpha = .81) [41].

Perceived Stress Scale (PSS).

The PSS Hebrew version [42] is comprised of a 10-item self-report measure of the degree to which common life situations are evaluated as stressful. Participants were asked to respond to each situation (relating to the last month) on a 5-point Likert scale, with answers ranging from 0 (‘never’) to 4 (‘very often’). Higher scores on this measure indicate greater levels of perceived stress. The PSS has been shown to have high internal consistency (coefficient alpha = .84-.86) and a good test–retest reliability (average r = .85) [42].

Fear of Pain Questionnaire (FPQ) Short Form.

The FPQ [43] was used to measure pain-related fears. The FPQ-SF is a 20-item self-report inventory designed to assess fear of specific pain causing stimuli. Research supports the internal consistency (coefficient alpha = .91) and construct validity of the FPQ-SF [43].

Statistical analysis

A priori power analysis using the G*power computer program [44] indicated that a total sample size of 34 people would be needed to detect medium effects of η2p = 0.06 with 80% power and alpha at .05 using an ANOVA repeated measure, within between interactions.

Data are expressed as mean ± SD or median and interquartile range, as appropriate according to the normality distribution assessed by the Shapiro-Wilks test.

A two way 2 (group: exercise; control) * 2 (time: 1st session; 2nd session) mixed-model repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was run to examine the effect of group and time on changes in PPT, and the interaction between them. This was done separately for each PPT measure (PPTd, PPTb, and PPTf). A simple mean analysis was conducted to further explore differences between the groups and we used the post hoc Bonferroni Procedure for multiple comparisons (4 comparisons).

For a secondary exploratory analysis the participants in the exercise group were divided into responders and non-responders to DOMS-related MEP based on Mankovsky-Arnold et al.’s study [32]. We defined responders if MEP ratings at the beginning of sessions 2 was VAS≥3 (above mild pain). Those with VAS of 2 or lower were defined as "non-responders". In order to characterize responders vs. non-responders, we performed univariate analysis using unpaired t-tests or Mann-Whitney tests as appropriate for baseline anthropometric, demographic, pain-related psychological factors, and PPTs. To investigate changes in pain sensitivity following DOMS induction in relation to responsivity to MEP, we applied two-way mixed-design ANOVA; time (1st session; 2nd session) defined as the within-subjects factor, group (responders; non-responders; controls) as between-subjects factor. We tested the main effect of time, group, and the interaction between time and group. This was done separately for each PPT measure (PPTd, PPTb, and PPTf). A simple mean analysis was conducted to further explore differences between groups and we used the post hoc Bonferroni Procedure for multiple comparisons (6 comparisons).

We conducted decision and regression tree analyses with the R party package, using random forest variable selection [45] and Monte Carlo simulation for multiple-testing adjustment. These were carried out in order to predict the probability of MEP occurrence and its intensity and to explore interactions between potential predictors. The potential predictors were variables with associated p values of < 0.05 on univariate analysis including the PPTf, PPTb, PPTd anxiety trait, BDI, PSS, and sex. In order to control for dumbbell weight used during DOMS induction, individual muscle load for Biceps curl and Deltoid were added as a covariate.

Statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 for Windows and the R Foundation for Statistical Computing version 3.6.1. Statistical significance was taken as p <0.05.

Results

Participants

Fifty-one (23 males) healthy subjects were recruited to the study, aged 25.9±4.9 years. The exercise group included 32 participants (14 males). The control group included 19 participants (9 males). No significant differences were found for age (t42.2 = -0.37, p = 0.730), weight (t28.6 = 0.51, p = 0.609), or height (t32.9 = -0.75, p = 0.434) between the groups. Psychological measures were not significantly different between groups (Table 1). Also, no group differences were found at baseline (1st session, before DOMS induction) for PPTs tested at the forearm flexors (t25.3 = -0.68, p = 0.502); Biceps (t20.2 = -1.09, p = 0.290) and Deltoid (t19.5 = -1.07, p = 0.298) (PPTs values are presented in Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 1. Scores of psychological factors in the exercise and control group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. PPT’s values at baseline (1st session) and following 24 hours (2nd session).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.t002

Change in pain sensitivity following eccentric exercise

After 10 sets of 5RM participants could no longer control the descent of the weight and their rate of perceived exertion in each of the exercises reached the following averages: 19.4±0.8 out of 20 for the Biceps, 19.8±0.7 for the Deltoid, and 19.7±0.9 for the Pectoralis, evident of peak activity in the involved muscles.

The DOMS intensity measures 24 hours following DOMS induction in the exercise group were: median (Q1, Q3) for the PRI 4.5 (4.3, 10.3), the PPI 2.0 (1.4, 2.4), the MEP 3.0 (2.0, 3.8) and for DOMS interference 2.5 (2.3, 3.4). The PPTs values for the exercise and the control group at 1st and the 2nd session (24 hours following the intervention) are shown in Table 2. Main effect of time was statistically significant for the PPT tested at the Biceps (F (1, 37.3) = 13.39 p = 0.0024, ɳ2p = 0.012), and Forearm Flexors (F(1,32) = 8.41 p = 0.018, ɳ2p = 0.077) yet not for the Deltoid (F(1,31.8) = 5.39, p = 0.081, ɳ2p = 0.050). No main effect of group was found. Furthermore, no group and interaction effects were found for the PPTb and PPTf. Yet, a group by time interaction was found for PPT at the Deltoid (F(1,31.8) = 6.38 p = 0.048, ɳ2p = 0.060). A simple mean analysis revealed that only the exercise group demonstrated a significant increase in pain sensitivity following eccentric exercise as expressed by reduction in PPT (exercise diff = 73 t30 = 4.20, p<0.001, 95% CI(37.57, 108.82), dz = 0.65; controls diff = -3.0 t16 = -0.13, p = 0.90, 95% CI(-55.35,49.16), dz = 0.013).

Prediction of DOMS-related MEP intensity

The tree regression analysis to predict DOMS intensity (Fig 2) revealed a significant split in the anxiety trait scores. When the trait anxiety scores were >46 the mean pain intensity was 5.4 VAS (out of 10) regardless of sex (this was true for 7 participants). However, female participants with anxiety trait scores ≤46 showed a higher mean pain intensity than males with the same anxiety levels (females = 3.38; males = 1.25 VAS).

thumbnail
Fig 2. The tree regression analysis to predict DOMS intensity.

A regression tree analysis was set up to predict DOMS pain intensity by anxiety score below or above 46 and sex. The numbers in the circles indicate the level of statistical significance (P); and the numbers in the rectangular boxes reflect the total number of cases for that outcome (n) and the mean of pain intensity (y).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.g002

Secondary exploratory analysis (responder vs. non-responders)

We found that out of the 32 participants in the exercise group, 17 (53%) showed a DOMS above mild intensity (i.e. VAS≥3) in response to MEP and defined as responders (as explained on the Methods section). There were more females than males who were DOMS responders (14 [72%] females vs. 4 [29%] males; p = 0.014).

Table 3 summarizes the of DOMS intensities 24 hours after the DOMS administration. Significant differences were found between DOMS responders and non-responders in PRI (U (17.88) = -3.88, p<0.001), PPI (U (24.44) = -2.56, p = 0.002), MEP (U (26.1) = -7.24, p<0.0001), and DOMS interference (U (26.1) = -7.24, p<0.001).

thumbnail
Table 3. DOMS intensity in responders and non-responders.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.t003

A mixed model ANOVA (Table 4) revealed that the main effect of time (1st and 2nd session), was significant in each statistical model PPTd, PPTb, and PPTf (p = 0.003, p = 0.0002, and p = 0.0007, respectively). The main effect for group was also identified for the PPTf and PPTb (p = 0.0009 and p = 0.010, respectively). Furthermore, a significant interaction (time by group) was found only for PPTd (p = 0.004). A simple mean analysis, (after Bonferroni correction for 6 comparisons the p-value was set to <0.008) showed that only the responders demonstrated a significant decrease in PPT (responders: Mdiff = 101.80,t16 = 6.15, CI:(66.73,136.89), p<0.001, dz = 0.90; non-responders: Mdiff = 38.45,t13 = 1.23, CI:(-29.05,105.96), p = 0.24, dz = 0.34; Con: Mdiff = 3.09,t16 = 0.13, CI:(-55.35,49.16), p = 0.90, dz = 0.012) (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Significant interaction (time by group) for PPTd.

The measurement of pain pressure threshold (mean±standard error) on the Deltoid (PPTd) showed that responders (black solid line) demonstrated a significant decrease in PPTd following eccentric exercise. PPT differences between the sessions were 101kPa for responders; 63 kPa for non-responders (brake black line); and -3kPa for the control group who did not perform the exercise (grey solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.g003

thumbnail
Table 4. PPTs values for responders, non-responders and controls at baseline (1st session) and 24 hours following DOMS induction (2nd session).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.t004

At the remote area (contralateral forearm), we did not find an interaction between time and group for the PPTf. Because this was a secondary exploratory analysis in which we had a major interest in the determining whether DOMS responsivity specifically and individually resulted in change over time, we performed a simple mean analysis and found that only responders demonstrated a significant decrease in PPTf (responders: Mdiff = 60.47, t16 = 3.28, CI:(21.42,99.51),p = 0.024, d = 0.65; non-responders: Mdiff = 75.73, t14 = 2.58, CI:(12.86,136.61), p = 0.132, dz = 0.68; controls: Mdiff = 22.28, t18 = 0.85, CI:(-32.83,77.39), p = 0.410, dz = 0.10).

In order to characterize responders vs. non-responders to DOMS-related MEP we compared the pain sensitivity between the groups at baseline (See Table 4, 1st session) and found that responders demonstrated lower PPTs than non-responders for the PPTf and PPTb (p = 0.0001, d = 1.55 and p = 0.004 d = 1.12, respectively).

For further characterization of responders vs. non-responders, comparisons for the psychological questionnaires’ scores were performed between groups (Table 5). Results show that responders had higher scores for anxiety trait depression and stress symptoms.

thumbnail
Table 5. Baseline scores of psychological questionnaires for responders and non-responders to DOMS-related MEP.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.t005

Prediction of DOMS response

The tree decision analysis (Fig 4) revealed a significant split in the PPTf variable such that participants with a value ≤378kPa at baseline showed a higher probability to become a DOMS responder (this was true for n = 12 participants). Participants with a value >378kPa at baseline for PPTf and an anxiety score >46 also showed a higher probability to become a DOMS responder (this was true for n = 7 participants).

thumbnail
Fig 4. The tree decision analysis to predict DOMS responders.

Decision tree analysis set up to predict DOMS responders (VAS≥3) by PPTf (pressure pain threshold in the forearm), below or above 378kPA, and anxiety score below or above 46. The numbers in the circles reflect the level of statistical significance (P); numbers in the rectangular boxes reflect the total number of cases for that outcome (n) and the probability (y) to become a non-responder (first number) or DOMS–responder (second number).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271336.g004

Discussion

By using a protocol for DOMS induction and focusing on provoked MEP as an outcome measure, we found that among all the pain and psychological measures tested, the level of anxiety trait was the only measure that predicted a higher intensity of MEP, 24 hours following eccentric exercise. In addition, females with mild to moderate levels of trait anxiety were prone to develop greater MEP intensities than males, even after controlling for muscle load during the DOMS induction. However, only 50% of our cohort developed MEP at a mild intensity (i.e. ≥ 3/10 VAS; responders). In order to understand who is at high risk of developing MEP, we investigated the individual pain sensitivity profile and psychological characteristics of these responders. Our novel finding is that systemic muscle pain hypersensitivity (i.e. lower PPTs) and heightened psychological distress (i.e. trait anxiety) predicted the probability to become a responder. The responders also showed a significant increase in muscle pain sensitivity following DOMS induction mainly in the exercised muscle and to some extent in a remote area, suggesting peripheral and central neuroplasticity (i.e., central sensitization), respectively.

Of the many studies that have used DOMS protocols, only a few have referred to non-responders after an exercise protocol for pain induction and reported that ~15–20% of their cohort did not respond with muscle soreness [25, 46, 47]. However, these studies did not specify responders based on a standardized task that elicited MEP and referred only to pain recalled through self-report questionnaires without provocative movement. Assessment of pain in general and not specifically in relation to movement does not evaluate the main clinical characteristic of DOMS. Likewise, questionnaires that are based on individuals’ recall are unable to differentiate pain in relation to movement from other types of pain [7, 48]. In the current study we used mild pain intensity (3 and above on a 0–10 NPS) in response to the MEP test (i.e. lifting a 3kg canister) as the threshold criterion for DOMS responders based on Mankovsky-Arnold et al. [32]. This is an advantage over many other previous studies which did not have such a threshold criterion, meaning that even participants with an incredibly low DOMS intensity were included in their group analysis.

We found that in a healthy population not customized for upper limb eccentric exercise, those who develop MEP are characterized as having basic systemic deep somatic tissue hypersensitivity i.e., lower PPTs in both exercised and remote body muscles. Indeed, PPTs are considered to predominantly reflect the sensitivity of deeper structures [49]. Furthermore, the clinical application of the PPT test has been validated for evaluating pain sensitivity [50, 51] and to identify group differences in pain sensitivity [52, 53]. In the current study, we found that PPT also predicted the probability of MEP occurrence. This finding is in line with the findings of a few previous studies that investigated the predictive ability of PPT in muscular pain [5456]. Our finding that PPT did not predict DOMS intensity is in line with another study that showed that temporal summation of pain but not PPT predicted DOMS intensity recalled through self-report questionnaires [25]. Taken together, our results emphasize the role of muscular tissue sensitivity in the prediction of the occurrence of MEP following DOMS induction.

The findings of this study have highlighted the significant influence of trait anxiety on the tendency to develop MEP and its intensity. Trait anxiety is a constant future-oriented state characterized by negative affect and anticipation of an unpredictable threatening event. Previous reports suggest that pain-related fear predicts pain intensity [57], physical performance, and interference with life activities following DOMS induction [34, 58]. Furthermore, an interaction between the fear of pain and pain catastrophizing has been shown to predict experimental muscle pain (i.e. intramuscular acidic infusion) intensity and referred pain intensity [56]. Specifically to DOMS, the experience of multisite pain [31] and evoked pain intensity in remote body areas [30] were predicted by fear of pain and pain catastrophizing. Yet in these studies, anxiety trait was not tested as a predictor and the outcome measures were pain in remote areas. In our study, trait anxiety was shown to have a consistent and pronounced correlation to the presence of DOMS-related MEP and its intensity in the exercised muscles. This is in line with previous studies that concluded that not only does trait anxiety have an excitatory effect on pain and vice versa, it also contributes to the intensity of experienced pain [56, 59, 60]. To summarize, our finding that both lower PPTs and high anxiety levels predict MEP is supported by another study that found a cumulative impact of psychological and sensitization risk factors (i.e., high pressure sensitivity measured by PPT) on pain-related outcomes [61].

We found that responders showed significantly decreased PPTs (i.e., mechanical allodynia) in the exercised muscles. This is a result of peripheral sensitization of intramuscular nociceptors from algogenic substances released subsequent to muscle damage [15, 16, 62]. In addition, the inflammation that occurs in the region of damage may turn on silent nociceptive neurons that become activated in response to normally innocuous movement [63]; a mechanism that may explain MEP. Moreover, the responders also demonstrated some reduction in PPT in the non-exercised muscle, suggesting the development of central sensitization [14, 47, 6467]. Indeed, MEP is a clinical manifestation of mechanical allodynia that may also arise from central sensitization that causes altered processing of mechanoreceptor afferent activity [11]. Thus, the coupling between MEP and decreased PPTs in remote body areas suggests that they are phenotypes of central sensitization [13]. The development of central sensitization following DOMS induction is also supported by previous reports of increased temporal summation of pain stimuli. [64, 67, 68], increased frequency of referred pain, and enlarged pain areas [47]. Thus, we suggest that both peripheral and central mechanisms are at the base of above mild intensity MEP. Moreover, we carefully suggest that the MEP test may serve as a diagnostic tool of pain hypersensitivity due to central neural plasticity [14].

This study has several limitations that need be taken into consideration for future studies. We cannot rule out the possibility that DOMS occurred in some participants for whom the MEP task did not evoke pain of 3 or greater. In addition, DOMS is a progressive process which reaches a peak 24–72 hours after exercise regimen. In the current study we measured pain sensitivity only after 24 hours. It is possible that later measurements along the time course of DOMS development will shed more light on the pain neuroplasticity progress. Furthermore, the current findings are limited by the moderate sample size, thus validation of the present findings with larger sample sizes are needed. Finally, DOMS is a type of acute muscle pain that is evoked by movement and therefore is a specific clinical condition that exemplifies the reciprocal relationships of pain and movement. Thus, the conclusion of this study regarding the mechanisms associated with MEP cannot be separated from those of DOMS.

Conclusions

This is the first study to characterize DOMS responders and non-responders that were classified based on a provoked MEP test and to predict those who will develop DOMS following eccentric exercise. Our findings show that high anxiety trait increases the probability to develop DOMS-related MEP and together with greater systemic muscle pain sensitivity it increases MEP intensity. Furthermore, once DOMS is established at a higher than mild intensity level of MEP, it is accompanied by systemic muscle hypersensitivity, suggesting peripheral and central neuroplasticity.

This knowledge is valuable in translating science into clinical musculoskeletal pain management. DOMS is considered an experimental model for clinical musculoskeletal pain and the same factors that shape responses to experimental pain stimuli also contribute to the experience of clinical pain. Therefore, by applying the DOMS as an acute muscle pain model in a clinical trial, putative risk factors can be assessed in order to identify those who are vulnerable to DOMS-related MEP.

References

  1. 1. Cheung K, Hume P, Maxwell L. Delayed onset muscle soreness: treatment strategies and performance factors. Sports Med. 2003;33(2):145–64. Epub 2003/03/06. pmid:12617692.
  2. 2. Armstrong RB. Mechanisms of exercise-induced delayed onset muscular soreness: a brief review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1984;16(6):529–38. Epub 1984/12/01. pmid:6392811
  3. 3. Armstrong RB. Initial events in exercise-induced muscular injury. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1990;22(4):429–35. Epub 1990/08/01. pmid:2205778.
  4. 4. Douglas J, Pearson S, Ross A, McGuigan M. Eccentric Exercise: Physiological Characteristics and Acute Responses. Sports Med. 2017;47(4):663–75. Epub 2016/09/18. pmid:27638040.
  5. 5. Vadasz B, Gohari J, West DW, Grosman-Rimon L, Wright E, Ozcakar L, et al. Improving characterization and diagnosis quality of myofascial pain syndrome: a systematic review of the clinical and biomarker overlap with delayed onset muscle soreness. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. 2020;56(4):469–78. Epub 2020/02/20. pmid:32072791.
  6. 6. Fullwood D, Means S, Merriwether EN, Chimenti RL, Ahluwalia S, Booker SQ. Toward Understanding Movement-evoked Pain (MEP) and its Measurement: A Scoping Review. Clin J Pain. 2021;37(1):61–78. Epub 2020/10/24. pmid:33093342; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7708514.
  7. 7. Corbett DB, Simon CB, Manini TM, George SZ, Riley JL III, Fillingim RB. Movement-evoked pain: transforming the way we understand and measure pain. Pain. 2019;160(4):757. pmid:30371555
  8. 8. Uddin Z. Movement-evoked Pain (MEP). Clin J Pain. 2021;37(4):310–1. Epub 2021/01/12. pmid:33427758.
  9. 9. Matsuda Y, Kan S, Uematsu H, Shibata M, Fujino Y. Pain-Related Brain Activity Evoked by Active and Dynamic Arm Movement: Delayed-Onset Muscle Soreness as a Promising Model for Studying Movement-Related Pain in Humans. Pain Med. 2015;16(8):1528–39. Epub 2015/05/02. pmid:25929675; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4692093.
  10. 10. Ayles S, Graven-Nielsen T, Gibson W. Vibration-induced afferent activity augments delayed onset muscle allodynia. J Pain. 2011;12(8):884–91. Epub 2011/06/15. pmid:21665552.
  11. 11. Barlas P, Walsh D, Baxter G, Allen J. Delayed onset muscle soreness: effect of an ischaemic block upon mechanical allodynia in humans. Pain. 2000;87(2):221–5. pmid:10924815
  12. 12. Nagi SS, Mahns DA. C-tactile fibers contribute to cutaneous allodynia after eccentric exercise. J Pain. 2013;14(5):538–48. Epub 2013/04/09. pmid:23562300.
  13. 13. Baron R, Hans G, Dickenson AH. Peripheral input and its importance for central sensitization. Annals of neurology. 2013;74(5):630–6. pmid:24018757
  14. 14. Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. 2011;152(3 Suppl):S2–S15. Epub 2010/10/22. pmid:20961685; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3268359.
  15. 15. Cheung K, Hume PA, Maxwell L. Delayed Onset Muscle Soreness. Sports Medicine. 2003;33(2):145–64. pmid:12617692
  16. 16. Newham DJ. The consequences of eccentric contractions and their relationship to delayed onset muscle pain. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. 1988;57(3):353–9. Epub 1988/01/01. pmid:3371343
  17. 17. Hotfiel T, Freiwald J, Hoppe MW, Lutter C, Forst R, Grim C, et al. Advances in Delayed-Onset Muscle Soreness (DOMS): Part I: Pathogenesis and Diagnostics. Sportverletz Sportschaden. 2018;32(4):243–50. Epub 2018/12/12. pmid:30537791.
  18. 18. Kellermann M, Heiss R, Swoboda B, Gelse K, Freiwald J, Grim C, et al. Intramuscular Perfusion Response in Delayed Onset Muscle Soreness (DOMS): A Quantitative Analysis with Contrast-Enhanced Ultrasound (CEUS). Int J Sports Med. 2017;38(11):833–41. Epub 2017/08/12. pmid:28799160.
  19. 19. Peake J, Nosaka K, Suzuki K. Characterization of inflammatory responses to eccentric exercise in humans. Exerc Immunol Rev. 2005;11:64–85. Epub 2006/01/03. pmid:16385845.
  20. 20. Damas F, Nosaka K, Libardi CA, Chen TC, Ugrinowitsch C. Susceptibility to Exercise-Induced Muscle Damage: a Cluster Analysis with a Large Sample. Int J Sports Med. 2016;37(8):633–40. Epub 2016/04/27. pmid:27116346.
  21. 21. Isaacs AW, Macaluso F, Smith C, Myburgh KH. C-Reactive Protein Is Elevated Only in High Creatine Kinase Responders to Muscle Damaging Exercise. Front Physiol. 2019;10:86. Epub 2019/02/26. pmid:30804809; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6378920.
  22. 22. Nosaka K, Newton M, Sacco P. Delayed-onset muscle soreness does not reflect the magnitude of eccentric exercise-induced muscle damage. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2002;12(6):337–46. Epub 2002/11/28. pmid:12453160.
  23. 23. Wideman TH, Finan PH, Edwards RR, Quartana PJ, Buenaver LF, Haythornthwaite JA, et al. Increased sensitivity to physical activity among individuals with knee osteoarthritis: relation to pain outcomes, psychological factors, and responses to quantitative sensory testing. Pain. 2014;155(4):703–11. Epub 2014/01/01. pmid:24378879.
  24. 24. Rakel B, Vance C, Zimmerman MB, Petsas-Blodgett N, Amendola A, Sluka KA. Mechanical hyperalgesia and reduced quality of life occur in people with mild knee osteoarthritis pain. Clin J Pain. 2015;31(4):315–22. Epub 2014/05/29. pmid:24866856.
  25. 25. McPhee M, Graven-Nielsen T. Alterations in Temporal Summation of Pain and Conditioned Pain Modulation Across an Episode of Experimental Exercise-Induced Low Back Pain. J Pain. 2019;20(3):264–76. Epub 2018/09/22. pmid:30236748.
  26. 26. Bishop MD, George SZ, Robinson ME. Dynamic, but not static, pain sensitivity predicts exercise-induced muscle pain: covariation of temporal sensory summation and pain intensity. Neuroscience letters. 2012;526(1):1–4. pmid:22967843
  27. 27. Parr JJ, Borsa PA, Fillingim RB, Tillman MD, Manini TM, Gregory CM, et al. Pain-related fear and catastrophizing predict pain intensity and disability independently using an induced muscle injury model. J Pain. 2012;13(4):370–8. Epub 2012/03/20. pmid:22424914; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3321109.
  28. 28. Bishop MD, Horn ME, George SZ. Exercise-induced pain intensity predicted by pre-exercise fear of pain and pain sensitivity. Clin J Pain. 2011;27(5):398–404. Epub 2011/03/19. pmid:21415719; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3092017.
  29. 29. Melzack R. The short-form McGill pain questionnaire. Pain. 1987;30(2):191–7. pmid:3670870
  30. 30. Niederstrasser NG, Meulders A, Meulders M, Slepian PM, Vlaeyen JW, Sullivan MJ. Pain catastrophizing and fear of pain predict the experience of pain in body parts not targeted by a delayed-onset muscle soreness procedure. The Journal of Pain. 2015;16(11):1065–76. pmid:26277643
  31. 31. Niederstrasser NG, Slepian PM, Mankovsky-Arnold T, Larivière C, Vlaeyen JW, Sullivan MJ. An experimental approach to examining psychological contributions to multisite musculoskeletal pain. The Journal of Pain. 2014;15(11):1156–65. pmid:25179149
  32. 32. Mankovsky-Arnold T, Wideman TH, Lariviere C, Sullivan MJ. TENS attenuates repetition-induced summation of activity-related pain following experimentally induced muscle soreness. J Pain. 2013;14(11):1416–24. Epub 2013/11/05. pmid:24182660.
  33. 33. Randall F. Recent guidance on resuscitation: patients’ choices and doctors’ duties. Palliat Med. 2001;15(6):449–50. Epub 2002/10/31. pmid:12403501.
  34. 34. Trost Z, France CR, Thomas JS. Pain-related fear and avoidance of physical exertion following delayed-onset muscle soreness. Pain. 2011;152(7):1540–7. Epub 2011/03/23. pmid:21419575.
  35. 35. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle TR, Treede DR, Beyer A, et al. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123(3):231–43. Epub 2006/05/16. pmid:16697110.
  36. 36. Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychological assessment. 1995;7(4):524.
  37. 37. Granot M, Ferber SG. The Roles of Pain Catastrophizing and Anxiety in the Prediction of Postoperative Pain Intensity: A Prospective Study. The Clinical Journal of Pain. 2005;21(5):439–45. pmid:16093750.
  38. 38. Teichman Y, Malineck F. Manual for the Hebrew state-trait anxiety inventory. Tel Aviv, Israel: Tel Aviv University. 1978.
  39. 39. Grös DF, Antony MM, Simms LJ, McCabe RE. Psychometric properties of the State-Trait Inventory for Cognitive and Somatic Anxiety (STICSA): comparison to the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Psychological assessment. 2007;19(4):369. pmid:18085930
  40. 40. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri WF. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories-IA and-II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of personality assessment. 1996;67(3):588–97. pmid:8991972
  41. 41. Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical psychology review. 1988;8(1):77–100.
  42. 42. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of health and social behavior. 1983:385–96. pmid:6668417
  43. 43. Asmundson GJ, Bovell CV, Carleton RN, McWilliams LA. The Fear of Pain Questionnaire–Short Form (FPQ-SF): factorial validity and psychometric properties. Pain. 2008;134(1–2):51–8. pmid:17482361
  44. 44. Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A. GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers. 1996;28(1):1–11. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203630.
  45. 45. Hothorn T, Hornik K, Zeileis A. Unbiased Recursive Partitioning: A Conditional Inference Framework. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics. 2006;15(3):651–74.
  46. 46. Szikszay TM, Adamczyk WM, Wojtyna E, Luedtke K. Pain inhibition is not affected by exercise-induced pain. Pain Reports. 2020;5(2):e817. pmid:32440610
  47. 47. Gibson W, Arendt-Nielsen L, Graven-Nielsen T. Delayed onset muscle soreness at tendon–bone junction and muscle tissue is associated with facilitated referred pain. Experimental brain research. 2006;174(2):351–60. pmid:16642316
  48. 48. Mankovsky-Arnold T, Wideman TH, Lariviere C, Sullivan MJ. Measures of spontaneous and movement-evoked pain are associated with disability in patients with whiplash injuries. J Pain. 2014;15(9):967–75. Epub 2014/07/08. pmid:24998695.
  49. 49. Arendt-Nielsen L, Yarnitsky D. Experimental and clinical applications of quantitative sensory testing applied to skin, muscles and viscera. The Journal of Pain. 2009;10(6):556–72. pmid:19380256
  50. 50. Fischer AA. Pressure algometry over normal muscles. Standard values, validity and reproducibility of pressure threshold. Pain. 1987;30(1):115–26. pmid:3614975
  51. 51. Keele K. Pain-sensitivity tests: the pressure algometer. The Lancet. 1954;263(6813):636–9. pmid:13143740
  52. 52. Fillingim RB, Edwards RR, Powell T. The relationship of sex and clinical pain to experimental pain responses. PAIN®. 1999;83(3):419–25.
  53. 53. Gibson SJ, Helme RD. Age-related differences in pain perception and report. Clinics in geriatric medicine. 2001;17(3):433–56. pmid:11459714
  54. 54. Walton D, MacDermid J, Nielson W, Teasell R, Reese H, Levesque L. Pressure pain threshold testing demonstrates predictive ability in people with acute whiplash. journal of orthopaedic & sports physical therapy. 2011;41(9):658–65. pmid:21885908
  55. 55. Tanabe N, Fujisawa M. Prospective cohort study of pressure pain threshold and electromyograms associated with masticatory muscle pain incidence. Prosthodontic Research & Practice. 2005;4(1):48–57.
  56. 56. Lee JE, Watson D, Frey‐Law LA. Psychological factors predict local and referred experimental muscle pain: a cluster analysis in healthy adults. European journal of pain. 2013;17(6):903–15. pmid:23165778
  57. 57. George SZ, Dover GC, Fillingim RB. Fear of pain influences outcomes after exercise-induced delayed onset muscle soreness at the shoulder. The Clinical journal of pain. 2007;23(1):76–84. pmid:17277648
  58. 58. Trost Z, France CR, Sullivan MJ, Thomas JS. Pain-related fear predicts reduced spinal motion following experimental back injury. PAIN®. 2012;153(5):1015–21. pmid:22377437
  59. 59. Lucchetti G, Oliveira AB, Mercante JPP, Peres MFP. Anxiety and fear-avoidance in musculoskeletal pain. Current pain and headache reports. 2012;16(5):399–406. pmid:22791352
  60. 60. Rhudy JL, Meagher MW. Fear and anxiety: divergent effects on human pain thresholds. Pain. 2000;84(1):65–75. pmid:10601674
  61. 61. Uddin Z, Woznowski-Vu A, Flegg D, Aternali A, Wideman TH. A Cumulative Impact of Psychological and Sensitization Risk Factors on Pain-Related Outcomes. Pain Pract. 2021;21(5):523–35. Epub 2020/12/15. pmid:33316140.
  62. 62. Appell H-J, Soares J, Duarte J. Exercise, muscle damage and fatigue. Sports Medicine. 1992;13(2):108–15. pmid:1561506
  63. 63. McDougall JJ. Arthritis and pain. Neurogenic origin of joint pain. Arthritis Res Ther. 2006;8(6):220. Epub 2006/11/23. pmid:17118212; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1794504.
  64. 64. Bajaj P, Graven-Nielsen T, Wright A, Arendt-Nielsen L. Muscle hyperalgesia in postexercise muscle soreness assessed by single and repetitive ultrasound stimuli. The Journal of Pain. 2000;1(2):111–21.
  65. 65. Weerakkody NS, Whitehead NP, Canny BJ, Gregory JE, Proske U. Large-fiber mechanoreceptors contribute to muscle soreness after eccentric exercise. The Journal of Pain. 2001;2(4):209–19. pmid:14622819
  66. 66. Hoheisel U, Mense S, Simons DG, Yu X-M. Appearance of new receptive fields in rat dorsal horn neurons following noxious stimulation of skeletal muscle: a model for referral of muscle pain? Neuroscience letters. 1993;153(1):9–12. pmid:8510831
  67. 67. Nie H, Arendt-Nielsen L, Madeleine P, Graven-Nielsen T. Enhanced temporal summation of pressure pain in the trapezius muscle after delayed onset muscle soreness. Experimental brain research. 2006;170(2):182–90. pmid:16328284
  68. 68. Nie H, Madeleine P, Arendt‐Nielsen L, Graven‐Nielsen T. Temporal summation of pressure pain during muscle hyperalgesia evoked by nerve growth factor and eccentric contractions. European Journal of Pain. 2009;13(7):704–10. pmid:18710817