Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Positive emotion dispositions and emotion regulation in the Italian population

  • Alice Chirico ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    alice.chirico@unicatt.it

    Affiliation Catholic University of the Sacred Heart of Milan, Milan, Italy

  • Michelle N. Shiota,

    Roles Supervision

    Affiliation Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America

  • Andrea Gaggioli

    Roles Supervision, Validation

    Affiliations Catholic University of the Sacred Heart of Milan, Milan, Italy, Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, I.R.C.C.S. Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy

Abstract

The goal of this large-scale study was to test the relationship between positive emotion dispositions (i.e., Joy, Contentment, Pride, Love, Compassion, Amusement, and Awe) and two strategies of emotion regulation (i.e., reappraisal and suppression) in the Italian population. 532 Italian-speaking adults completed the Dispositional Positive Emotion Scales (DPES), the Positive and Negative Affective Schedule (PANAS), the Italian Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), and the Big-Five Inventory (BFI). DPES scales showed high reliability. Exploratory Factor Analysis showed that a 6-factor model fits the Italian sample better. Joy and Contentment loaded on the same factor. Items assessing the other five emotions loaded on separate factors. The patterns of relationships between positive emotion dispositions, positive and negative affects traits (PANAS), and personality traits (BFI) indicated concurrent validity of the DPES. Twelve separated multiple regression models with BFI and ERQ factors as predictors and DPES factors as response variables showed that Extraversion significantly positively predicted of all DPES emotions. Agreeableness predicted Happiness, Love, Compassion, and Awe positively. Conscientiousness predicted Amusement and Love negatively and Compassion, Pride, and Happiness positively. Neuroticism predicted all emotions negatively except for Compassion. Positive emotions were significantly and positively predicted by reappraisal, and negatively predicted by suppression.

Introduction

Evolution has provided us with a unique and valuable tool to respond adaptively to environmental changes through emotions [1, 2]. An increasing number of researchers conceive emotions as discrete, universal, evolutionary, functional, and organized system composed of behavioral, psychophysiological, experiential, and cognitive responses to perceived challenges and opportunities [3, 4]. Particularly, emotions organize the mind’s responses to specific adaptive situational demands [5]. Consequently, recurrent features of the situation have progressively acquired a place in the architecture of the emotional process [6]. For instance, sadness is usually triggered in response to a significant loss [7]; thus, it is also characterized by physiological withdrawal reactions to recover from the loss.

The recurrent need to manage a specific situation triggering the most fitness-promoting responses—relevant for adaptation—constantly shapes and refines emotion-related features. Therefore, these regulatory processes vary according to a given emotion [e.g., 8]. In the first decade of the twentieth century, several researchers have started a new line of research focusing on the link between specific discrete emotions and their management, that is, peculiar emotion regulation (ER) strategies [e.g., 9, 10]. They showed differential emotion regulation paths, especially for each negative emotion, such as sadness [8] or shame [10]. However, although the interest in regulatory processes and discrete negative emotions-link has increased recently, regulation strategies occurring within the unfolding of positive emotions are still an open issue, maybe, since benefits associated with positive emotions are slower to come [11], while detrimental effects of negative emotions are immediately evident and urgent [9, 12]. Specifically, positive emotions have been mainly conceived as desirable phenomena that people attempt to maintain or enhance [19].

Only recently, an increasing number of researchers [e.g., 5, 6, 13] have devoted their attention to the relationship between other general positive (vs. negative) affect and specific emotion regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. These two strategies can be deployed at the two poles of emotion regulation process, thus, virtually covering the full process of an emotional response. Cognitive reappraisal comes early and affects the onset of an emotional response, and it involves re-thinking a situation to change its meaning and emotional effect [14]. On the other hand, Suppression is a response-focused strategy that moderates outward signs of an emotional response, already deployed and appraised, by decreasing concurrent emotion-expressive behavior [15]. Cognitive reappraisal displayed a healthier pattern of emotional, cognitive, and social effects compared to expressive suppression [16]. Trait cognitive reappraisal predicted reactivity to affective stimuli by increasing the positive effect and decreasing the negative one [14, 17]. On the other hand, trait suppression appraisal led to more negative affective states [18]. Crucially, Barrett et al. [9] found that individuals who were better able to distinguish discrete emotional states were also more competent in regulating negative and positive emotions compared to those who were less able to differentiate specific discrete emotions. In conclusion, fairly little is known about how these two strategies interact with specific positive emotions, which are crucial for our flourishing [19], and nothing has been investigated regarding cross-cultural variations of this link.

Take the case of compassion. It should be conceived as a positive emotion serving specific interpersonal functions [2022]. Compassion can be defined as a “feeling that arises in witnessing another’s suffering and that motivates a subsequent desire to help” [23]. It entails a commitment to and thus facilitation of a relationship [24]. Therefore, this emotion should be downregulated to save resources and direct them only to specific people [25, 26]. This would be far more crucial if dealing with a stable tendency to experience compassion related to stable or recurrent circumstances, as suggested by Goetz, Keltner [23]. With this regard, an emerging trend in emotion theories suggests that positive emotions should be considered not only as discrete states evolutionarily shaped to respond to specific demands [20, 24, 2729] but also as stable traits or dispositions to specific eliciting situations.

Daily life is imbued with systematic attempts to modify even specific positive emotions that do not fit the social norms of a situation. For instance, we try to avoid an amused laugh every time our colleague we dislike shows up. We try not to exhibit an excessive proudful smile every time we beat all other competitors at work. We attempt to empathize when our tenderhearted friend talks about her new boyfriend at dinner. These eliciting situations can repeatedly occur, thus bringing forth stable emotional tendencies.

However, while the relationship between discrete dispositional positive emotions and other durable tendencies, such as personality traits [e.g., 30] or specific strengths and virtues [31], has been deeply investigated, the way people usually regulate them has remained unexplored. Specifically, research has showed that in the domain of stable affect, not all emotion regulation strategies can be considered as equal [32]. Despite this evidence, nothing is known about the unique pathway linking specific emotion regulation strategies to specific discrete positive emotions, whose peculiar functional value it is crucial to support people’ wellbeing and health [33].

Measuring the disposition to experience positive emotions

In line with this evolutionary-functionalist approach on dispositional positive emotions, Shiota et al. [20], developed a self-reported instrument to assess individuals’ proneness to experience seven discrete positive emotional states, namely, Joy, Contentment, Pride, Love, Compassion, Amusement, and Awe. Joy is usually associated with a sense of strength and self-confidence [34] in safe and secure situations that promote the exploitation of new resources [35] and social connections [36]. Contentment is a low-arousal emotion related to the evaluation of actual material resources as exceeding individuals’ needs. This emotion invites people to savor their time while becoming aware of the available resources that can be integrated into schemas on self and others [37]. Therefore, Contentment can change both worldview and self-perception. Pride is a self-conscious emotion [38] arising from success during a socially relevant activity [20]. It increases our status, provides information about the level of social acceptance, and expands the possibility to exploit more group resources [20, 39]. Shiota et al. [20] defined Love based on Bowlby’s attachment behavioral system [40, 41]. People perceive a positive emotion of closeness towards trustworthy and dependable persons who take care of them. Compassion consists of feelings of concern and nurturance towards other people, even non-kin [42], especially if they are children, helpless, and vulnerable [e.g., 4345]. Love and compassion facilitate social connectedness [20]. Amusement arises when there is a shift between one’s expectancies and one’s experience, and it usually occurs during humoristic situations [46]. Finally, Awe is a complex emotion arising when faced with vast and cognitively challenging stimuli [47]. Awe is a prosocial [48], epistemological [24, 47], and self-transcendent emotion [49].

Shiota et al. [20] showed that each positive emotion disposition of this scale correlates significantly with at least one of the BFI dimensions (i.e., Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness to Experience), in line with the broadening nature of positive emotions [50]. The only exception was Compassion, which showed a small and not significant correlation with Neuroticism. Albeit preliminary, these findings are in line with the assumption that personality traits could predict specific emotional tendencies. Moreover, studies on dispositional discrete positive emotions have been conducted mainly in English-speaking countries. Thus, although DPES was developed to capture potentially universal, functionally distinct, and discrete emotion constructs, as well as their relationship with other universal phenomena, the definitions of each emotion reflect English-language constructs. Therefore, it is relevant to test the DPES factor structure also in Italian, among other languages.

In backing up this claim, a six-factor solution (instead of the original seven-factors structure) emerged from the exploratory analysis in the German validation of the scale, which was carried out at the item level [31] and in a more recent validation study carried out by Dixson, Anderson, & Keltner [51]. Specifically, Contentment and Joy loaded on the same factor, while other emotions loaded on the residual five factors. This study showed that emotions could be split into two subtypes. Pride, Joy, and Contentment appeared to reflect self-oriented emotions while Amusement, Love, Compassion, and Awe emerged as other-focused emotions, depending upon their main elicitors. In this regard, the evolutionary functionalist account suggests that emotions emerge quickly from specific eliciting situations and serve to manage them–usually–in the most appropriate way [14, 15, 52]. Since different emotions arise from different elicitors and trigger different action tendencies, it has been suggested that specific strategies could regulate each emotional state [5355]. These strategies, which help people redirect the instinctive emergence of emotions, are considered the components of the “emotion regulation” process [14]. However, to our best knowledge, no study has investigated the link between discrete positive emotions and specific emotion regulation strategies.

Therefore, it becomes far more relevant to investigate whether the frequent use of a given emotion regulation strategy is associated with specific positive emotions and whether it can, eventually, predict them. In this study, we sought to investigate the link between specific discrete positive emotions and two main categories of emotion regulation strategies of emotion-regulation, that is, cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Thus, we aimed to determine whether there can be differential paths linking the two emotion regulation categories and each positive emotion. We also aimed to test whether specific BFI factors could predict each discrete emotion disposition. In the following paragraph, we detail the direction of the causal link between emotion regulation strategies and dispositions to experience positive emotions.

The current study

To contribute to the current literature on emotion regulation and discrete positive emotions, we carried out an exploratory study aimed at investigating the relationship between the seven discrete positive emotions of DPES and two major emotion regulation strategies, namely, Reappraisal and Suppression [15, 18]. With this regard, while the relationship between discrete dispositional positive emotions and other durable tendencies, such as personality traits [e.g., 30] or specific strengths and virtues [31], has been deeply investigated, the way people usually regulate them has remained unexplored. Despite it has been demonstrated the unique predictive value of specific emotion regulation strategies on stable affect [32], nothing is known about the unique pathways linking specific emotion regulation strategies to specific discrete positive emotions considered as stable traits, whose peculiar functional value it is crucial to support peoples’ wellbeing and health [33].

In this study, the DPES by Shiota et al. [20] and the Italian version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) [18] were administered to a sample of native Italian participants. ERQ assesses how frequently individuals use cognitive reappraisal or suppression strategies. Given the high degree of differentiation among discrete positive emotions [20], we hypothesized the existence of different correlation patterns between each emotion and each emotion regulation strategy, which can be explained in light of a functional model of emotions. Specifically, starting from evidence on the impact of peculiar ER strategies on stable affect and emotions [19, 32, 5658], we assumed that the frequent use of a given emotion regulation strategy (cognitive reappraisal or suppression) could predict also discrete stable positive emotions.

Moreover, since the definitions of each emotion reflected English-language constructs, as mentioned earlier, we also tested the factor structure of the translated Italian version of the DPES, as well as its concurrent and divergent validity. We also administered the Italian version of Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [59] as a measure of concurrent validity concerning the DPES (see [51]. PANAS is a well-validated measure of positive vs. negative affect in Italian. Finally, we also assessed the Big Five personality traits using the Big Five Inventory (BFI) [60] Italian version [61]. Therefore, the concurrent validity of the Italian DPES was studied by examining both the extent to which the different scales were associated with the two global dimensions of affect, as assessed by PANAS, and with Big Five factors, as assessed by the BFI Italian version. In line with previous validation studies [20, 31, 62], we expected positive correlations of all DPES factors with PANAS scales, as well as with Extraversion and Openness to Experience personality traits, and negative correlations with Neuroticism trait. Moreover, given preliminary but promising evidence on the link between BFI and DPES factors [30], we tested also the contribution of BFI factors to each discrete emotion.

Method

Participants

The Italian DPES version was administered to 532 participants from Italy (mean age = 25.2; SD = 5.698, range = 18–49) of whom 439 were women (mean age = 25.05; SD = 5.448) and 93 were men (mean age = 26.42; SD = 6.671). Sample size was established on the base of previous works that validated or used scale in US or European populations [20, 31, 51, 62]. Their religious orientation was as follows: 63.62% was Catholic, 0.38% was Buddhist, 0.56% were Muslim or Protestant, 34.77% indicated practicing a non-conventional religion or being Atheists. Mean years of education of the sample was 16.20 (S.D. = 2.812), ranging from a minimum of 5 to a maximum of 26 years. Mean years of education was 15.58 (S.D. = .310) for men, ranging from 8 to 22 years, and 16.32 (S.D. = .133) for women, ranging from 5 to 26 years. The participants were recruited through social networks and flyers. They completed the online version of the DPES voluntarily and received no credit for their participation in the study. The same participants also completed the Italian Emotion Regulation Questionnaire [18], Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Italian version [59], and the BFI Italian version [60] on the same occasion. All the instruments are reported and described in the next section “Materials”. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore of Milan prior to data collection and it was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Materials

In this study, the Italian version of the DPES was translated from the original English questionnaire [20], and it uses the same item numbering. The DPES is a self-administered questionnaire, composed of 38 items that measure the proneness to live seven discrete positive emotions. It was structured by seven different scales, each representing a specific positive emotion: Joy (6 items), Contentment (5 items), Pride (5 items), Love (6 items), Compassion (5 items), Amusement (5 items), and Awe (6 items). The 38 items were presented to responders in a seven-step Likert scale ranging from Strongly Disagree (1) to Strongly Agree (7). First, two bilingual translators (one expert in the field of emotions and the other naïve) translated Dispositional Positive Emotions to Italian, as suggested by Borsa, Damásio and Baindeira [63]. Further, the accuracy of the translation was tested by a back-translation from Italian to English done by two different bilingual translators, both fluent in Italian and English. The back-translation was checked by one of the authors of the original version. Afterward, three expert judges compared the original and back to finalize the Italian version.

Italian version of the ERQ

Italian ERQ [18] is a 10-item 7-points likert (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree) scale consisting into two scales each measuring an emotion regulation strategy: suppression (4 items) and reappraisal (6 items). Participants were instructed to think about how they control (that is, regulate and manage) their emotions. Cronbach alpha for both original scales was moderate to high (reappraisal = .84; suppression = .72).

Italian version of the PANAS

The Italian PANAS version [59] captures the two main clusters of the affective experience. Specifically, this questionnaire is composed of 20 adjectives measuring the two main dimensions of affective experience: the positive (10 adjectives) [Positive Affect scale (PA)] and the negative one (10 adjectives) [Negative Affect (NA)]. In this study, we adopted the trait version by instructing participants to report the extent to which they usually feel as described by each of the 20 adjectives. The Italian PANAS version showed an adequate internal-consistency reliability for PA (.83) and for NA (.87) using the trait time instruction.

Italian version of the BFI

The Italian Big Five Inventory (BFI) [60] is a is a self-report questionnaire relying on the work of [64], which measures the five personality trait dimensions defined by the Five-Factor Theory of Personality: Conscientiousness (9 items), Agreeableness (9 items), Extraversion (8 items), Neuroticism (8 items), and Openness (10 items). It consists of 44 items on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 “Disagree Strongly” to 5 “Strongly Agree”. The internal consistency range in this sample was α = 0.70−0.84.

Procedure

Participants completed the survey in Qualtrics survey software and were provided with an online informed consent. No minors were included. Data have been anonymized. Participants were fully informed regarding the procedure and objectives of this study. They were recruited through flyers, social networks and by word of mouth. They were first informed that they would be completing some questionnaires about positive and negative emotions. Then, first, they filled the first part of the questionnaire created to gather socio-demographic information. Secondly, they completed the Italian translation of the DPES. Finally, they completed the Italian version of the PANAS and of the BFI.

Results

This study was designed to test the relationship between two emotion regulation strategies and seven emotion dispositions. This was the first time that this scale was administered to an Italian sample. Therefore, we first assessed the structure of the DPES to determine the number of components needed to adequately describe the psychological constructs of Positive Emotions Dispositions in the Italian sample. We then assessed concurrent and divergent validity with PANAS and BFI subscales. Finally, we examined the relationship between the two emotion regulation strategies and the resulting emotion dispositions.

Primary analyses

To examine the structure of the Italian DPES, an Explorative Factor Analysis (EFA) analysis was carried out on the original set of items. Data were analyzed by IBM SPSS Statistics software (Version 21, release 21.0.0.0 64-bit edition). No missing values were found. Skewness and kurtosis of DPES items indicated a normal distribution. The means of factors ranged from 5.704 (compassion) to 4.420 (love). To get a clear idea of the relationships between the seven emotional dispositions and their underlying latent constructs, we carried out an EFA. The aim was to uncover the underlying structure of this large set of variables (38 items) in an Italian sample [65].

Structure of the DPES scale: Exploratory factor analysis

Before carrying out the Exploratory Factor Analysis, a parallel Monte Carlo simulation analysis [66] was run on the 38 items to determine the number of factors to retain in EFA. This analysis revealed a maximum of seven-factor structure. The seven-factor solution was confirmed by the eigenvalues, indicating that 7 factors had eigenvalues greater than 1.0, which accounted for 36.0% of the total variance. Inspection of the correlation matrix showed several coefficients of 0.30 and above.

Therefore, we chose to test the 7-factor solution in line with the original structure of the scale, as suggested by Parallel Analysis. Moreover, given the nature of the scale, which assesses positive emotions, we expected that our factors were correlated. Therefore, first, we carried out a Principal Axis Factoring (PAF) analysis with oblimin rotation (delta = 0). Pearson’s r correlations among factors were closer to .3. The 7-factor solution explained 60.301% variance. This solution was also supported by Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy (.90) and Bartlett’s test of Sphericity [χ2 (703) = 9818.071; p < .01]. However, item 36 had a very low level of communality (.106) and items did not load clearly in the seven factors; therefore, we chose to trim item 36, and we ran the PAF again, with oblimin rotation forcing a 7-factor solution. Global explained variance increased (61.481%). However, most items still double-loaded on several factors, and the resulting factorial structure was not easily interpretable. Therefore, we chose to run PAF again, with oblimin rotation forcing a 6 factors solution, in line with the findings of Gusewell and Ruch [31] (see Table 1 for Factors correlation matrix using principal axis factoring with oblimin rotation 6-factor solution).

thumbnail
Table 1. Factors correlation matrix using principal axis factoring with oblimin rotation 6-factor solution.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245545.t001

This solution explained less variance (58.398%), but all items loaded on each factor more clearly, without double loading items or just one item per factor. Specifically, the final solution had all Joy (i.e., 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6) and Contentment (i.e., 7, 8, 9, 10, 11) items loading on the first factor, while the Pride, Love, Compassion, Humor, and Awe items each loaded on separate factors. Only items from the original Joy scale merged with those of the original Contentment scale, while Pride emerged as a separate factor. This is in line with the validation study of the German version of this scale by Güsewell and Ruch [31]. Consistent with Güsewell and Ruch [31], we selected the 6-factors solution as the best model to explain our variables (see Table 2. for the final factor solution).

thumbnail
Table 2. Final factor solution: Pattern matrix showing factor loadings from exploratory factor analysis PAF with oblimin rotation in the final version of the scale (37 items, n = 532).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245545.t002

Items loading on Factor 1, which we called “Happiness” (ten items), represent a positive attitude towards life in general (e.g., “I am generally a contented person”). This factor encompasses all items from the original “Contentment” scale (except for item 11 that loaded on Factor 5) as well as items from the original “Joy” scale (e.g.., “Good things happen to me all the time”; “My life is always improving”). Items loading on Factor 2, called “Compassion” (five items), represent a desire to take care of, or a concern for, another’s well-being, especially for those in need of help and vulnerable. Items loading on Compassion factors are the same as those loading on the original scale. Items loading on Factor 3, “Amusement” (five items), refer to positive feelings in humorous situations perceived as inconsistent with one’s expectations [46]. Items loading on Factor 4, called “Love” (six items), are the same as the original factors called “Love.” This factor concerns trust towards other people who are conceived as straightforward and authentic. Items loading on Factor 5, called “Pride” (six items), the same items as those in the original scale along with the last item from the original “Contentment” scale (item: “My life is very fulfilling), capture a social recognition of self-identity (e.g., “Many people respect me”). Finally, items loading on Factor 6 “Awe” (five items) all belong to the original scale. This Factor refers to a deep sense of wonder and astonishment when facing something grand to an extent to require an update of individuals’ current mental frames [47]. Only original item 36 was omitted due to the low response variance (i.e., “I often look for patterns in the objects around me”).

Gender differences for DPES factors in the Italian sample

We also tested gender differences in each DPES factor, since Güsewell and Ruch [31] found significant differences between men and women in joy, contentment, compassion, and awe. A one-way ANOVA with gender as factor showed that women (mean = 5.75; S.D. = .791) scored significantly higher compared to men on compassion (mean = 5.46; S.D. = .982) [F(1) = 9.468; p = .002; partial eta square = .018], while men reported significantly higher levels of amusement (mean = 5.24; S.D. = .904) compared to women (mean = 4.928; S.D. = .996) [F(1) = 7.861; p = .005; partial eta square = .018].

Scale reliabilities and concurrent validities

A further aim of this study was to assess the internal consistency and reliability of the DPES scales. We calculated the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for all scales. Internal consistency of each scale was high: “Happiness” scale (.906), Pride scale (.798), and the Awe scale (.794), with .80 for love, .80 for compassion, and .75 for amusement. The Awe scale and the overall reliability increased compared to the original scale. This study also investigated the criterion validity of the scale, correlating it first with the two subscales of PANAS, which are self-report measures conceptually similar, assessing the same construct of experiencing positive emotions. Second, the correlations between DPES, BFI and ERQ scores were computed. The internal consistency and criterion validities were calculated using the data from the 532 participants. All correlations between DPES, BFI, and ERQ, as well as PANAS, were carried out considering the Italian structure of DPES (i.e., 6-factors structure).

PANAS.

Reliability was calculated using the data from the 532 participants. The scale showed high reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha = .911). To calculate the scale criterion validity, we computed a Pearson correlation between the DPES and the PANAS positive and negative subscales scores (i.e., PA- Positive Affect; NA- Negative Affect). In this sample, the PA subscale of the PANAS had good reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha = .839), as did the NA subscale (Cronbach’s Alpha = .881).

Factors from the DPES correlated positively with the Positive Affect Scales and negatively, or not at all, with the Negative Affect Scale. Most correlations were significant (See Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Pearson correlations between PANAS factors (Positive–PA- and Negative–NA–Affect) and DPES factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245545.t003

BFI

Reliability was calculated using the data from the 532 participants. The BFI scale showed a good level of reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha = .783). To calculate the scale criterion validity, we computed Pearson correlations between the DPES and the BFI subscales scores (i.e., Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, Openness). Correlations are reported in Table 4.

thumbnail
Table 4. Pearson correlations between BFI factors (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, Openness) and DPES factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245545.t004

Finally, all emotions displayed significant positive correlations with Openness to experience personality factors. Given the correlations between discrete positive emotions of DPES and BFI factors, we chose to carry out six multiple linear regression models with Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness as predictors and each discrete emotion as the response variable. The results are reported in Table 5.

thumbnail
Table 5. Summary of multiple regression analyses for BFI factors predicting each discrete emotion (n = 532).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245545.t005

Given the correlations between discrete positive emotions of DPES and ERQ factors (Table 6.), we chose to carry out six multiple linear regressions to test whether trait cognitive reappraisal and trait suppression significantly predicted participants’ ratings of each positive emotion disposition. See Table 7 for all multiple regression analyses with DPES factors as measures and each emotion regulation strategy (Cognitive Reappraisal and Suppression) as predictors.

thumbnail
Table 6. Pearson correlations between ERQ factors (Suppression and reappraisal) and DPES factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245545.t006

thumbnail
Table 7. Summary of single regression analyses for each discrete emotion predicted by cognitive reappraisal and suppression as factors (n = 532).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245545.t007

Cognitive Reappraisal was a significant positive predictor of all emotions of DPES. Suppression emerged as a significant negative predictor of all emotions of DPES, except for Amusement and Awe, which were not significantly predicted by Suppression.

Discussion

This is the first study in which the relationship between specific discrete positive emotional tendencies and two of the major emotion regulation strategies of reappraisal and suppression was tested. To this end, we administered one stable measure of emotion regulation (ERQ)–already validated in Italian–and of discrete positive emotion dispositions (DPES), which was translated into Italian. Although DPES was intended to capture potentially universal, functionally distinct emotion constructs, the definitions of each emotion reflect English-language constructs. Therefore, we also tested the DPES factor structure, as well as its concurrent and divergent validity, in Italian. The Italian version of the DPES was validated through an analytic strategy that involved exploratory factor analysis and concurrent validity analysis. These steps yielded a six-factor scale, comprising 37 items out of the original 38 items, with acceptable to excellent alpha coefficients.

In this regard, some adjustments had to be made to reflect Italian culture. Specifically, the dimension of the original scales of Joy (items: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6) and Contentment (items: 7, 8, 9, 10, 11) underlined the same dimension in this sample, which we labeled “Happiness.” Moreover, one item of the original Contentment scale (i.e., My life is very fulfilling) loaded on the Pride factor. These findings could be due to the “other-oriented” and interdependent nature of Italian culture as opposed to English-speaking societies [66]. While the first items of joy referred explicitly to joy, the other was related to satisfaction towards life and self-domain in general. Indeed, the concept of Joy in Italy should be based on interpersonal-relational elicitors more than on one’s own life [67]. However, no DPES items explicitly referred to as interpersonal situations. Therefore, it could be possible that participants did not recognize peculiar elicitors or prototypical situations of joy while reading the joy-related items.

Consequently, a macro factor encompassing all items of life fulfillment and life satisfaction, in general, emerged from a blend of Joy and Contentment. This scale was labeled “Happiness” since it entailed all aspects related to satisfaction with one’s own. Following the Contentment factor, Compassion and Love also showed a high frequency of occurrence, in line with the findings of previous studies on emotional lexicon usage in Italian samples [e.g., 68]. Finally, in this study, one Awe item (i.e., the mental shift: “I often look for patterns in the objects around me”) was removed due to its initial low communality with other items (.141), improving the fit the scale to our data. Finally, Pride had a high mean occurrence frequency. This result could be due to the renowned Italian disposition towards others [67], which could facilitate social comparison at the base of Pride. Finally, one item from the Pride original scale loaded on the general Happiness factor. It could be due to the semantic domain covered by this item “My life is very fulfilling,” which, in Italian, is close to the domain of self-accomplishment. The concurrent validity of the Italian-DPES scale was evaluated with PA and NA and the BFI traits. First, the results showed that Italian-DPES had moderate to strong positive correlations with positive affect and moderate correlations with negative affect. Specifically, all discrete emotions showed significant correlations with PA; only Amusement and Compassion showed positive (but not significant) correlation with Negative Affect.

Furthermore, all dispositions showed significant correlations with BFI personality traits. All factors were significantly positively correlated with Extraversion, in line with Shiota et al. [20]. On the other hand, while in Shiota et al. [20]. All DPES subscales correlated significantly also with Agreeableness, except for Amusement. All DPES scales correlated negatively with Neuroticism, except for Compassion, which is also in line with Shiota et al. [20]. Finally, all emotions displayed significant positive correlations with Openness to experience personality factors. Agreeableness predicted positively Happiness, Love, Compassion, and Awe, consistent with the claim of McCrae and Costa [69], who suggested that this trait facilitates positive affect. Conversely, highly conscientious people were less inclined to experience life situations as amusing and to feel the love in their daily activities. Conscientiousness is defined as the propensity to respect social conventions to control impulses and to achieve personal goals [70], and some evidence demonstrated that only certain specific amusing situations were positively associated with this personality trait (i.e., self-enhancing humor as a coping strategy). It would be relevant to investigate amusement-related elicitors that the participants considered while completing the scale. At the same time, Conscientiousness predicted Love negatively. This result is surprising since previous research demonstrated the positive role of Consciousness in determining different facets of love (i.e., intimacy, passion, commitment, relationship satisfaction) [e.g., 71]. Additionally, Shiota et al. [20] showed that Conscientiousness and Love are positively, even though not significantly, related. However, all these studies were conducted in English-speaking countries. A possible explanation of this finding is that the Italian culture defines love differently from other Anglo-Saxon cultures.

On the other hand, Conscientiousness predicted Compassion, Pride, and Happiness positively. Neuroticism predicted all emotions negatively except from Compassion, which is also in line with Shiota et al. [20]. Almost all emotions displayed significant positive correlations (and were predicted by) with Openness to experience personality factors, consistent with the literature on positive emotions. However, Openness to experience did not predict Happiness, and it predicted Love negatively. This was surprising since DPES measure a Love directed towards other people. Italian people are perhaps more inclined to address this Love only to their intimate partners and not to others.

Finally, and importantly, our results revealed significant correlational patterns between each emotion, suppression, and reappraisal emotion regulation strategies, in line with previous studies [14, 17, 18, 72]. Reappraisal promotes better management of stressful consequences; thus, it can increase the likelihood of experiencing positive states. Cognitive reappraisal was positively associated with all positive emotion tendencies, thus supporting its main adaptive evolutionary function. Crucially, cognitive reappraisal strongly predicted Happiness, Awe, and Pride. This result is in line with the existing literature on Joy and positive emotions-emotion regulation strategies in general [9, 24, 73] and suggests that Italian people may prefer reappraising situations in terms of happiness, pride, and awe, which usually trigger a cognitive restructuring people’ actual worldview. Indeed, reappraisal is an effective strategy also used to decrease positive affect [74]. Therefore, participants could have activated this process also to down-regulate positive emotions, that is, to diminish their intensity. For instance, the high uncertainty related to awe-inspiring situations [75] could have prompted participants to diminish its intensity and turn it into something more manageable and acceptable.

On the other hand, suppression also intervenes in modulating the outcome of emotional response at the expressive level; thus, we assumed, again, a causal link from this strategy and each emotion. In line with the literature on positive affect [13, 15, 18, 73, 76], we also found that suppression predicted Happiness, Pride, Love, and Compassion negatively and significantly. However, amusement was positively, but not significantly, associated with suppression. Maybe because Amusement is associated with both adaptive and maladaptive humorous responses to stressful situations [7779]; thus, sometimes, Italian people may need to regulate it to fit the delicate demands of a social situation.

Conclusions

In this study, for the first time, the relationship between specific emotion dispositions and the two emotion regulation strategies of reappraisal and suppression was tested. Each positive emotion tendency correlated positively with reappraisal and negatively with suppression, except for Amusement, which showed a positive and even non-significant correlation. This supported the adaptive value of cognitive reappraisal in the emotion regulation process. We also conducted a cross-cultural validation of the Dispositional Positive Emotions Scale in an Italian sample. The results showed an excellent internal consistency of this scale as well as a convergent validity with the PANAS factors. All BFI factors were significant predictors of almost all discrete emotions. Although these findings are promising, future research should consider also other cultural and contextual-specific factors to deepen these aspects. For instance, it could be useful to focus on different religious Italian samples, since some emotions, such as awe or compassion, can be related to this aspect [80, 81]. Specifically, it would be interesting to elucidate the peculiar situational elicitors of cognitive reappraisal of positive emotions, to understand when people tend to reappraise a situation as happy and thus capture cultural variations of this positive emotions. Specifically, DPES cannot connect peculiar positive emotion elicitors with systematic ER strategies. This can be a useful aspect to be deepened by future studies, for instance, by integrating also the Ecological Momentary Assessment or daily diaries.

Moreover, the final Italian DPES structure did not match the original English factor structure. Future studies should corroborate our finding and it could be useful to add other items to DPES to differentiate between Joy and Contentment in the Italian population. Further, our sample composed of women for the most part. Therefore, future validation studies should investigate the role of gender in emotion disposition and regulation. Finally, adopting a finer granularity of emotion regulation strategies (e.g., different reappraisals strategies [82]) to manage positive emotions could provide useful information, especially about the understudied Italian population. In this regard, the main discrepancies with previous studies could be explained in terms of cultural variations. This observation may support the assumption that emotions can be conceived as containing prototypical elements (core themes), even though their peripheral aspects can vary according to the context [4, 83]. Therefore, the present cross-cultural validation study may allow capturing both core emotional themes, and their variations also related to other phenomena, in this case, personality traits. This result maybe relevant also in the clinical field, since it could help clinicians design tailored interventions for people suffering from personality or affective disorders, in relation to the emotion regulation strategies they usually adopt to manage positive emotions [e.g., 84, 85]. In brief, the Italian DPES is easy to administer and can provide quick information on frequently experienced emotions and their link with other emotional or cognitive processes across a wide array of domains.

Overall, the findings demonstrated specific and strong links between cognitive and emotional depositions and evidenced that Italian DPES has good validity and reliability [86, 87] and is suitable to be used with an Italian sample.

References

  1. 1. Izard C. Human Emotions Plenum Press. New York. 1977.
  2. 2. Lazarus RS, Smith CA. Knowledge and appraisal in the cognition—emotion relationship. Cognition & Emotion. 1988;2(4):281–300.
  3. 3. DeSteno D, Petty RE, Rucker DD, Wegener DT, Braverman J. Discrete emotions and persuasion: the role of emotion-induced expectancies. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2004;86(1):43. pmid:14717627
  4. 4. Ekman P. An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & emotion. 1992;6(3–4):169–200.
  5. 5. Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions: Oxford university press; 2004.
  6. 6. Tooby J, Cosmides L. The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. 2008. pmid:19255605
  7. 7. Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF. Handbook of emotions: Guilford Press; 2010.
  8. 8. Rivers SE, Brackett MA, Katulak NA, Salovey P. Regulating anger and sadness: An exploration of discrete emotions in emotion regulation. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2007;8(3):393–427.
  9. 9. Barrett LF, Gross J, Christensen TC, Benvenuto M. Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition & Emotion. 2001;15(6):713–24.
  10. 10. Mikolajczak M, Nelis D, Hansenne M, Quoidbach J. If you can regulate sadness, you can probably regulate shame: Associations between trait emotional intelligence, emotion regulation and coping efficiency across discrete emotions. Personality and individual differences. 2008;44(6):1356–68.
  11. 11. Fredrickson BL. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American psychologist. 2001;56(3):218. pmid:11315248
  12. 12. Caprara GV, Di Giunta L, Pastorelli C, Eisenberg N. Mastery of negative affect: A hierarchical model of emotional self-efficacy beliefs. Psychological Assessment. 2013;25(1):105. pmid:22731674
  13. 13. Gross JJ, Richards JM, John OP. Emotion regulation in everyday life. Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction and health. 2006;2006:13–35.
  14. 14. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2003;85(2):348. pmid:12916575
  15. 15. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Review of general psychology. 1998;2(3):271.
  16. 16. Cutuli D. Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies role in the emotion regulation: an overview on their modulatory effects and neural correlates. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2014;8:175. pmid:25285072
  17. 17. Egloff B, Schmukle SC, Burns LR, Schwerdtfeger A. Spontaneous emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: associations with negative affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion. 2006;6(3):356. pmid:16938078
  18. 18. Balzarotti S, John OP, Gross JJ. An Italian adaptation of the emotion regulation questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2010.
  19. 19. Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL. Regulation of positive emotions: Emotion regulation strategies that promote resilience. Journal of happiness studies. 2007;8(3):311–33.
  20. 20. Shiota MN, Keltner D, John OP. Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2006;1(2):61–71.
  21. 21. Nelson-Coffey SK, Ruberton PM, Chancellor J, Cornick JE, Blascovich J, Lyubomirsky S. The proximal experience of awe. PLOS ONE. 2019;14(5):e0216780. pmid:31121008
  22. 22. Stellar JE, Gordon AM, Piff PK, Cordaro D, Anderson CL, Bai Y, et al. Self-transcendent emotions and their social functions: Compassion, gratitude, and awe bind us to others through prosociality. Emotion Review. 2017:1754073916684557.
  23. 23. Goetz JL, Keltner D, Simon-Thomas E. Compassion: an evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological bulletin. 2010;136(3):351. pmid:20438142
  24. 24. Shiota MN, Campos B, Keltner D, Hertenstein MJ. Positive emotion and the regulation of interpersonal relationships. The regulation of emotion. 2004:127–55.
  25. 25. Cameron CD, Payne BK. Escaping affect: how motivated emotion regulation creates insensitivity to mass suffering. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2011;100(1):1. pmid:21219076
  26. 26. DeSteno D. Compassion and altruism: how our minds determine who is worthy of help. Current opinion in behavioral sciences. 2015;3:80–3.
  27. 27. Keltner D, Shiota MN. New displays and new emotions: A commentary on Rozin and Cohen (2003). 2003. pmid:12899320
  28. 28. Shiota MN, Neufeld SL, Yeung WH, Moser SE, Perea EF. Feeling good: autonomic nervous system responding in five positive emotions. Emotion. 2011;11(6):1368. pmid:22142210
  29. 29. Lench HC, Bench SW, Darbor KE, Moore M. A functionalist manifesto: goal-related emotions from an evolutionary perspective. Emotion Review. 2015;7(1):90–8.
  30. 30. Silvia PJ, Fayn K, Nusbaum EC, Beaty RE. Openness to experience and awe in response to nature and music: Personality and profound aesthetic experiences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 2015;9(4):376–84.
  31. 31. Güsewell A, Ruch W. Are only emotional strengths emotional? Character strengths and disposition to positive emotions. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being. 2012;4(2):218–39. pmid:26286979
  32. 32. Quoidbach J, Berry EV, Hansenne M, Mikolajczak M. Positive emotion regulation and well-being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening strategies. Personality and individual differences. 2010;49(5):368–73.
  33. 33. Shiota MN, Campos B, Oveis C, Hertenstein MJ, Simon-Thomas E, Keltner D. Beyond happiness: Building a science of discrete positive emotions. American Psychologist. 2017;72(7):617. pmid:29016167
  34. 34. Meadows CM. The phenomenology of joy, an empirical investigation. Psychological Reports. 1975;37(1):39–54. pmid:1153595
  35. 35. Fredrickson BL. What good are positive emotions? Review of general psychology. 1998;2(3):300. pmid:21850154
  36. 36. Izard CE. Emotions and facial expressions: A perspective from Differential Emotions Theory. The psychology of facial expression. 1997;2:57–77.
  37. 37. Fredrickson BL. Gratitude, like other positive emotions, broadens and builds. The psychology of gratitude. 2004;145:166.
  38. 38. Hutcherson CA, Gross JJ. The moral emotions: A social–functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2011;100(4):719. pmid:21280963
  39. 39. Tracy JL, Robins RW. Emerging insights into the nature and function of pride. Current directions in psychological science. 2007;16(3):147–50.
  40. 40. Ainsworth MDS. Attachment: Retrospect and prospect: basic books; 1982.
  41. 41. Bowlby J. The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. London (Tavistock) 1979. 1979.
  42. 42. Keltner D, Haidt J, Shiota MN. Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. Evolution and social psychology. 2006;115:142.
  43. 43. Estrada P. Adolescents’ self-report of prosocial responses to friends and acquaintances: The role of sympathy-related cognitive, affective, and motivational processes. Journal of research on adolescence. 1995;5(2):173–200.
  44. 44. Hildebrandt KA, Fitzgerald HE. The infant’s physical attractiveness: Its effect on bonding and attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1983;4(1):1–12.
  45. 45. Eisenberg N, Mussen PH. The roots of prosocial behavior in children: Cambridge University Press; 1989.
  46. 46. Piff PK, Dietze P, Feinberg M, Stancato DM, Keltner D. Awe, the small self, and prosocial behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2015;108(6):883. pmid:25984788
  47. 47. Keltner D, Haidt J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition & Emotion. 2003;17(2):297–314.
  48. 48. Prade C, Saroglou V. Awe’s effects on generosity and helping. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2016:1–9. pmid:26640507
  49. 49. Yaden DB, Haidt J, Jr. H, W. R, Vago DR, Newberg AB. The Varieties of Self-Transcendent Experience. Review of General Psychology. 2017.
  50. 50. Fredrickson BL. Positive emotions broaden and build. Advances in experimental social psychology. 2013;47(1):53.
  51. 51. Dixson DD, Anderson CL, Keltner D. Measuring positive emotions: An examination of the reliability and structural validity of scores on the seven dispositional positive emotions scales. Journal of Well-Being Assessment. 2018;2(2–3):115–33.
  52. 52. Campos JJ, Mumme D, Kermoian R, Campos RG. A functionalist perspective on the nature of emotion. Japanese Journal of Research on Emotions. 1994;2(1):1–20. pmid:7984165
  53. 53. Zimmermann P, Iwanski A. Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. International journal of behavioral development. 2014;38(2):182–94.
  54. 54. Endrerud MS, Vikan A. Five to seven year old children’s strategies for regulating anger, sadness, and fear. Nordic Psychology. 2007;59(2):127–34.
  55. 55. Waters SF, Thompson RA. Children’s perceptions of the effectiveness of strategies for regulating anger and sadness. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2014;38(2):174–81.
  56. 56. Schindler S, Querengässer J. Coping with sadness-How personality and emotion regulation strategies differentially predict the experience of induced emotions. Personality and Individual Differences. 2019;136:90–5.
  57. 57. Kafetsios K, Loumakou M. A comparative evaluation of the effects of trait emotional intelligence and emotion regulation on affect at work and job satisfaction. International Journal of Work Organisation and Emotion. 2007;2(1):71–87.
  58. 58. Megias-Robles A, Gutiérrez-Cobo MJ, Gómez-Leal R, Cabello R, Gross JJ, Fernández-Berrocal P. Emotionally intelligent people reappraise rather than suppress their emotions. PloS one. 2019;14(8):e0220688. pmid:31404096
  59. 59. Terraciano A, McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. Factorial and construct validity of the Italian Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2003;19(2):131. pmid:20467578
  60. 60. John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL. The big five inventory—versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research; 1991.
  61. 61. Ubbiali AC, Carlo; Hampton, Patricia; Deborah, Donati. Italian Big Five Inventory. Psychometric properties of the Italian adaptation of the Big Five Inventory (BFI). Applied Psychology Bulletin. 2013;59 (266):37.
  62. 62. Akın A, Akın U, Turan ME, Kaya Ç, Altundağ Y. The validity and reliability of the Turkish version of the dispositional positive emotion scales. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2014;152:78–81.
  63. 63. Borsa JC, Damásio BF, Bandeira DR. Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of psychological instruments: Some considerations. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto). 2012;22(53):423–32.
  64. 64. Child D. The essentials of factor analysis. London: Cassell Educational Limited; 1990.
  65. 65. Franklin SB, Gibson DJ, Robertson PA, Pohlmann JT, Fralish JS. Parallel analysis: a method for determining significant principal components. Journal of Vegetation Science. 1995;6(1):99–106.
  66. 66. Grazzani-Gavazzi I. The experience of emotions of interdependence and independence following interpersonal errors in Italy and Anglophone Canada. Cognition & Emotion. 1999;13(1):49–63.
  67. 67. Scherer KR, Wallbott HG, Summerfield AB. Experiencing emotion: A cross-cultural study: Cambridge University Press; 1986.
  68. 68. Zammuner L. Tecniche dell’intervista e del questionario, il Mulino, Bologna. Renato Votta HR Metrics. 1998.
  69. 69. McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. The NEO Personality Inventory: Using the five‐factor modeI in counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development. 1991;69(4):367–72.
  70. 70. Roberts BW, Jackson JJ, Fayard JV, Edmonds G, Meints J. Conscientiousness. 2009.
  71. 71. Engel G, Olson KR, Patrick C. The personality of love: Fundamental motives and traits related to components of love. Personality and Individual Differences. 2002;32(5):839–53.
  72. 72. Koole SL. The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and emotion: Psychology press; 2010. p. 138–77.
  73. 73. Haga SM, Kraft P, Corby E-K. Emotion regulation: Antecedents and well-being outcomes of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in cross-cultural samples. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2009;10(3):271–91.
  74. 74. Nezlek JB, Kuppens P. Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life. Journal of personality. 2008;76(3):561–80. pmid:18399953
  75. 75. Valdesolo P, Graham J. Awe, uncertainty, and agency detection (vol 25, pg 170, 2014). Psychological Science 2014;25(3):844–.
  76. 76. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(3):281–91. pmid:12212647
  77. 77. Morreall J, mit Inkongruenz S. John Morreall," Enjoying incongruity", Humor 2–1 (1989), 1–18.-zusammengefasst von Dr. Kareen Seidler John Morreall argumentiert, dass die menschliche Vernunft mit unserer Fähigkeit zusammenhängt, uns über Inkongruenzen zu freuen–und damit mit unserem Sinn für Humor. Tiere nehmen. Humor. 1989;2(1):1–18.
  78. 78. Herring DR, Burleson MH, Roberts NA, Devine MJ. Coherent with laughter: Subjective experience, behavior, and physiological responses during amusement and joy. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2011;79(2):211–8. pmid:21034782
  79. 79. Cann A, Collette C. Sense of humor, stable affect, and psychological well-being. Europe’s Journal of Psychology. 2014;10(3):464–79.
  80. 80. Krause N, Hayward RD. Assessing whether practical wisdom and awe of God are associated with life satisfaction. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. 2015;7(1):51.
  81. 81. Emmons RA. Emotion and religion. Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality. 2005:235–52.
  82. 82. Webb TL, Miles E, Sheeran P. Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological bulletin. 2012;138(4):775. pmid:22582737
  83. 83. Haidt J. The moral emotions. Handbook of affective sciences. 2003;11:852–70.
  84. 84. Malighetti C., Sciara S., Chirico A., & Riva G. Emotional Expression of# body on Instagram. Social Media+ Society, 2020; 6(2), 2056305120924771.
  85. 85. Malighetti, C., Chirico, A., Sciara, S., & Riva, G. (2019). # Eating disorders and Instagram: What emotions do you express? Annual Review of Cybertherapy and telemedicine
  86. 86. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistics notes: Cronbach’s alpha. Bmj. 1997;314(7080):572. pmid:9055718
  87. 87. DeVellis RF. Scale development: Theory and applications: Sage publications; 2016.