Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Neural correlates of the natural observation of an emotionally loaded video

  • Melanni Nanni ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Melanni Nanni, Joel Martínez-Soto

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Instituto de Neurobiología, Querétaro, México

  • Joel Martínez-Soto ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Melanni Nanni, Joel Martínez-Soto

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Instituto de Neurobiología, Querétaro, México, Department of Psychology, Universidad de Guanajuato, León, Guanajuato, México

  • Leopoldo Gonzalez-Santos,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Software, Visualization

    Affiliation Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Instituto de Neurobiología, Querétaro, México

  • Fernando A. Barrios

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    fbarrios@unam.mx

    Affiliation Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Instituto de Neurobiología, Querétaro, México

Abstract

Studies based on a paradigm of free or natural viewing have revealed characteristics that allow us to know how the brain processes stimuli within a natural environment. This method has been little used to study brain function. With a connectivity approach, we examine the processing of emotions using an exploratory method to analyze functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data. This research describes our approach to modeling stress paradigms suitable for neuroimaging environments. We showed a short film (4.54 minutes) with high negative emotional valence and high arousal content to 24 healthy male subjects (36.42 years old; SD = 12.14) during fMRI. Independent component analysis (ICA) was used to identify networks based on spatial statistical independence. Through this analysis we identified the sensorimotor system and its influence on the dorsal attention and default-mode networks, which in turn have reciprocal activity and modulate networks described as emotional.

Introduction

Understanding how the human brain processes information in a natural environment reveals information about its integral functioning during daily life. Under conditions simulating normal life certain areas of the brain function as networks participating in coding complex stimuli, such as natural vision [1]. This brain activity reflects characteristics similar to those evoked in the natural human environment. The vast majority of neuroimaging experiments to date sought to better understand the neural basis of perception using carefully controlled stimuli and tasks [1,2]. fMRI has been used to measure brain activity, primarily in the context of highly controlled experiments using purposefully design stimuli [3]. In these cases, researchers use pre-determined, static, and isolated-object images that are flashed on the screen in image acquisition synchronized paradigms [4]. Many contemporaneous experiments reduce the temporal complexity of their visual or auditory stimuli, presenting stimuli for one or two seconds or less. Yet, in order to sense and act in real-life circumstances, the brain must gather information over both long and short time intervals [5]. The use of precisely parameterized stimuli is critical for isolating the experimentally relevant dimensions from the extremely multidimensional natural stimuli.

Thus, in general, the world seen in the highly controlled fMRI experimental settings seems to bear little resemblance to our natural viewing experience [4,6]. Because many studies use simplified static stimuli, surprisingly little is known about how the human brain operates during real-world experiences. Exploring brain function with dynamic, naturalistic stimuli is important for several reasons. First, it is vital to determine whether results obtained in experiments using simplified stimuli hold true under natural conditions. Second, some research questions can only be addressed with naturalistic tasks where there is little temporal regularity [7]. For these reasons, using more natural stimuli helps to detect brain activation patterns that are difficult to observe using simple stimuli and enables us to study the human brain under ecologically valid, naturalistic stimulus and task conditions [1].

Naturalistic viewing paradigms (e.g. movies) have been used to study both stimulus-evoked BOLD signal changes and functional connectivity [8]. In order to preserve the dynamic sensory and affective qualities of real events, movies are used as tools closer to ecological vision in several studies [1,4,9,10].

The use of aversive cinematographic material has been reported as a means to induce brain related changes associated with acute stress [11,12]. Usually, the word stress is used to describe experiences that are challenging physiologically and emotionally [13]. Stress refers to a situation in which demands are perceived to exceed one’s personal resources. Different stressor types characterize different neurophysiological responses. For example, reactive stressors (such as pain) tend to implicate brainstem and specific hypothalamic nuclei, and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, which all have direct connections to the paraventricular nucleus [14]. On the other hand, anticipatory stressors (e.g. unfamiliar situations) seem to engage limbic system regions, namely the hippocampus, the amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex [14]. Physical stressors would engage more heavily the limbic system (amygdala), while psychological acute stressors would emphasize the hippocampus [15]. Acute stress has an important impact on higher-order cognitive function, whose effects are believed to result from stress-induced alterations of large-scale brain networks [12]. Studies about the impact of stress on resting state networks (RSNs) show that stressed participants exhibited greater activation in the RSNs -Default Mode, Ventral Attention, Dorsal Attention, Primary Visual, and Sensorimotor networks- than non-stressed participants (Soares, Sampaio, Ferreira, et al., 2013a; Soares, Sampaio, Marques, et al., 2012b). Also, participants in a stressful condition have displayed deficiencies in the deactivation of RSNs vs. non-stressed participants [16]. Similarly, brain connectivity during acute stress is related to alterations in brain areas related to perception, vigilance, and deactivation of the Default Mode Network (DMN), suggesting the promotion of focused attention that optimizes threat detection [17]. Hermans et al. 2011, investigated changes in brain interconnectivity after exposure to a fear related acute stressor (through a movie clip), resulting in noradrenergic neuromodulatory activity that leads to a relocation of neural responses related to attentional reorienting, vigilant perceptual intake, and autonomic control findings that agree with a causal link between the salience network and stress-induced noradrenergic activity.

In Independent Component Analysis (ICA), the BOLD signal is treated as a mixed signal that is mathematically divided into several, statistically separate signals known as independent components. These independent components can then be related back to the stimuli to understand the relationship between brain activity and stimuli [7]. In fMRI studies, ICA has been used to extract networks of brain activity during resting state studies. In the field of emotions, functional connectivity has been used to estimate the stress on the connectivity networks [1]. There are few studies in which ICA has been used to estimate the neural networks in the course of emotion processing [1821]. Also, ICA has been applied to analyze fMRI data collected during movie watching [9,10]. Specifically, there is a lack of studies that describe the connectivity networks associated with resting state conditions under a stressful or emotionally disturbing setting [22,23]. Through this perspective it is proposed that the connectivity patterns evaluated reflect brain-environment interactions rather than responses to stimuli [24] as observed in fMRI experiments on stress processing. Therefore, it is possible to consider the ICA approach as a valuable tool for revealing novel information about functional brain connectivity related with the stress processing phenomena.

Given the nature of the ICA approach, the present work proposes studying the neuronal bases of the emotional processes in natural and free conditions without establishing a controlled variable a priori. The aim is to validate an induced-stress method using functional neuroimaging paradigms. We propose an ICA connectivity approach to characterize the brain networks implicated during the view of a stress-inducing video in order to explore the brain networks implicated during the perception of an emotionally aversive condition. We also aim to identify the main connectivity networks and to elucidate their contrasts and the modulation that takes place during aversive emotional processing during a free-viewing paradigm. We approached this question by studying the functional organization of the human cortex when free-viewing a continuous sequence (4.54 minutes) taken from an original film. We reasoned that such rich and complex visual stimuli are much closer to an ecological vision than the controlled stimuli usually used in fMRI. Using behavioral tests, we also evaluated whether the video observation was a valid model for inducing psychological stress within an fMRI environment [25]. This video clip has been used in previous studies to induce acute psychological stress [2628]. Likewise, the present stress induction method is consistent with the exogenous determinants of human stress response: unpredictability, uncontrollability and novelty [29]. Therefore, in the present study we also link the behavioral responses (perceived stress) derived from specific parts of said clip with increase connectivity in specific brain structures.

Results

Behavioral data

The results show the statistically significant differences in the groups before (t1) and after watching the stressful video (t2). The scores (Mdn = 2.10) at t2 are those with the highest stress subscale intensity (Z = -2.89, p = .00) when compared with t1 scores (Mdn = 1.60); this indicates that the stress-induction manipulation was successful. No statistically significant differences in perceived stress were found in the group control before (t1; Mdn = 1.55) and after seeing the movie´s segments (t2; Mdn = 1.66) Z = -0.56, p = .96 indicating no influence of the control stimulus in the stress levels.

fMRI ICA data

An estimated total of 31 components were observed by ICA (31 spatial maps with their corresponding time course) and ordered by the extent to which they explain the total variation in data. Five components were excluded (C17, C23, C24, C28 and C29) because they contained elements characteristic of a typical artifact [4]. The explained variance percentage value for the first component was around 6%, and it decays for later components, with the value for the last component being 1.13%. On the other hand, in the average power spectrum of the time course of all the components, a predominant frequency was identified at 0.05 Hz, indicating the reliability of the components [30].

The common components resulting of the join activity show the variety of relative responses in the whole session/subject analysis. The components have been ordered according to the mean response per decreasing component, without considering artifact components (17, 23, 24, 28 and 29), and all adjustments were significant (p < 0.00) for all except 30 (p < 0.00278) and 31 (p < 0.00268); the significance test was not corrected. Therefore, the trustworthiness of each component is known with regard to its representation of the joint activity.

Network dynamics

A matrix correlation was built from time courses of every component (27) (Fig 1). We took the components whose most correlations were significant among them p<0.002 (of a 0.05 Bonferroni corrected for 25 pairwise comparisons) for further analysis. These represent diverse networks. C1, C2, C3 and C5 were identified as the same network (dorsal attention network), by having a similar hemodynamic pattern in their spatial courses and having this high correlation. These four signals were averaged, obtaining a unique network for further analysis (Fig 2). Networks previously described as resting state [31], as sensorimotor (SMN), default mode (DMN) and dorsal attention network (DAN) (Figs 2 and 3) were identified. The networks represented by these components are shown in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Correlation matrix of the BOLD signal.

Correlation matrix of the BOLD signal time courses for the 26 ICA components. The matrix elements (I, j) represent the Pearson’s coefficient resulting from the cross correlation of the BOLD signal time course between the i and j components. The axes represent each of the component indices, the color scale shows the Pearson’s value. Associated network activity is depicted in Fig 2. Outline box contains the components which most of their correlation were significant (p<0.002 corrected) taken for further analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Brain networks included in the correlation matrix.

Some of the networks taken from the correlation matrix in Fig 1 (outlined box) based on its order: A) C1, C2, C3 and C5; Dorsal Attention Network (DAN). B) C14; Sensory motor Network (SMN). C) C6; Default Mode Network (DMN). D) C7 posterior cingulate gyrus, lingual gyrus, precuneus. E) C12 Medial frontal gyrus, precuneus, medial temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus. F) C20 Lingual gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, postcentral gyrus. G) C8 cerebellum anterior right lobe VIII-B area, angular bilateral gyrus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. BOLD signal components with signitican correlations.

Shown are components with significant correlation (see Fig 1). Average signals (black) obtained from BOLD signals. (group A mean = 0.1060 SD = 2.3151; group B mean = 0.1122 SD = 1.9606) a) Components 12 and 20. b) Components 7, 8, 10 and 14.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g003

We took the DMN and the DAN as time series references, since their activity has been better characterized in fMRI studies [3236]. For demonstration purposes, two groups were formed based on their time courses. One group, comprising C12 and C20 (group A) has reciprocal activity to DMN. The other group, formed by C7, C8, C10 and C14, (group B), has activity reciprocal to DAN (Fig 3). The time courses of each group were averaged, resulting in two unique signals (group A mean = 0.1060 SD = 2.3151; group B mean = 0.1122 SD = 1.9606). By splicing these signals, a remarkable negative correlation between them was observed (r = -0.8452, p< 0.01) (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Plots of time signal components average.

Two signal examples. The red line represents the average of component time courses 12 and 20 group A; the blue line of components 7, 8, 10 and 14, group B. The axes indicate the normalized BOLD signal (X) over time (Y).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g004

As we know, changes in these time courses depend on the video display; thus, we asked what movie frames elicited the highest activation in these two signals. Fig 5 shows the movie frames from the highest activation peaks. The frames were ordered according to descending signal amplitude. Group a) was triggered mainly by scenes from the time prior to hurting the animal, whereas group b) was activated by scenes where the animal was dying.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Types of scenes corresponding to high BOLD activity.

A) Scenes corresponding to the highest BOLD signal peaks, from group A (C12 and C20 signal); these images are related to the slaughtering actions and are images where the animals are prepared to be hurt. B) Images from group B (C7, C8, C10 and C14 signal) while the animal is hurting (in the dying process). Note that due to copyright reasons, the images showed in this figure are not the actual images used in the study. However, all the images used in this figure represent similar situations observed in the original video. All the images used in this figure were constructed from images acquired and processed in our laboratory.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g005

Intersubject correlation analysis (ISC) revealed that the high correlation is mostly present in posterior areas, mainly the precuneus surface cortex, fusiform gyrus, followed by the lateral occipital areas, lingual gyrus, as well as parts of the dorsal limbic system (Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Areas with higher correlation.

Areas with higher correlation between subjects during the visualization of the video. Precuneus surface cortex, fusiform gyrus, lateral left occipital areas, lingual gyrus, and posterior cingulate cortex.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g006

We can observe two clusters with higher correlation, the precuneus cortex and the lingual gyrus. That means the activity in those areas was present in a most synchronized manner in all subjects across the time. Comparing with ICA results the lingual gyrus appears in three of the networks (components 7, 10 and 20). Precuneus is part of component 7, also an important part of the DMN, together with the posterior cingulate cortex, involved in visual, sensorimotor, and attentional information.

Cross correlation

The results of cross correlation analysis were significant in most of comparisons p<0.0125 (of a 0.05 Bonferroni corrected for 8 pairwise comparisons) with estimation error bounds 2.7 standard deviations from 0, (Fig 7), show a tendency for most of the networks to be one lag against the other. As it shows DMN vs C10, SMN vs DAN, DAN vs C12 and C7 vs C8, their highest value is in lag 0, however the next value is in lag -1 which indicates a tendency to be ahead, i.e. DMN tends to be ahead C10 but not in a fully way. Standing out among these findings is the SMN, which predict directly the group of networks on the left side (DMN), and in an inverse way those in the right column (DAN) (Fig 8). These two networks positively predict other networks, whose areas include posterior cingulate gyrus, lingual gyrus and precuneus; b) anterior cingulate cortex, lingual gyrus, postcentral gyrus and posterior insula; and c) SMN, whose connectivity changed immediately after the animal-slaughtering scenes were shown. On the other hand, areas of networks with increased activity during scenes that predated the time of death were a) medial frontal gyrus, precuneus, medial temporal gyrus and fusiform gyrus; and b) lingual gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and postcentral gyrus.

thumbnail
Fig 7. Highest values of cross correlation analyses.

Results from cross correlation analysis showing the highest values for each comparison. For some (DMN-C10, SMN-DAN, DAN-C12, C7-C8) it shows also the next to the highest values. Eight comparisons, from which seven were significant (p<0.0125 corrected), the red lines correspond to the upper and lower confidence bounds.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. Network organization in temporal delay and correlation.

Networks organized based on the temporary delay and correlation (positive or negative) that exist amongst them, according to Fig 7. They are set out in levels according to their activation point in time.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198731.g008

Discussion

The present study was designed to locate the main networks, their dynamics, and the modulation that takes place in aversive emotional processing using model free neuroimaging analysis [11]. Using ICA we were able to document the dynamics that exist between the BOLD activity of each network detected while a stressful video was being viewed. What stood out the most in this study was the identification of the DAN and the DMN. Our findings are consistent with previous research, especially the determination that these networks are intrinsically inversely correlated [37,38]. Furthermore, the signal of each network is also associated with the reciprocal activity of other networks among the six networks whose areas have been associated to emotional processing. Based on the correlation of their time courses, these six networks formed two subgroups, where the average signal of each subgroup had a high negative correlation value (r = -0.8452). The first subgroup, made up of components 12 and 20, contained networks that involve regions, such as the medial frontal gyrus, precuneus, middle temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, lingual gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, and postcentral gyrus, that have reciprocal activity with the DAN and are observed in moments of preparation before killing an animal. This response coincides with processes associated with this specific network, which include expectation and fixed attention when knowing that something is going to happen soon, marking a striking difference from other processes related to attention [39]. This activity begins to decline the moment the expected event occurs. From this point on, activity begins for the following group of networks. The ICA components 7, 8, 10, and 14 include regions 7 (posterior cingulate, lingual gyrus, and precuneus), 8 (right frontal lobe of the cerebellum area III-B and bilateral angular gyrus), 10 (anterior cingulate, lingual gyrus, post-central gyrus and posterior insula), and 14 (sensorimotor system, pre- and post-central gyrus). The corresponding BOLD activity involves the DMN. Despite the fact that the DMN is characterized as being present mostly during a resting state, its activity also persists during states of sensory processing that impose a minimum cognitive demand [40,41]. A modular focus of the brain tells us about its capacity to assemble quickly and robustly, in order to segregate an infinity of processes. This idea has been developed more tangibly using computer mechanisms that demonstrate its high efficiency and functioning by modulating multiple processes of complex information and developing tasks in a changing environment [42]. The main networks for the modular control on a resting state brain, have been demonstrated before and include the occipital, sensorimotor, and default mode networks [43,44]. These same networks were found at the hierarchical level of modulation [45], whereas at the voxel level researchers found visual, auditory, and default mode networks. As was made visible in our results of crossed correlation, the SMN precedes both of these groups and its activation in the absence of pain or physical sensation is associated with the perception of others’ pain, as suggested by other studies [46]. At the moment when the before-injury scene starts in the movie, DAN is fully activated, at the injury scene moment SMN begins to activate and DAN begins to deactivate. Afterwards, this same network (SMN) gives way to the DMN (after injury scene), which modulates other networks with areas such as the lingual gyrus, which has numerous connections with the amygdala, central lobe, (associated with the perception of unpleasant stimuli), the anterior and posterior cingulated cortex, part of the limbic system, cerebellum, postcentral gyrus, and angular gyrus.

The basic brain processes are the result of a series of events that occur in certain pathways through in the brain. For a stimuli processing in a complex environment, which combine top-down / bottom-up process, is necessary a network which emphasizes the behavior of a complex system probably shaped by the interactions between its constituents [47]. We think this is the first description of a path that operates in a complex network environment in response of complex stimuli. The present study also showed that the same modulating networks are maintained in both a stable state [44] and a stimulus-triggered altered state, leading to the possibility that these networks act as part of the general modulation of brain activity, regardless of the stimulus or process. However, this conclusion must be confirmed by studies made under the same model with different types of stimuli that provoke responses related to other cognitive processes. There are some studies with positive valence movies used in naturalistic behavioral paradigms [48]. However, the studies of processing of stress during free movie viewing are scarce [11]; movie film with interpersonal violence evaluated with neuroendocrine and affective measures of affect; with Salience Network related activation).

Here we have chosen a negative valence movie in order to create an ecological valid model for studying some neurocognitive process elicited by an acutely stressful experience [29]. Some alternative explanation for the amygdala activation absence could be considered, including: a) the variability of type of stressors and its engaging physiological and psychological activity [14]. Psychological acute stressors show high variability on the physiological systems [15,49]. b) psychological stress commonly involves ambivalent signals that require considerable integration and evaluation before a response stress has been started [15]. Since there is not much previous evidence of emotional studies with a free viewing stimulation, some of the results presented here lead to interpretations and questions of brain activity. One of them is the absence of amygdala activity in spatial maps, since its activity has not been reported in the analysis of long time windows under emotional stimuli, it can be considered the fact that there is no significant sustained temporal activity of it over other areas / networks, when viewed in a longer time window than those commonly used in fMRI block studies. This evidence has been proven only with dynamic stimuli [50,51]. We have explored the human brain’s response under stress induced by a dynamic naturalistic stimulus. Our technical and methodological approach differs from previous studies of psychological and physical stress paradigms suitable for neuroimaging environments [52,53]. Further, using more natural stimuli will probably allow us to detect patterns of brain activation that are difficult to observe using simple stimuli, and enable us to study a “stressed human brain” under ecologically valid naturalistic stimulus and task conditions [1]. Future studies would consider additional measures (physiological) of stress together with this method.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-four right-handed healthy male subjects were enrolled in the study. All subjects gave their written informed consent, and the experimental protocol was approved by the Bioethics Review Board of the Institute of Neurobiology, UNAM, and was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Subjects had an average age of 36.42 (SD 12.14) and a minimum of 12 years of schooling. All subjects were mid-to-high socioeconomic level. Individuals who met any of the following criteria were excluded: history of head injury, treatment with psychotropic medications, use of narcotics, steroids, or any other medication that affects the central nervous or endocrine systems, having had a medical illness within 3 weeks prior to testing, self-reported substance use or mental disorders (SCL R-90 [54]), daily tobacco use, regular nightshift work, current stressful episode or major life event, and regularly viewing extremely violent movies or playing violent computer games. One subject was excluded during the analysis due to numerous motion artifacts. Women were excluded from this study since the hormonal cycle may influence emotional responses and may cause a bias in the study [55].

Stimuli and procedure

Prior to entering the scanner, subjects received thorough instructions about the scanning procedure and the tasks to perform. Perceived stress (aversive stimuli) was tested immediately after entering the scanner to obtain a baseline level (t1) and again after viewing 4.54 minutes of the stressful movie (t2). To show that the results are specific to our particular experimental condition and do not merely reflect generic modulations in the audio-visual stimulation, a control experiment was carried out with a comparable set of "neutral" movie segments in an independent group of subjects.

Psychological measures

Perceived stress.

The computerized version of the Stress and Activation Adjectives Checklist-SAACH [56] was employed, considering only the stress dimension (11 adjectives with four points answer format: absolutely true, probably true, not sure and absolutely not). In the present study the scale was adapted to be administered in the scanner environment. To respond to the test, subjects had a magnetic resonance (MR) compatible ResponseGrip (NordicNeuroLab) with their left and right index fingers and thumbs each placed on one of four buttons. All sentences and instructions were presented on a graphical interface programed in house where participants were instructed to rate their present emotional state, all the items presented were randomized. The sentences were printed in lower case letters, displayed in approximately 18-mm tall Arial, white on a black background, providing high legibility. Prior to the scanning session, each subject was instructed in the performance of SAACH and the use of the computer mouse by practicing a shortened version outside of the scanning environment on a desktop computer with verbal instruction from the researcher.

Aversive stimuli

We use fMRI coupled with the eye tracking technology to confirm attentive viewing of all projected stimulus and movie fragments as suggested by other studies [5759], where it is recognized that the monitoring of eye movements during fMRI is something essential in research on natural stimulation paradigms [60]. The eye tracker was calibrated for each participant before the experiments began. A MR-compatible NNL eye-tracking camera was used to record video data of the subject’s eyes during the fMRI task-related scanning (Visual System NNL EyeTracking Camera, and ViewPoint eye tracker software, Arrington Research Inc., Scottsdale AZ). Subjects watched 4 min and 54 sec. of the movie “Faces of Death #1” (dir. John Alan Schwartz, 1978, original length 105 min) in the fMRI scanner. Three short movie fragments were used to create the proper context (1 × 60s, 1 × 120s, 1 × 114s). This shorter version was re-edited with computer software. Selected fragments were comparable in amount of speech, human presence, luminance, and language. The aversive movie clips contained scenes of a woman on a farm decapitating a rooster, images of a slaughterhouse where sheep and cattle are being sacrificed in a brutal manner and video fragments of a group of actors pretending to eat the brain of a monkey that they had to kill themselves. Participants were instructed to carefully watch the video and feel free to stop the video if the images were disturbing them. Participants were informed before the experiment that watching the film might be stressful and that they could terminate the experiment at any point. The video clip image contents were classified with negative valence and high arousal content according to Bradley and Lang [61].

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

During projection of the video, the sequence of images was captured with a 3.0 Tesla Discovery MR750 MRI unit, using the 32-channel reel for cranium, at the Neurobiology Institute of the UNAM. The functional images were acquired with a sequence of Eco Planar (EPI) pulses for heavy images at T2*, GE-EPI of TR / TE = 2000 / 40 ms in a 64 × 64 matrix over a FOV of 25.6 cm in 36 slices with a width of 4 mm per slice. This resulted in isometric voxels with a spatial resolution of 4 × 4 × 4 mm3. The high-resolution structural images were acquired using a T1 weighted SPGR pulse sequence with 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 of spatial resolution.

Data analysis

fMRI data.

All data were transferred to offline work stations to convert them from DICOM to NIfTI format (dcim2nii, Ch. Rorden, http://www.nitrc.org/projects/dcm2nii); afterwards they were processed using the MELODIC ICA module of the FSL program, which runs an algorithm of independent components per voxel in the stack of images for the whole group of subjects. All the functional studies were processed using the typical pre-statistics pipeline that includes timing correction for interleaved slice acquisition, for motion correction, normalization to standard space MNI with a 12 degrees linear transformation, resampled to 2mm spatial resolution, finally a smoothing of 6mm Gauss kernel at FWHM. The statistical analysis was estimated in to steps, single- and multi-session. The first analysis was made using the Single-session ICA mode that makes up a standard of each entrance file (23 subjects) to a two-dimensional matrix (time and space) in order to decompose them later into two-dimensional matrixes (time and components, space and components). The result estimates the independent components for each subject without taking others into account. The results of the “single session” were used to identify the components that corresponded to each subject’s noise, and these were eliminated (between four and five per subject), thus arriving at a more reliable database [30]. For the second level of analysis in MELODIC, “Multi-session Tensor-ICA” was used, which takes the entry data as a three-dimensional matrix (time, space, and subjects) and breaks it down into triplet, two-dimensional matrixes: time courses-ICA components and spatial maps-ICA components. The final observable product describes common components in all or most of the subjects and orders them according to the highest percentage of variation explained by the model, optimized by a distribution of non-Gaussian spatial sources, using fixed point iteration. The resulting components went through Pearson correlation tests and linear regressions were calculated; to determine the network dynamics and the modulation between networks [62], linear regression was used to measure the strength and direction of the linear relationship between two joint signals [63]. All these analyses were processed using our own written programs in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA).

We used Inter-subject correlation analysis, which is highly comparable with GLM [64]. ISC analysis allows visualizing selective and time-locked activity across a wide network of brain areas, under some natural stimuli, comparing the whole neural response across all subjects, without a model [65]. We used the same data of the 23 subjects, normalized into an MNI coordinate system, spatially smoothed and realigned. The ISC analysis was performed using ISCtoolbox [66] (http://code.google.com/p/isc-toolbox/). Since ICA is a method that has been used mainly in blind signal separation unrelated to an external stimulus, in order to study the neural correlates of the extrinsic stimulus-evoked component of the brain activity, ISC was used to complement the analysis. For instance, inter-subject correlation has been proven effective in localizing cortical regions activated across subjects during free-viewing of movies [64], which in this study showed high consistency with our ICA results.

Cross correlation.

The results of cross correlation were significant in most of comparisons p< 0.0125 (of a 0.05 Bonferroni corrected for 8 pairwise comparisons) with estimation error bounds 2.7 standard deviations from 0 (Fig 7), show a tendency for most of the networks to be one lag against the other. As it shows DMN vs C10, SMN vs DAN, DAN vs C12 and C7 vs C8, their highest value is in lag 0, however the next value is in lag -1 which indicates a tendency to be ahead, i.e. DMN tends to be ahead C10 but not in a fully way. Standing out among these findings is the SMN, which predict directly the group of networks on the left side (DMN), and in an inversely way those in the right column (DAN) (Fig 8). These two networks positively predict other networks, whose areas include posterior cingulate gyrus, lingual gyrus and precuneus; b) anterior cingulate cortex, lingual gyrus, postcentral gyrus and posterior insula; and c) SMN, whose connectivity changed immediately after the animal-slaughtering scenes were shown. On the other hand, areas of networks with increased activity during scenes that predated the time of death were a) medial frontal gyrus, precuneus, medial temporal gyrus and fusiform gyrus; and b) lingual gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and postcentral gyrus.

Behavioral data.

To test whether the video caused aversive psychological effects, behavioral data was analyzed with the Wilcoxon signed-rank test comparing stress measures between t1 and t2.

Neutral movie condition

Participants.

The participants were 31 students (21 women, 10 men; M age = 20.90 yr., SD = 5.32) from the Psychology Department of Universidad de Guanajuato, who gave their informed consent to being part of the study. One participant lacking complete data was omitted (final n = 32).

Psychological measures.

Perceived stress was measured with the pencil-paper version of the Stress and Activation Adjectives Checklist.

Neutral stimuli.

The subjects watched 17 neutral fragments of the same movie projected in the previous study with 4 min and 53 sec length (1–17 x 16 s). This neutral version of the movie was re-edited and implemented thorough Java software. The neutral movie clips contained colored emotionally neutral scenes (e.g. hands, one house, sheep and cows in their ordinary context and scenes of people in abstract or trivial circumstances).

Procedure.

Perceived stress was tested before (t1) and after viewing (t2) the neutral fragments. All video stimuli were presented via a video projection system in a semi-darkened experimental room isolated from noise and distractions. This system projected a 1.2 x 1.8 image onto a reflexive white screen. For all experimental stimuli, the picture took up 80% of the screen on a black background. The stimuli were evaluated in one single experimental session and 15 minutes of length.

Data analysis.

Behavioral data for the neutral movie condition was analyzed with the Wilcoxon signed-rank test comparing stress measures between t1 and t2, same as in the fMRI movie condition.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Programa de Maestría en Ciencias (Neurobiología) of the Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México (UNAM) for the masters fellowship (MN) supported by the National Council of Science and Technology, Mexico (Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología, CONACYT); DGAPA-UNAM for the Postdoctoral Fellowship (JM-S); the PASPA program at DGAPA-UNAM for the sabbatical fellowship (FAB); and CONACyT for the funding received via grant CB167271 (FAB). We are also grateful to Dr. Erick H. Pasaye for technical support and J. González-Norris and Dr. M.C. Jeziorski for their revision of the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Lahnakoski JM, Salmi J, Jääskeläinen IP, Lampinen J, Glerean E, Tikka P, et al. Stimulus-related independent component and voxel-wise analysis of human brain activity during free viewing of a feature film. PLoS ONE. Public Library of Science; 2012;7: e35215. pmid:22496909
  2. 2. Schultz J, Pilz KS. Natural facial motion enhances cortical responses to faces. Exp Brain Res. Springer-Verlag; 2009;194: 465–475. pmid:19205678
  3. 3. Hasson U, Landesman O, Knappmeyer B, Vallines I, Rubin N, Heeger DJ. Neurocinematics: The Neuroscience of Film. Projections. Berghahn Journals; 2008;2: 1–26.
  4. 4. Hasson U, Malach R. Human brain activation during viewing of dynamic natural scenes. Novartis Found Symp. 2006;270: 203–12– discussion 212–6–232–7. pmid:16649716
  5. 5. Hasson U, Honey CJ. Future trends in Neuroimaging: Neural processes as expressed within real-life contexts. NeuroImage. 2012;62: 1272–1278. pmid:22348879
  6. 6. David SV, Mesgarani N, Fritz JB, Shamma SA. Rapid synaptic depression explains nonlinear modulation of spectro-temporal tuning in primary auditory cortex by natural stimuli. J Neurosci. Society for Neuroscience; 2009;29: 3374–3386. pmid:19295144
  7. 7. Spiers HJ, Maguire EA. Decoding human brain activity during real-world experiences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11: 356–365. pmid:17618161
  8. 8. Vanderwal T, Eilbott J, Finn ES, Craddock RC, Turnbull A, Castellanos FX. Individual differences in functional connectivity during naturalistic viewing conditions. NeuroImage. 2017;157: 521–530. pmid:28625875
  9. 9. Malinen S, Hlushchuk Y, Hari R. Towards natural stimulation in fMRI—Issues of data analysis. NeuroImage. 2007;35: 131–139. pmid:17208459
  10. 10. Bartels A, Zeki S. Brain dynamics during natural viewing conditions—a new guide for mapping connectivity in vivo. NeuroImage. 2005;24: 339–349. pmid:15627577
  11. 11. Hermans EJ, van Marle HJF, Ossewaarde L, Henckens MJAG, Qin S, van Kesteren MTR, et al. Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neural network reconfiguration. Science. American Association for the Advancement of Science; 2011;334: 1151–1153. pmid:22116887
  12. 12. Qin S, Cousijn H, Rijpkema M, Luo J, Franke B, Hermans EJ, et al. The effect of moderate acute psychological stress on working memory-related neural activity is modulated by a genetic variation in catecholaminergic function in humans. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012;6: 16. pmid:22593737
  13. 13. McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Reviews. American Physiological Society; 2007;87: 873–904. pmid:17615391
  14. 14. Herman JP, Figueiredo H, Mueller NK, Ulrich-Lai Y, Ostrander MM, Choi DC, et al. Central mechanisms of stress integration: hierarchical circuitry controlling hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2003;24: 151–180. pmid:14596810
  15. 15. Pruessner JC, Dedovic K, Khalili-Mahani N, Engert V, Pruessner M, Buss C, et al. Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;63: 234–240. pmid:17686466
  16. 16. Soares JM, Sampaio A, Ferreira LM, Santos NC, Marques P, Marques F, et al. Stress Impact on Resting State Brain Networks. Zhan W, editor. PLoS ONE. Public Library of Science; 2013;8: e66500. pmid:23840493
  17. 17. Quadflieg S, Gentile F, Rossion B. The neural basis of perceiving person interactions. Cortex. 2015;70: 5–20. pmid:25697049
  18. 18. Romero-Rebollar C, Jiménez-Ángeles L, Dragustinovis-Ruiz EA, Medina-Bañuelos V. Neural Modulation in Aversive Emotion Processing: An Independent Component Analysis Study. Comput Math Methods Med. Hindawi; 2016;2016: 2816567–10. pmid:27579051
  19. 19. Escartí MJ, la Iglesia-Vayá de M, Martí-Bonmatí L, Robles M, Carbonell J, Lull JJ, et al. Increased amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus activation in schizophrenic patients with auditory hallucinations: an fMRI study using independent component analysis. Schizophrenia Research. 2010;117: 31–41. pmid:20071145
  20. 20. Cullen KR, Vizueta N, Thomas KM, Han GJ, Lim KO, Camchong J, et al. Amygdala functional connectivity in young women with borderline personality disorder. Brain Connectivity. 2011;1: 61–71. pmid:22432955
  21. 21. Broicher SD, Frings L, Huppertz H-J, Grunwald T, Kurthen M, Krämer G, et al. Alterations in functional connectivity of the amygdala in unilateral mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol. Springer-Verlag; 2012;259: 2546–2554. pmid:22688567
  22. 22. Cisler JM, Scott Steele J, Smitherman S, Lenow JK, Kilts CD. Neural processing correlates of assaultive violence exposure and PTSD symptoms during implicit threat processing: a network-level analysis among adolescent girls. Psychiatry Res. 2013;214: 238–246. pmid:23969000
  23. 23. Shi H, Wang X, Yi J, Zhu X, Zhang X, Yang J, et al. Default mode network alterations during implicit emotional faces processing in first-episode, treatment-naive major depression patients. Front Psychol. Frontiers; 2015;6: 1198. pmid:26322003
  24. 24. Mathiak K, Weber R. Toward brain correlates of natural behavior: fMRI during violent video games. Hum Brain Mapp. Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company; 2006;27: 948–956. pmid:16628606
  25. 25. Dedovic K, Rexroth M, Wolff E, Duchesne A, Scherling C, Beaudry T, et al. Neural correlates of processing stressful information: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Research. 2009;1293: 49–60. pmid:19555674
  26. 26. Gerrards-Hesse A, Spies K, Hesse FW. Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. British Journal of Psychology. Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 2011;85: 55–78.
  27. 27. Brand N, Verspui L, Oving A. Induced mood and selective attention. Perceptual and motor skills. 1997;84: 455–463. pmid:9106834
  28. 28. van den Berg AE, Koole SL, van der Wulp NY. Environmental preference and restoration: (How) are they related? Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2003;23: 135–146.
  29. 29. Mason JW. A review of psychoendocrine research on the pituitary-adrenal cortical system. Psychosom Med. 1968;30: Suppl:576–607.
  30. 30. Tohka J, Foerde K, Aron AR, Tom SM, Toga AW, Poldrack RA. Automatic independent component labeling for artifact removal in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2008;39: 1227–1245. pmid:18042495
  31. 31. Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2005;360: 1001–1013. pmid:16087444
  32. 32. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2003;100: 253–258. pmid:12506194
  33. 33. Greicius MD, Menon V. Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16: 1484–1492. pmid:15601513
  34. 34. Uddin LQ, Clare Kelly AM, Biswal BB, Xavier Castellanos F, Milham MP. Functional connectivity of default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;30: 625–637. pmid:18219617
  35. 35. Ptak R, Schnider A. The Dorsal Attention Network Mediates Orienting toward Behaviorally Relevant Stimuli in Spatial Neglect. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30: 12557–12565. pmid:20861361
  36. 36. Vossel S, Weidner R, Driver J, Friston KJ, Fink GR. Deconstructing the architecture of dorsal and ventral attention systems with dynamic causal modeling. J Neurosci. 2012;32: 10637–10648. pmid:22855813
  37. 37. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. National Acad Sciences; 2005;102: 9673–9678. pmid:15976020
  38. 38. Fransson P. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum Brain Mapp. Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company; 2005;26: 15–29. pmid:15852468
  39. 39. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL. Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3: 292–297. pmid:10700263
  40. 40. Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, et al. Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997;9: 648–663. pmid:23965122
  41. 41. Mazoyer B, Zago L, Mellet E, Bricogne S, Etard O, Houdé O, et al. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54: 287–298. pmid:11287133
  42. 42. Meunier D, Lambiotte R, Bullmore ET. Modular and hierarchically modular organization of brain networks. Front Neurosci. 2010;4: 200. pmid:21151783
  43. 43. He Y, Wang J, Wang L, Chen ZJ, Yan C, Yang H, et al. Uncovering Intrinsic Modular Organization of Spontaneous Brain Activity in Humans. Sporns O, editor. PLoS ONE. 2009;4: e5226–18. pmid:19381298
  44. 44. Meunier D, Achard S, Morcom A, Bullmore E. Age-related changes in modular organization of human brain functional networks. NeuroImage. 2009;44: 715–723. pmid:19027073
  45. 45. Meunier D, Lambiotte R, Fornito A, Ersche KD, Bullmore ET. Hierarchical modularity in human brain functional networks. Front Neuroinform. 2009;3: 37. pmid:19949480
  46. 46. Morrison I, Tipper SP, Fenton-Adams WL, Bach P. “Feeling” others’ painful actions: the sensorimotor integration of pain and action information. Hum Brain Mapp. Wiley-Blackwell; 2013;34: 1982–1998. pmid:22451259
  47. 47. Newman MEJ. The Structure and Function of Complex Networks. SIAM Review. Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics; 2006;45: 167–256.
  48. 48. Nguyen VT, Breakspear M, Hu X, Guo CC. The integration of the internal and external milieu in the insula during dynamic emotional experiences. NeuroImage. 2016;124: 455–463. pmid:26375211
  49. 49. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull. 2004;130: 355–391. pmid:15122924
  50. 50. van der Gaag C, Minderaa RB, Keysers C. The BOLD signal in the amygdala does not differentiate between dynamic facial expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2007;2: 93–103. pmid:18985128
  51. 51. Straube T, Preissler S, Lipka J, Hewig J, Mentzel H-J, Miltner WHR. Neural representation of anxiety and personality during exposure to anxiety-provoking and neutral scenes from scary movies. Hum Brain Mapp. Wiley-Blackwell; 2010;31: 36–47. pmid:19585588
  52. 52. Dedovic K, Renwick R, Mahani NK, Engert V, Lupien SJ, Pruessner JC. The Montreal Imaging Stress Task: using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2005;30: 319–325. pmid:16151536
  53. 53. Wang J, Rao H, Wetmore GS, Furlan PM, Korczykowski M, Dinges DF, et al. Perfusion functional MRI reveals cerebral blood flow pattern under psychological stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. National Acad Sciences; 2005;102: 17804–17809. pmid:16306271
  54. 54. Gonzalez-Santos L, Mercadillo RE, Graff-Guerrero A, Barrios FA. Versión computarizada para la aplicación del listado de sintomas 90. Salud Mental. 2007;30: 31–40.
  55. 55. Bouma EMC, Riese H, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Oldehinkel AJ. Adolescents’ cortisol responses to awakening and social stress; effects of gender, menstrual phase and oral contraceptives. The TRAILS study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34: 884–893. pmid:19195792
  56. 56. King MG, Burrows GD, Stanley GV. Measurement of stress and arousal: validation of the stress/arousal adjective checklist. Br J Psychol. 1983;74 (Pt 4): 473–479.
  57. 57. Beauchamp MS. Detection of eye movements from fMRI data. Magn Reson Med. Wiley-Blackwell; 2003;49: 376–380. pmid:12541259
  58. 58. Tregellas JR, Tanabe JL, Miller DE, Freedman R. Monitoring eye movements during fMRI tasks with echo planar images. Hum Brain Mapp. Wiley-Blackwell; 2002;17: 237–243. pmid:12395391
  59. 59. Kanowski M, Rieger JW, Noesselt T, Tempelmann C, Hinrichs H. Endoscopic eye tracking system for fMRI. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2007;160: 10–15. pmid:16978705
  60. 60. Hasson U, Furman O, Clark D, Dudai Y, Davachi L. Enhanced intersubject correlations during movie viewing correlate with successful episodic encoding. Neuron. 2008;57: 452–462. pmid:18255037
  61. 61. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1994;25: 49–59. pmid:7962581
  62. 62. Aertsen AM, Gerstein GL, Habib MK, Palm G. Dynamics of neuronal firing correlation: modulation of "effective connectivity". Journal of Neurophysiology. 1989;61: 900–917. pmid:2723733
  63. 63. Şişbot S. A cross-correlation technique as a system evaluation tool; application to blood flow measurement in extra-corporeal circuits. Flow Measurement and Instrumentation. 2005;16: 27–34.
  64. 64. Hasson U. Intersubject Synchronization of Cortical Activity During Natural Vision. Science. 2004;303: 1634–1640. pmid:15016991
  65. 65. Kauppi J-P, Jääskeläinen IP, Sams M, Tohka J. Inter-subject correlation of brain hemodynamic responses during watching a movie: localization in space and frequency. Front Neuroinform. 2010;4: 5. pmid:20428497
  66. 66. Pajula J, Kauppi J-P, Tohka J. Inter-subject correlation in fMRI: method validation against stimulus-model based analysis. Harrison BJ, editor. PLoS ONE. Public Library of Science; 2012;7: e41196. pmid:22924089