Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Are the unken reflex and the aposematic colouration of Red-Bellied Toads efficient against bird predation?

  • Debora Wolff Bordignon ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    deborawbordignon@gmail.com

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biologia Animal, Departamento de Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil

  • Valentina Zaffaroni Caorsi,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biologia Animal, Departamento de Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil

  • Patrick Colombo,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Museu de Ciências Naturais, Fundação Zoobotânica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil

  • Michelle Abadie,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil

  • Ismael Verrastro Brack,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil

  • Bibiana Terra Dasoler,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil

  • Márcio Borges-Martins

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biologia Animal, Departamento de Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil

Abstract

Aposematic signals as well as body behaviours may be important anti-predator defences. Species of the genus Melanophryniscus are characterised by having toxic lipophilic alkaloids in the skin and for presenting a red ventral colouration, which can be observed when they perform the behaviour called the unken reflex. Both the reflex behaviour and the colouration pattern are described as defence mechanisms. However, there are currently no studies testing their effectiveness against predators. This study aimed to test experimentally if both ventral conspicuous colouration and the unken reflex in Melanophryniscus cambaraensis function as aposematic signals against visually oriented predators (birds). We simulated the species studied using three different clay toad models as follows: (a) in a normal position with green coloured bodies, (b) in the unken reflex position with green coloured body and extremities and (c) in the unken reflex position with a green body and red extremities. Models were distributed on a known M. cambaraensis breeding site and in the adjacent forest. More than half of the attacks on the models were from birds; however, there was no preference for any model type. Thus, just the presence of the red colour associated with the motionless unken reflex position does not seem to prevent attacks from potential predators. It is possible that the effective aposematic signal in Melanophryniscus is achieved through the unken reflex movement together with the subsequent exhibition of the warning colouration and the secretion of toxins.

Introduction

Antipredator strategies encompass several mechanisms involving the behaviour, morphology and colouration of prey species, which evolve either to avoid detection (e.g. camouflage) or to enhance honest or dishonest unprofitability signaling (e.g. aposematism, masquerade, pursuit deterrence, deflection, and deimatism) [1,2]. Organisms that contain toxic chemical substances may exhibit conspicuous colourations as visual honest signals of their toxicity [3,4]. Aposematic colouration is a common trait in nature which serves to warn potential predators that an individual is unpalatable, harmful, or potentially dangerous and should be avoided [58]. The effectiveness of aposematic signals depends on the ability of predators to associate the conspicuous colouration of a prey species with the disadvantage of attacking that species [810].

Aposematic colouration can be associated with behavioural signals to enhance its effect. Several prey postures, movements or sounds can cause fear responses in predators, the so-called deimatism [2]. These displays could cause predators to misclassify a potential threat by giving the impression of a larger body size or by displaying body areas which contain higher concentrations of toxic substances, for example [1,2,11]. A behaviour shared by different amphibian families is the unken reflex which was first described for Bombina bombina [12]. In the unken reflex, the individual arches its body, raising the head and the posterior region to reveal hidden areas with aposematic colouration [12,13]. During these behaviors individuals usually close their eyes or cover them with their hands [13,14], the extremities of the body are exposed, and the ventral area of the hands, feet and throat are displayed. The unken reflex posture is also generally associated with aposematic ventral colourations and with the presence of toxic substances [15]. This behaviour has been reported in many amphibian species [16,17], including the anuran genera: Bombina (Bombinatoridae) [18], Melanophryniscus (Bufonidae) [1821], Hemisus (Hemisotidae) [22], Boana and Smilisca (Hylidae) [23,24], Neobatrachus (Limnodynastidae) [25], Boophis (Mantellidae) [26], Pseudophryne (Myobatrachidae) [25], Rana (Ranidae) [2731] and Nyctixalus and Rhacophorus (Rhaphocoridae) [3234]. In urodeles, the unken reflex occurs in some Salamandridae such as Lissotriton [35], Salamandrina [36,37], Taricha [38] and Triturus [39]. However, behaviour terminology is evidently not uniform and many of the reported species lack some behavioural or morphological attributes of the typical unken reflex as seen in Bombina and Melanophryniscus. The unken reflex seems to have been confounded with other behaviours (e.g. eye-protection), particularly in species lacking aposematic ventral colouration or toxic substances [17].

Due to the display of aposematic colouration, it is generally assumed that the unken reflex may be efficient primarily against visually oriented predators. It is known that birds possess one of the most elaborate mechanisms of colour vision within the vertebrates [40,41] and may represent the main predators of several aposematic species, including the poison frogs of the Dendrobatidae family. However, predation events are very difficult to observe in nature and there is still little experimental evidence of predator attacks on this family [42]. A convenient experimental method used in short-term studies of predation consists of recording attack marks on soft replicas (such as clay models) of the species of interest [43,44]. Yet despite this, there are currently no studies of predation on anuran species which display ventral aposematic colouration during the unken reflex.

Using clay models of the south American red-bellied-toad Melanophryniscus cambaraensis the present study aimed to experimentally test whether conspicuous colouration and the unken reflex function as effective aposematic signals against visually oriented predators (i.e. birds). If both strategies are effective and independent defence mechanisms, we expect to find different rates of predation attempts related to the interaction between these strategies.

Materials and methods

Species model

The Bufonidae genus Melanophryniscus Gallardo, 1961 includes 29 species [45] restricted to southeastern South America [46], which are known as south American red-bellied toads. These small toads secrete skin toxins, such as alkaloids and bufadienolides (e.g. [47,48]) and when disturbed they display the unken reflex in which they expose their brightly coloured ventral areas [16,18,21,49,50]. The conspicuous ventral colouration and the unken reflex of this species have been suggested as probable defence mechanisms to avoid predation. However, their effectiveness has not yet been investigated. Previous studies describe only the behaviour itself, usually in response to human manipulation, and not in response to natural predation attempts (e.g. [16,21]).

Melanophryniscus cambaraensis Braun & Braun, 1979 (Fig 1A and 1B) is one of the three species of the genus Melanophryniscus with green dorsal colouration, and with ventral colouration which may vary from red to orange [5153]. This red-bellied-toad is a threatened, vulnerable species [54,55], endemic of the southern of the Mixed Ombrophilous Forest and of the northern of South Brazilian Grasslands of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil [56]. The species is historically known only from two populations, located respectively in São Francisco de Paula and Cambará do Sul municipalities [57]. However, in the last locality it has not been seen since 1990 (according to several field expeditions conducted on this locality to search this toad). As with other species of the genus, M. cambaraensis exhibits explosive reproduction, without a defined reproductive season, breeding during a few days immediately after copious rains [56].

thumbnail
Fig 1.

Melanophryniscus cambaraensis (A) in normal position and (B) in unken reflex position displaying the aposematic colouration of its hands and feet (animal with open eyes, unlike previous descriptions). Clay model replicas of the species (C) in normal position, (D) with green hands and feet in unken reflex position and (E) with red hands and feet in unken reflex position.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193551.g001

Study area

We performed this study in the Floresta Nacional de São Francisco de Paula (FLONA SFP) (29° 25’ 41.3” S, 50° 23’ 44.5” W, 923 m above sea level). The FLONA SFP is a protected area of 1607 ha, covered mostly by the native Mixed Ombrophilous Forest [58]. The climate is Temperate Superhumid, with temperatures ranging from -3–18°C in the winter and 18.3–27°C in the summer, with average annual temperature of 14.5°C [59] and annual precipitation exceeding 2000 mm [60].

We conducted the experiment in May 2016 around the breeding site of the species. This site is located at the edge of a small, unpaved road, and is a rocky outcrop partially covered by a thin layer of vegetation (approximately 20 m long and 4 m wide). A planted forest of Parana pine, Araucaria angustifolia, composes the surrounding area. Toads have been recorded in this adjacent forest up to 120 m from the breeding site [56].

Clay models

We made 900 models by hand which were phenotypically simillar to Melanophryniscus cambaraensis using pre-coloured, non-toxic plasticine modeling clay (Sculpey III® and Acrilex®). This clay is well suited for this type of study as it does not harden and retains markings left by predators [61]. We created two rubber moulds in two different positions: normal position (as animals are usually found in nature) and unken reflex position. These were created using two fixed specimens deposited in the herpetological collection of the Museu de Ciências Naturais of Fundação Zoobotânica of Rio Grande do Sul (MCN 13459 and unvouchered specimen), with snout-to-vent-lengths representative of the average size of the FLONA SFP population (SVL 32 ± 1,6; [62]). The clay models were organised into three categories: (a) green body and extremities in a normal position (normal) (Fig 1C), (b) green body and extremities in unken reflex position (green unken) (Fig 1D), and green body with red extremities in unken reflex position (red unken) (Fig 1E). For each category, we constructed 300 clay models, totalling 900 toads. A black permanent pen (Sharpie®) was used to draw the eyes only on the normal models, since animals in the unken reflex position usually close their eyes or cover them with their hands [13,14].

We collected one individual of Melanophryniscus cambaraensis in order to measure the reflectance of different colours and ultraviolet light, since birds have tetrachromatic vision and can perceive ultraviolet wavelengths [40,63]. These measurements were made using a spectrometer (“Ocean Optics”, version 2200 SD). We measured the wavelengths of light reflected from living and clay toads and compared the results. We mixed the clay to match the wavelengths reflected from the living animal as closely as possible (green: 550 nm; red: 650 nm) radiation intensity (green: 120 counts; red: 160 counts). Neither the animal nor the clay models reflected ultraviolet. The voucher specimen collected was deposited in the Coleção Herpetológica do Departamento de Zoologia from the Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS.7317).

Ethical and legal procedures

This study was authorised by the Instituto Chico Mendes de Conservação da Biodiversidade–ICMBio, under SISBIO (Sistema de Autorização e Informação em Biodiversidade) license number 52119–1.

Experimental design

A total of 810 out of 900 models were placed sequentially (temporally and spatially) into the forest. The experimental design was composed of 18 transects positioned perpendicular to the unpaved road. The middle transects were located next to the breeding site. Each transect was 90 m long and was separated by 20 m from other transects. Each transect had 15 blocks distributed every 6 m, containing the three model categories placed parallel to each other. Categories were randomly ordered and placed every 1 m inside the blocks (Fig 2). To remove possible cryptic effects [44], half of the blocks were placed on the leaf litter and half onto a white opaque plastic board (9 cm x 9 cm). We alternated each background between the blocks. On the same day, the remaining 90 models (30 of each category) were randomly distributed across the breeding site of the species and half of them selected at random were placed on a white background. In total the experiment lasted five days with the following design: Day 1 (models were placed) > Days 2–4 (models were exposed to the environment) > Day 5 (models were removed). During the environmental exposure of the models (72 hours) the area was not disturbed by anthropic activity. On the last day, the 900 models were carefully collected, photographed and classified with the type and number of attacks recorded.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Experimental design.

The 18 transects were divided into 15 blocks distributed every 6 m, containing the three model categories in parallel. Categories were randomly ordered and placed every 1 m inside the blocks. Half of the blocks were placed on the leaf litter and half onto a white plastic board.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193551.g002

Data analyses

Initially, we classified models as attacked (when there were bite marks) or not attacked. Additionally, we recorded the position of the marks on the clay toads as either anterior, posterior or both. Consecutive attacks on the same toad model were scored as a single event [44]. Attacks were categorised according to the type of predator as: (1) Birds; (2) Mammals; (3) Arthropods and (4) Unidentified. Attempts of predation from birds were recognised by beak prints in the shape of a “U” or “V”, as well as long stripes left on the clay (Fig 3A) (e.g. [42,44,64,65]). Mammal predation attempts were identified by characteristic tooth marks (Fig 3B.), and arthropods by little, subtle jaw prints and incisions left on the clay (Fig 3C) [44,66]. Any mark which did not fit in the categories above was classified as unidentified (Fig 3D). Nevertheless, only bird marks were considered for this work, since mammals and arthropods are not visually oriented for predation and might have been attracted to the models despite their shape or colour and visual cues are the basis of our hypothesis [6769]. Models not recovered after 72h (n = 6) were scored as missing. The effect of inclusion (assuming bird predation) or exclusion (not assuming bird predation) of the missing models was tested.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Examples of clay models attacked.

(A) bird, (B) mammal, (C) arthropod, and (D) unidentified origin.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193551.g003

We used a binomial Generalized Linear Model to determine if attack rate by birds was influenced by the category of the model, the background type and the interaction between model category and background. To test the significance of the variables we used Likelihood Ratio Tests, starting with the general model (with interaction term) and dropping variables sequentially. All statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.3.3. Results were considered significant when p < 0.05.

Results

Of the 810 models placed on the forest transects, 86 (10.6%) were attacked (being 5.7% by birds) and six (0.7%) were missing—three normal (two over white board and one over the leaf litter) and three green unken (three over the leaf litter). Birds were responsible for 53.5% (46) of the attacks, of which 41.3% (19) were on normal models, 19.6% (9) on green unken models and 39.1% (18) on red unken models (Table 1). Of the 46 attacks, 63% (29) were above the leaf litter and 37% (17) were on the white background.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of birds predation attempts on clay models of Melanophryniscus cambaraensis in relation to body and background.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193551.t001

When missing models were excluded from the analysis, the interaction between model category and background did not have a significant effect on which models were predated by birds (χ2 = 2.01; p = 0.37). The toad model categories alone were also not a predictor of bird predation (χ2 = 4.59; p = 0.10). When comparing background types, a higher number of clay toads were attacked in the leaf litter, however this difference was not significant (χ2 = 3.42; p = 0.06) (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Estimated probability of bird attack on clay models of Melanophryniscus cambaraensis.

Three toad posture treatments (normal, green unken and red unken) and two background types (white or leaf litter), excluding missing models. Lines represent 95% confidence intervals around the mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193551.g004

When missing models were included as predation attempts, the interaction of model category and background was not significant (χ2 = 1.00; p = 0.61). There was also no significant difference between models (χ2 = 3.30; p = 0.19). However, the type of background was a predictor of bird predation (χ2 = 4.07; p = 0.04), showing that toads on the leaf litter were on average 1,7 times more likely to be attacked than models placed on the white board.

Mammals, arthropods and unidentified predators were responsible for 7.7%, 6.6% and 32.2% of the total attacks, respectively. Besides that, only five out of 90 models were attacked on the breeding site, two by mammals and three by arthropods.

Even though our experiments were not originally designed to evaluate preferences for body parts by the predator, we consider it relevant to present our results here for the benefit of future experiments. In the models placed in the forest, the normal models received about 70% of the bird attacks on the anterior part of the body, especially on the head and eye region. Another 26% of the attempts were on both anterior and posterior region and 5% only on the posterior region. When comparing this to the position of the attacks on unken models, only 30% of the attempts were on the anterior region, 18% on the posterior and 52% in both.

Discussion

Our results suggest that attacks by birds on the three different models occur at the same rate. Therefore, these results do not support the working hypotheses that unken reflex behaviour and red colouration in Melanophryniscus cambaraensis are efficient warning signals against predation attempts by birds. However, we found evidence that the static unken position reduces the ability of the predator to identify the toad’s head and therefore may reduce the consequences of predation attempts.

Several studies with clay models have suggested that organisms with aposematic colouration have lower predation rates than the non-aposematic organisms (e.g. [14,44,64,66,69]). However, recent studies using Dendrobatidae models also found, similar to our results, that aposematic colouration was not a predictor for the rate of attacks from visually oriented predators [42,70]. A lack of difference in the number of avian attacks on the cryptic and the local aposematic forms of Dendrobates tinctorius was also reported, however there was a difference in the attack rate when an unknown aposematic models were introduced [61].

Although the model type was not a predictor of bird attack rates on Melanoprhyniscus cambaraensis, the rate of attacks on normal and red unken toads was twice than on green unken toads. These results may be explained by the absence of the green unken phenotype in the environment, inducing a neophobic reaction in the predators [70]. Alternatively, it may be explained as a result of differences in detectability. The red warning colour in the red unken models and the eyes in the normal model might have increased detectability by the predators [71,72]. Recent studies reinforced that birds can be very effective at discriminating similarly shaped objects as they can distinguish a toad from a fruit of similar size, colour and shape [69]. Therefore, we consider that the attacks on the red unken models were the result of a choice and not a mistake.

The effectiveness of aposematic signals depends on the predator`s learning ability and the intensity of the association between the signal and the perceived unprofitability of the prey [810]. Melanophryniscus spp. contains lipophilic alkaloids sequestered from its diet [7375] and biosynthesized bufadienolide-like compounds and indolealkylamines as a chemical defence [48]. Therefore, the warning mechanisms (unken reflex and colouration) can be assumed as honest signals of unprofitability [76,77]. However, the effectiveness of the aposematic signal is expected to be higher in prey with high population densities, which are frequently encountered by predators [6,9,78]. The population density of Melanophryniscus cambaraensis in the study area is usually low and they are mainly found in the forest, except during reproductive events when the species can be found in aggregations at the breeding site at the edge of the forest. Despite this, the species is spatially very rare, since only one small population is known for the area [56]. This rarity can result in a weak warning signal due to reduced learning opportunities for predators. The lack of other aposematic anurans in the region ([56], Garcia, unpublished data) may also weaken the strength of the signal. We suggest that further studies should investigate the effect of aggregation on bird attack rates. If our assumptions about the strength of the warning signals are correct, we expect to find consistent lower rates of attack in the breeding sites.

When excluding the missing toads from our analysis, the background was found not to be a significant predictor of bird attacks. On the other hand, when including the six missing toads, the rate of attacks is higher on the leaf litter and does not change on the white background. Studies have reported the same pattern for other amphibians [42,64], snakes [44] and moth caterpillars [79], finding a lower bird predation rate when exposing models to high-contrast backgrounds. These results may be explained by a neophobic reaction of predators to white backgrounds [70] and therefore demonstrate that the choice of background is relevant and must be considered for the design of further studies.

One remarkable result of our study was the difference in the rates of bird attacks on body regions. Normal models were attacked 2.3 times more on the head than the unken models. Previous studies have also reported higher attempts of predation on the head region when it could be identified by different morphology or by the presence of eyes, but not in models that lacked this differentiation [71,72]. The lack of conspicuous difference between the anterior and posterior regions in the unken models used in this study may have confused the predators, which were not able to identify the head region. Hence, we suggest that the unken reflex position reduces the ability of the predator to identify the head region of the toads, which may reduce the chances of death or serious damage to the eyes, especially because birds probably perceive the unpalatability of the animal after the first bite. In this sense, the static unken position could function as a deflection mechanism [2], that might allow the prey to avoid mortal injury by directing the predators’ attacks towards a less-vital body area.

In Melanophryniscus cambaraensis the presence of conspicuous colouration together with a static unken reflex did not reduce the number of attacks by visually oriented predators. However, a study with static and moving Dendrobatidade models indicated that movement could be important for predicting a predator’s decision to attack or not [66]. We suggest that the strongest anti-predator mechanism in this species may be a synergistic association between four distinct mechanisms: colouration (aposematism), unken reflex movement (deimatism), unken static position (deflection) and secretion of skin toxins (chemical defence). If this is true, the unken reflex should be further investigated concerning its possible deimatic effect which may be independent of the other anti-predator mechanisms and necessary to render the anti-predatory signal effective.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Bird attacks on clay models.

Table containing original data used for the analysis of the bird attacks on the clay models placed in the forest. In the Bird Attack column: 0 = not attacked and 1 = attacked. In Missing Model column: 0 = not missing and 1 = missing. Model column: number of models; Background column: type of background where the model was placed (white or leaf litter); Transect column: number of the transect where the model was placed; Block column: number of the block where the model was placed; Category column: model type category (normal, red unken or green unken).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193551.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by CAPES, CNPq and PPG Biologia Animal, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul and Fundação Zoobotânica do Rio Grande do Sul. We thank Edenice Brandão Ávila de Souza and the personnel at the Floresta Nacional de São Francisco de Paula, for supported our fieldwork; ICMBio for the collection license (No. 52119–1); Luiz Gustavo Pereira, for the loan and assistance in the use of the spectrophotometer; Ronaldo Gemerasca, for the creation of the moulds; Natália Dallagnol Vargas, Pablo Inchausti, Adriana Jeckel, Alexandro Tozetti and Sam Hardman for commentaries and suggestions of an early version of the manuscript. In addition, we thank the field assistants Camila de Oliveira, Debora Newlands, Deivid Pereira and Thayná Mendes for their help. We thank the other friends and colleagues from the Laboratório de Herpetologia of UFRGS who assisted in the preparation of the models and made possible the accomplishment of this work.

References

  1. 1. Umbers KDL, Mappes J. Towards a tractable working hypothesis for deimatic displays. Anim Behav. 2016;113: e5–e7.
  2. 2. Caro T, Allen WL. Interspecific visual signalling in animals and plants: a functional classification. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. The Royal Society; 2017;372: 20160344. pmid:28533461
  3. 3. Poulton EB. The colours of animals: their meaning and use especially considered in the case of insects Kegan, Trench, Trubnes & Co. Ltd, London. 1890; 360.
  4. 4. Cott HB. Adaptive coloration in animals. Methuen; London; 1940.
  5. 5. Forsman A, Herrström J. Asymmetry in size, shape, and color impairs the protective value of conspicuous color patterns. Behav Ecol. 2004;15: 141–147.
  6. 6. Comeault AA, Noonan BP. Spatial variation in the fitness of divergent aposematic phenotypes of the poison frog, Dendrobates tinctorius. J Evol Biol. 2011;24: 1374–1379. pmid:21418119
  7. 7. Edmunds M. Defence in animals: a survey of anti-predator defences. Longman Publishing Group; 1974.
  8. 8. Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP. Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. Oxford University Press; 2004.
  9. 9. Mappes J, Marples N, Endler J. The complex business of survival by aposematism. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005;20: 598–603. pmid:16701442
  10. 10. Darst CR, Cummings ME, Cannatella DC. A mechanism for diversity in warning signals: Conspicuousness versus toxicity in poison frogs. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103: 5852–5857. pmid:16574774
  11. 11. Skelhorn J, Holmes GG, Rowe C. Deimatic or aposematic? Anim Behav. Academic Press; 2016;113: e1–e3.
  12. 12. Hinsche G. Vergleichende untersuchungen zum sogenannten unkenreflex. Biol Zent Bl. 1926;46: 296–305.
  13. 13. Bajger J. Diversity of Defensive Responses in Populations of Fire Toads (Bombina bombina and Bombina variegata). Herpetologica. 1980;36: 133–137.
  14. 14. Kuchta SR. Experimental Support for Aposematic Coloration in the Salamander Ensatina eschscholtzii xanthoptica: Implications for Mimicry of Pacific Newts. Reeder TW, editor. Copeia. 2005;2005: 265–271.
  15. 15. Griffiths RA. Newts and Salamanders of Europe. London, UK and San Diego, USA: T. & AD Poyser Natural History. Academic Press; 1996.
  16. 16. Toledo LF, Sazima I, Haddad CFB. Behavioural defences of anurans: an overview. Ethol Ecol Evol. Taylor & Francis; 2011;23: 1–25.
  17. 17. Jablonski D, Balej P. Case of defensive behavior in Pelobates syriacus (Amphibia: Pelobatidae). Herpetol Notes. 2014;7: 141–143.
  18. 18. Fernández K. Sobre la biología y reproducción de batracios argentinos. Segunda parte. Boletín la Acad Nac Ciencias Córdoba. 1927;29: 271–328.
  19. 19. Brusquetti F, Baldo D, Motte M. Amphibia, Anura, Bufonidae, Melanophryniscus krauczuki: Geographic distribution map and first record for Paraguay. Check List. 2007;3: 141–144.
  20. 20. Almeida-Santos P, Souza do Nacimento D, Abrunhosa PA, Van Sluys M. Melanophryniscus moreirae (Maldonado Red belly Toad). Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. 2010;41: 200–201.
  21. 21. Caorsi VZ, Colombo P, Freire MD, Amaral IB, Zank C, Borges-Martins M, et al. Natural history, coloration pattern and conservation status of the threatened South Brazilian red bellied toad, Melanophryniscus macrogranulosus Braun, 1973 (Anura, Bufonidae). Herpetol notes. 2014;7: 585–598.
  22. 22. Greenbaum E, Kusamba C, Moninga W, Aristote M. Hemisus marmoratus (Marbled Snout-Burrower). Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. 2012;43: 320–321.
  23. 23. Angulo A, Funk WC. Hyla calcarata (Rana de Espolones) and Hyla fasciata (NCN). Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. 2006;37: 203–204.
  24. 24. Firschein IL. Phragmosis and the”unken reflex” in a Mexican hylid frog, Pternohyla fodiens. Copeia. 1951;1951: 74.
  25. 25. Williams CR, Brodie ED, Tyler MJ, Walker SJ. Antipredator Mechanisms of Australian Frogs. J Herpetol. 2000;34: 431–443.
  26. 26. Andreone F. Boophis albilabris. Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. 2002;33: 299–300.
  27. 27. Means DB. Rana capito (Florida gopher frog). Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. 2004;35: 163–164.
  28. 28. Haberl W, Wilkinson JW. A note on the Unkenreflex and similar defensive postures in Rana temporaria (Anura, Amphibia). Br Herpetol Soc Bull. 1997;61: 16–20.
  29. 29. Schlüpmann M. Schreckruf und Unkenreflex beim Grasfrosch (Rana temporaria) sowie Anmerkungen zu Schreckreaktionen bei Amphibien. Zeitschrift für Feldherpetologie. 2000;7: 29–35.
  30. 30. Jablonski D, Gvoždík V. Imobilita jako obranné chování žab pozorované u skokana hnědého. Ziva. 2009;57: 228–229.
  31. 31. Carretero MA, Sillero N, Corti C, Jorge F, Freitas S, Arakelyan M. Unkenreflex in Rana macrocnemis from Armenia. Herpetol Notes. 2011;4: 67–69.
  32. 32. Das I, Leong M, Tan M. Nyctixalus pictus (cinnamon tree frog). Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles; 2004;35: 373–374.
  33. 33. Duong LTT, Rowley JJL. Rhacophorus feae (Thao Whipping Frog). Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. 2010;41: 342.
  34. 34. Streicher JW, Smith EN, Harvey MB. Rhacophorus margaritifer (Java Flying Frog). Defensive behavior. Herpetol Rev. 2011;42: 590–591.
  35. 35. Marco A, Leguía J. Unken reflex” en el tritón ibérico, Triturus boscai: postura defensiva innata en hábitats terrestres. Rev Española Herpetol. 2001;15: 5–11.
  36. 36. Utzeri C, Antonelli D, Angelini C. Note on the behavior of the Spectacled Salamander Salamandrina terdigitata (Lacépede, 1788). Herpetozoa. 2005;18: 182–185.
  37. 37. Lanza B. Reazione di tipo Unkenreflex in un urodelo (Salamandrina terdigitata). Ethology. Wiley Online Library; 1966;23: 855–857.
  38. 38. Johnson JA, Brodie ED. The Selective Advantage of the Defensive Posture of the Newt, Taricha granulosa. Am Midl Nat. 1975;93: 139.
  39. 39. Brodie Jr ED. Salamander antipredator postures. Copeia. 1977;3: 523–535.
  40. 40. Bowmaker JK, Heath LA, Wilkie SE, Hunt DM. Visual pigments and oil droplets from six classes of photoreceptor in the retinas of birds. Vision Res. 1997;37: 2183–94. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9578901 pmid:9578901
  41. 41. Master TL. Predation by Rufous Motmot on black-and-green poison dart frog. Wilson J Ornithol. 1999;111: 439–440.
  42. 42. Hegna RH, Saporito RA, Gerow KG, Donnelly MA. Contrasting Colors of an Aposematic Poison Frog Do Not Affect Predation. Ann Zool Fennici. 2011;48: 29–38.
  43. 43. Madsen T. Are Juvenile Grass Snakes, Natrix natrix, Aposematically Coloured? Oikos. 1987;48: 265–267.
  44. 44. Brodie ED. Differential avoidance of coral snake banded patterns by free-ranging avian predators in Costa Rica. Evolution (N Y). 1993;47: 227–235. pmid:28568087
  45. 45. Frost DR. Amphibian species of the world: an online reference. Version 6.0. Eletronic database. New York: American Museum of Natural History [Internet]. 2017. Available: http://research.amnh.org/herpetology/amphibia/index.html
  46. 46. Zank C, Becker FG, Abadie M, Baldo D, Maneyro R, Borges-Martins M. Climate Change and the Distribution of Neotropical Red-Bellied Toads (Melanophryniscus, Anura, Amphibia): How to Prioritize Species and Populations? PLoS One. 2014;9: e94625. pmid:24755937
  47. 47. Daly JW, Spande TF, Garraffo HM. Alkaloids from Amphibian Skin: A Tabulation of Over Eight-Hundred Compounds. J Nat Prod. 2005;68: 1556–1575. pmid:16252926
  48. 48. Jeckel AM, Grant T, Saporito RA. Sequestered and Synthesized Chemical Defenses in the Poison Frog Melanophryniscus moreirae. J Chem Ecol. 2015;41: 505–512. pmid:25902958
  49. 49. Kwet A, Maneyro R, Zillikens A, Mebs D. Advertisement calls of Melanophryniscus dorsalis (Mertens, 1933) and M. montevidensis (Philippi, 1902), two parapatric species from southern Brazil and Uruguay, with comments on morphological variation in the Melanophryniscus stelzneri group (Anura: Bufon. Salamandra. 2005;41: 3–20.
  50. 50. Santos RR, Grant T. Diel pattern of migration in a poisonous toad from Brazil and the evolution of chemical defenses in diurnal amphibians. Evol Ecol. Springer Netherlands; 2011;25: 249–258.
  51. 51. Braun PC, Braun CAS. Nova espécie de Melanophryniscus Gallardo, 1961 do Estado do Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil (Anura, Bufonidae). Iheringia Série Zool. 1979;54: 7–17.
  52. 52. Di-Bernardo M, Maneyro R, Grillo H. New species of Melanophryniscus (anura: bufonidae) from Rio grande do sul, southern brazil. J Herpetol. BioOne; 2006;40: 261–266.
  53. 53. Caorsi VZ, Santos RR, Grant T. Clip or Snap? An Evaluation of Toe-Clipping and Photo-Identification Methods for Identifying Individual Southern Red-Bellied Toads, Melanophryniscus cambaraensis. South Am J Herpetol. 2012;7: 79–84.
  54. 54. Ministério do Meio Ambiente—MMA. Portaria MMA n. 444/2014, de 17 de dezembro de 2014. Diário Oficial da União, Brasília, DF, v. 126, n. 245, p. 121, 18 dez. 2014. Seção 1.
  55. 55. Decreto Estadual N° 51.797 de 8 de Setembro de 2014. Lista de espécies da fauna ameaçada. Diário Oficial do Rio Grande do Sul n.o 173, de 09 de setembro de 2014.
  56. 56. Santos RR, Leonardi SB, Caorsi VZ, Grant T. Directional orientation of migration in an aseasonal explosive-breeding toad from Brazil. J Trop Ecol. Cambridge University Press; 2010;26: 415–421.
  57. 57. Garcia PCA, Vinciprova G. Anfíbios . In: Fontana C, Bencke G, Reis R, editors. Livro vermelho da fauna ameaçada de extinção no Rio Grande do Sul. Porto Alegre: EDIPUCRS; 2003. pp. 147–164.
  58. 58. Sonego RC, Backes A, Souza AF. Descrição da estrutura de uma Floresta Ombrófila Mista, RS, Brasil, utilizando estimadores não-paramétricos de riqueza e rarefação de amostras. Acta Bot Brasilica. 2007;21: 943–955.
  59. 59. Maluf J. Nova classificação climática do Estado do Rio Grande do Sul. Rev Bras Agrometeorol. 2000;8: 141–150.
  60. 60. Backes A, Prates FL, Viola MG. Produção de serapilheira em Floresta Ombrófila Mista, em São Francisco de Paula, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil. Acta Bot Brasilica. 2005;19: 155–160. Available: http://www.scielo.br/pdf/%0D/abb/v19n1/v19n1a14.pdf
  61. 61. Noonan BP, Comeault AA. The role of predator selection on polymorphic aposematic poison frogs. Biol Lett. 2009;5: 51–54. pmid:19019778
  62. 62. Caorsi VZ. Estudo taxonômico e filogeográfico de Melanophryniscus macrogranulosus Braun, 1973 e M. cambaraensis Braun & Braun, 1979 (Anura: Bufonidae), duas espécies ameaçadas de extinção do extremo sul da mata atlântica. 2014.
  63. 63. Cuthill IC. Color perception. In: Hill GE, McGraw KJ, editors. Bird Coloration: Mechanisms and measurements. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press; 2006. pp. 3–40.
  64. 64. Saporito RA, Zuercher R, Roberts M, Gerow KG, Donnelly MA. Experimental evidence for aposematism in the dendrobatid poison frog Oophaga pumilio. Copeia. The American Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists 810 East 10th Street, P.O. Box 1897, Lawrence, Kansas 66044; 2007;2007: 1006–1011.
  65. 65. McElroy MT. Teasing apart crypsis and aposematism—evidence that disruptive coloration reduces predation on a noxious toad. Biol J Linn Soc. 2016;117: 285–294.
  66. 66. Paluh DJ, Hantak MM, Saporito RA. A Test of Aposematism in the Dendrobatid Poison Frog Oophaga pumilio: The Importance of Movement in Clay Model Experiments. J Herpetol. 2014;48: 249–254.
  67. 67. Bell WJ, Cardé RT. Chemical ecology of insects. Chapman and Hall; 1984.
  68. 68. Vander Wall SB. Foraging success of granivorous rodents: Effects of variation in seed and soil water on olfaction. Ecology. Ecological Society of America; 1998;79: 233–241.
  69. 69. Paluh DJ, Kenison EK, Saporito RA. Frog or Fruit? The Importance of Color and Shape to Bird Predators in Clay Model Experiments. Copeia. 2015;103: 58–63.
  70. 70. Hegna RH, Saporito RA, Donnelly MA. Not all colors are equal: predation and color polytypism in the aposematic poison frog Oophaga pumilio. Evol Ecol. 2013;27: 831–845.
  71. 71. Smith SM. A Study of Prey-Attack Behaviour in Young Loggerhead Shrikes, Lanius Ludovicianus L. Behaviour. 1973;44: 113–140.
  72. 72. Smiths SM. Predatory Behaviour of Young Turquoise-Browed Motmots, Eumomota Superciliosa. Behaviour. 1976;56: 309–320.
  73. 73. Daly JW, Secunda SI, Garraffo HM, Spande TF, Wisnieski A, Nishihira C, et al. Variability in alkaloid profiles in neotropical poison frogs (Dendrobatidae): Genetic versus environmental determinants. Toxicon. 1992;30: 887–898. pmid:1523680
  74. 74. Mebs D, Pogoda W, Maneyro R, Kwet A. Studies on the poisonous skin secretion of individual red bellied toads, Melanophryniscus montevidensis (Anura, Bufonidae), from Uruguay. Toxicon. 2005;46: 641–650. pmid:16157358
  75. 75. Daly JW, Wilham JM, Spande TF, Garraffo HM, Gil RR, Silva GL, et al. Alkaloids in Bufonid Toads (Melanophryniscus): Temporal and Geographic Determinants for Two Argentinian Species. J Chem Ecol. 2007;33: 871–887. pmid:17333373
  76. 76. Blount JD, Speed MP, Ruxton GD, Stephens PA. Warning displays may function as honest signals of toxicity. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2009;276: 871–877. pmid:19019790
  77. 77. Maan ME, Cummings ME. Poison frog colors are honest signals of toxicity, particularly for bird predators. Am Nat. University of Chicago Press Chicago, IL; 2011;179: E1–E14. pmid:22173468
  78. 78. Endler JA, Mappes J. Predator Mixes and the Conspicuousness of Aposematic Signals. Am Nat. The University of Chicago Press; 2004;163: 532–547. pmid:15122501
  79. 79. Lindstedt C, Lindström L, Mappes J. Hairiness and warning colours as components of antipredator defence: additive or interactive benefits? Anim Behav. 2008;75: 1703–1713.