Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

An introduction to the ‘Psycho-Physiological-Stress-Test’ (PPST)—A standardized instrument for evaluating stress reactions

  • Elisabeth Neureiter ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft

    elisabeth.neureiter@charite.de

    Affiliations Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany, Alpen-Adria-Universität Klagenfurt, Klagenfurt, Austria

  • Loreen Hajfani,

    Roles Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany

  • Anne Ahnis,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany

  • Annett Mierke,

    Roles Software

    Affiliation Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany

  • Matthias Rose,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany

  • Gerhard Danzer,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany, Alpen-Adria-Universität Klagenfurt, Klagenfurt, Austria, Medical University Brandenburg - Campus Ruppiner Kliniken, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Neuruppin, Germany

  • Burghard F. Klapp

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Abstract

Using a standardized instrument to evaluate patients’ stress reactions has become more important in daily clinical routines. Different signs or symptoms of stress are often unilaterally explored: the physiological, psychological or social aspects of stress disorders are each viewed on a single dimension. However, all dimensions afflict patients who have persistent health problems due to chronic stress. Therefore, it is important to use a multidimensional approach to acquire data. The ‘Psycho-Physiological-Stress-Test’ (PPST) was established to achieve a comprehensive understanding of stress and was further developed at the Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin in collaboration with the Psychological Department of Freie Universität Berlin. The PPST includes a series of varying stress phases, embedded in two periods of rest. Physiological and psychological parameters are simultaneously measured throughout the test session. Specifically, the PPST activates the sympathetic stress axis, which is measured by heart rate, blood pressure, respiration depth and rate, electro dermal activation and muscle tension (frontalis, masseter, trapezius). Psychological data are simultaneously collected, and include performance, motivation, emotion and behavior. After conducting this diagnostic test, it is possible to identify individual stress patterns that can be discussed with the individual patient to develop and recommend (outpatient) treatment strategies. This paper introduces the PPST as a standardized way to evaluate stress reactions by presenting the results from a sample of psychosomatic inpatients (n = 139) who were treated in Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany. We observed that the varying testing conditions provoked adjusted changes in the different physiological parameters and psychological levels.

Introduction

Given the problems with the psychophysiological discipline’s multidisciplinary approach, we present an economical stress test for evaluating stress reactions in the clinical routine. Patients who suffer from body related complaints, in the context of psychosocial distress, are often referred to a psychosomatic expert after several years. Prior to the psychosomatic consultation, these patients have usually completed a large number of physicals without pathological results [1,2]. It is often difficult for patients to understand the relation between physiological and psychosocial aspects of illness. Stress is associated with a wide range of different diseases that involve almost every physiological system, such as cardiovascular, gastrointestinal and respiratory system [35]. Many clinical studies have experimentally verified that stress affects individual’s physiological and psychological systems [69]. Selye defined stress as the body’s nonspecific reaction to any demand [10]. In 1975, Selye [11] differentiated between ‘dis- and eustress’, or pathological stress (negative, distress) vs. health-promoting stress (positive, eustress). Although distress leads to (severe) physiological and psychological health problems, eustress has beneficial outcomes, including the ability to adjust to new situations or focus on solving problems, for example, at work [12]. Lazarus and Folkman postulated, that stress is a pattern of a negative psychophysiological condition in which individuals are (or feel) unable to cope with situations which they perceive as threats to their well- being [13]. Stress and distress evoke person-, organ- and stimuli-specific (stress) reactions. Therefore, it is important to assess a wide range of physiological and psychosocial parameters when studying individuals’ stress reaction(s).

Other multidimensional stress tests that are comparable with the PPST are the ‘Trier Social Stress Test (TSST)’ [14], the ‘Trier Mental Challenge Test’ (TMCT) [15] and the ‘Mannheimer Multikomponenten Stress Test’ (MMST) [16]. These psychophysiological instruments often examine physiological parameters, such as salivary cortisol, heart rate and heart rate variability [17].

In our experimental setting, patients are exposed to stimuli in varying conditions that are supposed to evoke psychophysiological activity. As such, we perform a statistical analysis with 139 unselected inpatients with the usual spectrum of diseases of a psychosomatic division (i.e., adaptive disorders, somatoform disorders, affective disorders, stress related disorders) to test the hypothesis that the PPST detects stress reactions in the biopsychosocial dimensions of interest.

Methods

PPST protocol

The PPST consists of periods of (simple) challenge and complex tasks that should result in stress or distress and two periods of rest. Each period lasts for approximately 2 minutes (Fig 1). Between the testing periods, the patient is provided with additional instructions for the next tasks, which includes providing psychological data to measure emotions, motivations, expectations and performance.

The patient’s task is to find two numbers from a random matrix of 36 numbers (in the range of 01 to 99) as quickly as possible. The patient has four options to report their presence: number one or number two is present, both are present or neither is present.

The test begins with a first rest period that is indicated by a standardized picture on the monitor and serves as a baseline for the physiological variables. This period is followed by a training unit. The training unit familiarizes the patient with the task. Then, the main test is initiated, and the first period is a challenge in which the patient must perform the task as quickly as possible; the matrix presentation depends on the patient’s pace of responding. For correct and incorrect answers, patients receive visual feedback at the bottom of the computer screen. A green smiley face is shown for correct answers. For incorrect answers, a red glum emoticon arises, and the patient receives an acoustic signal. The next two periods are complex task periods that are characterized by the computer’s algorithm, which is a matrix presentation that accelerates based on how quickly the patient responds, resulting in stress or distress: The patient has no influence on the task’s timeframe. The computer calculates an algorithm that sets the pace and is based on achieving the maximum 40% correct answers. The participant is instructed that his peer group, on average, responds correctly 60% in the complex task periods. This discrepancy is an additional stress component (a social reference in addition to a loss of control for performance to create a defining criterion of distress). After these two forced task periods, there is an additional period in which the patient proceeds at his own pace and a second rest period prior to the test completion.

The experimental session lasted approximately 40 minutes (minimum 30 min. & maximum 60 min). After arriving at the laboratory, patients were provided with a standardized introduction about the test procedure and the computer simulation. Immediately prior to beginning the stress test, the examiner, who was a psychologist, checked the placement of the line cables that were used for the physiological parameters (heart rate, blood pressure, respiration, electro dermal activation and the muscle tension—see the following paragraph). After the test and removing the test equipment, patients are provided with detailed analyses about their PPST results with cautious interpretations by the examiner. Together, the patient and examiner identify patterns that resemble coping strategies or physiological stress reactions and develop and discuss possible ambulatory treatment options.

Physiological parameters

The PPST software runs on a standard desktop computer and presents all instructions, questions, visual analogue scales and visual search tasks to the patient while controlling data acquisition via a serial connection (RS232) to the data-acquisition computer. The data are stored as ASCII files and are processed offline with Excel 2007 to produce the test report. The ECG (Electrocardiography) was recorded with a modified Einthoven II Lead that used Kendall H34SG electrodes. The EMG (Electromyography) was recorded as bipolar with Kendall H124SG electrodes. EDA (Electrodermal activity) was recorded thenar and hypothenar (the palm of left hand) with Kendall H34SD electrodes. Respiration was recorded with a respiration belt. These signals were measured by a Nexus10 device (MindMedia Inc., NL) that sampled at 1024 Hz and transmitted to the data-acquisition computer via bluetooth. Blood pressure and pulse were continuously measured from the left middle finger with an Ohmeda 2300 Finapres device. Every 2 minutes, the arterial blood pressure from the right arm was auscultatorily measured with a Dinamap 1846sx. Both blood pressure measuring devices send their data to the data-acquisition computer via a serial connection.

The recording software was programmed with DasyLab 8.0.4. The ECG was high-pass (1 Hz) filtered and processed to RR-intervals to compute the heart rate and RMSSD (root mean square of successive differences, HRV—Heart Rate Variance). The three EMG signals were high-pass (20 Hz) filtered and processed with a root mean square (RMS) algorithm that had a time constant of 125 ms. EDA was recorded as skin conductance level (SCL) and was high-pass (0.5 Hz) filtered to obtain the skin conductance response (SCR). The respiration signal was high-pass (0.6 Hz) filtered, and the breathing depth and rate were computed online. Artefacts were automatically removed by a plausibility algorithm. All signals were down sampled to 4 Hz and recorded as ASCII files. These files were then processed with Excel 2007 to compute the physiological data’s means and standard deviations and to produce the graphical reports.

Psychological parameters

Psychological data included patients’ self-reported estimations of task performance, emotions, motivations and physical discomfort. Likert scale items were used to record emotions and motivations and patients’ ideal and real expectations for the ongoing testing process. As mentioned above, these data were collected between the challenge periods. The psychological data summarizes how patients behave or cope in challenging situations (stress) that are similar to the special challenges that are presented in the PPST.

Statistical analysis

All physiological and psychological variables were analyzed as dependent variables in a univariate ANOVA for repeated measures (within subject design—1x 7) to assess differences between the test-periods (the independent variables were the arrangements of stress test periods: rest periods 1 & 2, challenge at ones’ own pace 1 & 2; and forced task from the computer algorithm with a social reference / stress / distress 1 & 2). First, we assessed whether condition had an overall influence on the variable. Then, we analyzed contrasts to test differences between subsequent periods.

If the sphericity assumption was not met, the Greenhouse-Geisser approach was used to adjust the degrees of freedom, additionally we employed the Bonferroni adjustment for the multiple comparisons. To investigate if the medical condition of the participants has an impact to the results of the PPST, we conducted a mixed repeated-measures ANOVA with medical condition (main diagnoses) as a between—subject factor and all the test parameters as well as the test periods as within-subject factors. All analyses were conducted using SPSS Version 21.

For the physiological values that were collected during 2-min PPST periods, we computed the means per period for the SCL, SCR, three EMG signals from right sided m. trapezius, left sided m. masseter and m. frontalis, systolic and diastolic finger cuff blood pressure (SFB resp. DFB) and for heart rate (HR) and RMSSD from the ECG (HRV). Additionally, the Ohmeda 2300 Finapres was used to compute the pulse rate (PR), which was used for non-artefact-free ECG. The EDA was used to obtain the SCL and SCR means. For respiration, we used the means for each 2-min period of breathing amplitude (RA) and respiration rate (RR).

Sample

We enrolled 139 unselected inpatients at treatment admission (i.e., internistic therapy and physiotherapy, as well as both individual and group psychotherapy, music and art therapy and body centered psychotherapy) from the Division of General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine, Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany. Participants represented the average diagnostic spectrum range in our division. We found no significant differences between our study sample and all other inpatients during this study’s time period on the ‘Patient Health Questionnaire’ (PHQ) [18]; the ‘Perceived Stress Questionnaire’ (PSQ) [19]; and the COPE [20]. A descriptive summary of our sample is provided in Table 1. According to our medical doctors or clinical psychologist’s prescriptions, participating in a stress test was part of the clinical schedule for evaluating stress reactions. All patients agreed in written form to the use of their secondary data for clinical routine studies on admission to the Center for Internal Medicine and Dermatology, Division for General Internal and Psychosomatic Medicine. This study was approved by the Vote of the Charité ethics committee (EA1/114/10).

Results

Results for physiological and psychological parameters—Tables 2, 3, 4 and 5

thumbnail
Table 2. Performance parameters: Repeated measures ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Physiological parameters: Repeated measures ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Psychological parameters: Repeated measures ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Expectations and valence parameters: Repeated measures ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.t005

For all PPST periods (the varied conditions), means and standard deviations for the different variables (physiological and psychological parameters) and the p-values from the ANOVAs (indicated with bent arrows) are presented in Tables 2, 3, 4 and 5.

Results of the mixed repeated-measures ANOVA

The overall results of the mixed repeated-measures ANOVA showed no significant differences between the total group of participants and the group of participants with either F30, F40 or F50 diagnoses. But we investigated significant within—subject factors. The following Figs (211) show the significant differences. They are marked with an asterisk.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Respiration rate (total participants; F30-yes; F30-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Breath-depth (total participants; F30-yes; F30-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. EMG-masseter (total participants, F30-yes; F30-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Heart-rate (total participants, F40-yes; F40-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. EMG-masseter (total participants; F40-yes; F40-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g006

thumbnail
Fig 7. Anger about the tasks (total participants; F40-yes; F40-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. Anger about the tasks (total participants; F50-yes; F50-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. The tasks are annoying (total participants; F50-yes; F50-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g009

thumbnail
Fig 10. Relevance of failure (total participants; F40-yes; F40-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g010

thumbnail
Fig 11. Relevance of failure (total participants; F50-yes; F50-no).

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g011

Results summary

In this sample, the PPST showed the most prominent physiological reactions in the cardiovascular system (blood pressure and heart rate) throughout the examination (Table 3). Further, we found that the different testing conditions provoked adjustments at the different psychological levels (cognitive: performance, expectations, and valence) (Tables 2 and 5). Hence, comparing the results for the challenge periods at the beginning and end of the test shows that in the 2nd challenge period (after both complex task periods), patients’ estimations of performance, ideal and real expectations significantly decreased. Further, to appraise the different testing conditions, we assume that the PPST evokes a stress experience. According to Lazarus [22], the individual´s appraisal of the stress-provoking situation is essential. The uncontrollable complex task testing periods negatively influence patients’ self-estimations of their performance; thus, the valences and expectations decline significantly. For the physiological parameters, the statistical analysis shows significantly increased activity for the cardiovascular system reactions (systolic blood pressure, heart rate) in the complex task periods, whereas for the other physiological variables (EMG, breath depth, respiration rate, diastolic blood pressure) differences in the complex task periods are not significant.

Case example

We show the graphs of a 19-year-old female student who was diagnosed with anorexia nervosa—ICD 10: F50.0—(BMI 14 kg/m2) that lasted for six months. The patient reported that she is under permanent pressure to perform her studies but is not able to achieve good results due to her eating disorder as well as her physiological and psychological condition. Her stress reactions and patterns show noticeable psychophysiological patterns (Tables 6 & 7 Figs 12 & 13), which are obscured when the data are shown at the aggregate level because each patient reacts differently. The patient’s treatment attempted to ameliorate her health complaints with psychotherapy in single and group setting, and creative therapy (i.e., music therapy, art therapy, relaxation techniques, and body perception centered therapy). Inpatient treatment lasted for approximately three months.

thumbnail
Table 7. Expectation and valence data from the case report.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.t007

thumbnail
Fig 12. Patient M.M..; 19 y.; ICD 10: F50.0: Blood pressure, heart rate, respiration rate and RMSSD.

Challenge 1st and 2nd: tasks with self-determined speed without a social comparison. Complex 1st and 2nd tasks with the computer-determined speed provided a comparison to an unrealistically high social ‘norm’. Blood pressure values by Finapres leads to falsely high values—since gauging is missing—and are suited only for depicting an uninterrupted course. Therefore some measurements of blood pressure via the riva rocci method are taken additionally for clinical comparison: 1st period of rest– 88/62 mmHg, 1st complex task– 103/65 mmHg, 2nd complex task– 99/60 mmHg, 2nd period of rest– 89/56 mmHg.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g012

thumbnail
Fig 13. Patient M.M.; 19 y.; ICD 10: F50.0: Muscle tension—m. frontalis, m. masseter, m. trapezius, and electro dermal activity (SCL).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g013

Due to the bad physical condition of the patient, we expected a delayed adaption for almost all of the physiological variables. Concerning the different dimensions of the psychological levels, we expected differences between her real performance and her self-estimation of performance, as well as a large discrepancy between the real and ideal expectations, because it is well known that patients who suffer from anorexia nervosa are characterized by a sense of ineffectiveness and perfectionism [23]. Additionally, we expected that success and failure would be almost equally important for the patient.

We assumed that there would be differences between the physiological reactions and her appraisal of her own body perception, because of the body image failure, which is a criterion of anorexia nervosa. Because of the need of self-control, we expected that the patient would report a higher fear of failure in the complex task periods than in the challenge periods.

Measuring performance and the ability to concentrate

The patient processed 14 matrices in the 1st challenge period (average 12.5 +/- 3.92), with the best performance found at 86% correct answers. According to the test construction, performance in both complex task periods is lower than in both challenge periods. In the 2nd challenge phase, the patient processed again 14 matrices with 12 correct solutions (86%), which was the exact same performance as in the 1st challenge period. This result indicates that this patient did not experience training effects or improvements in concentration during the test. In all test periods the patient estimates her performance lower than she achieved.

Physiological parameters

The continuously measured systolic and diastolic blood pressure values show a parallel course, with increases from the 1st rest period until the 2nd complex task. From the 2nd complex task period until the 2nd rest period, the levels decrease.

The heart rate shows almost the same course as the blood pressure, with an earlier climax in the 1st complex task phase. The RMSSD (HRV) only had slight fluctuations throughout the examination, with a climax in the 1st rest period and the 2nd complex task period. Respiration rate had the highest value in the 1st complex task period. The continuously measured systolic and diastolic blood pressure values are high for a woman of her age who has an anorexia nervosa diagnosis with a BMI of 14 kg/m2. These values are higher compared to the values of the other patients of our study (Table 3).

The m. trapezius activity increases from the 1st rest period to the 1st complex task period and then decreases until the 2nd complex task period, holds this level in the 2nd challenge phase, and then steeply declines in the 2nd rest period to a lower level than in the 1st rest phase. This may be seen as a ‘delayed adaptive’ curve: after activation up to a climax, the course does not show a continuously progressive decline but instead creates a ‘shoulder’. In comparison, an ‘early adaptive’ course occurs when a continuously progressive decline follows activation, which we could find for respiration rate, heart rate, and systolic and diastolic blood pressure (see above).

The m. frontalis had the highest activity in the 2nd complex task period, after which the activity continuously declines until the 2nd rest period. Waterink & van Boxtel [24] postulated that the facial EMG uninterruptedly increases when the participant’s performance is stable. The m. frontalis’ (Fig 13) course shows an uninterrupted increase until the 2nd complex task period, when the patient’s performance is stable (Table 6).

The m. masseter’s course is marked by alternating steep increases and declines throughout the test.

Skin conductance level (SCL) continuously increases until the 2nd complex task period with only slight decrease thereafter, representing a ‘delayed adaptive’ curve.

Emotional experiencing

The patient reports scarce physical reactions. (Fig 14) She stated ‘a little’ tenseness after the rest periods. Throughout the test, the patient identified ‘a little’ muscle tension or other body-related discomforts.

thumbnail
Fig 14. Patient M.M.; 19 y.; ICD 10: F50.0: Psychological data graphs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.g014

For the ‘tasks challenge me’ question, she reported ‘considerably’ after the 1st complex task period and ‘a little’ for all other periods. She did not report ‘annoyance’ except for the complex task periods.

Anger about her performance was reflected by reporting the maximum values for ‘predominantly and completely,’ whereas anger related to the tasks was rated lower and reported as ‘not at all’ in the second half of the test. She reported the minimum values (‘not at all, a little’) for being pleased with her success and reported a considerable fear of failure.

The patient reports maximal ideal expectations throughout the test. Contrasting her real expectations show low values. When the computer algorithm was activated and the instructions indicated that her peer group performed at approximately 60% correct, her ideal expectation remains high level (80% or more). In contrast, her real expectation declines to 20% and 10%. Therefore, her ideal and real expectations sharply diverge across testing periods. The importance of success is rated with the maximum values throughout the test. The values for the importance of failure fluctuate heavily, with the highest values (95%) after the 1st and before the 2nd challenge phases and the lowest value (29%) before the 1st complex task.

Case example summary

We found a ‘delayed adaption’ in activation of the m. trapezius and m. masseter, whereas the other physiological variables showed an ‘early adaptive’ course (Figs 12 and 13). Thus, we assume that the patient perceives her own physiological body reactions (Fig 14) differently from the physiological measures (Figs 12 and 13). The body perception impairment reflects a body image failure. Her performance self-estimation and (real) performance values show differences. The patient reported high levels for fear of failure. We view the psychological variables as person-related—internal factors, including ‘I am angry about my performance,’ ‘tasks challenge me,’ ‘I am pleased with my success’ and ‘I have a fear of failure’, compared with external factors, such as ‘tasks annoy me’ and ‘I am angry about the tasks’. This patient scores higher on internal factors, which may indicate a need for person-related coping strategies to better adjust to the varied stress periods. The discrepancy between real- and ideal expectations is large and success is important to the patient (high importance of success). In contrast the importance failure looks down-regulated, which further reflects her internal person- related coping strategies. Consequently, we stressed the importance of relaxation techniques and encouraged the patient to practice these techniques and gain experience in body centered creative therapy (i.e., music & art therapy).

Discussion

The PPST is supposed to reflect psychophysiological reactions in an artificial situation where challenging tasks must be performed under time pressure, which may be comparable with daily life stress, e.g., at work. There is a large body of literature that postulates the problem of transferability of results of experimental studies and observations of people’s behavior in real life [2528]. Stress examinations in laboratory situations are unavoidably reductionistic; therefore, it is complicated to transfer the results into the daily lives of patients. Most patients are involved in daily life with different stressors simultaneously (i.e., psychosocial stressors)–the so-called daily hassles [29]. However, we must consider that inpatient treatment and waiting for the results of the physiological tests may induce comparable stress reactions. Hence, to be in inpatient treatment has an influence on physiological and psychological systems. Patients with distress experiences in daily life are often exhausted, and their treatment should tend to recover their impairments. The reading, analysis and interpretation of the PPST results are supposed to serve or to promote patients’ introspection and self-reflection regarding their individual bio-psycho-social conditions. The encouragement of introspection and self-reflection can be an advantage concerning the further ambulatory treatment. That is, the better the patient knows his/her patterns of stress reaction, the better he/she applies discussed coping strategies in his/her daily life. Our results show that the PPST allows psychophysiological stress reactions to be measured in a laboratory situation, even though our results cannot represent general population. Previous studies of the PPST—using our previous protocol that had four additional challenge periods—have shown that there are comprehensive influences concerning the reactions and stress patterns of special diagnostic groups such as depression or back pain [3033], we are now focused on reproducing the results with the new modified protocol.

We could demonstrate the PPST showed stress-related changes in several dimensions as results at the group level. However, PPST’s main advantage is its qualities at the individual level because it identifies stress patterns and has the potential to give the patients a better understanding of their psychophysiological condition and discussion of outpatient treatment options. Beyond the horizon of psychophysiological studies, the PPST may be helpful in approaches of so called mindfulness. Because the PPST provokes psychophysiological stress reactions, as shown here, the PPST can be used as a standardized instrument in stress diagnostics, and its need for only one examiner is beneficial concerning the personal resources in clinical departments and practice.

Limitations

We adapted the protocol into seven periods because the examination was extremely exhausting for the patients and because the investigators (psychologist, medical doctors) have less time resources for such a long experimental session. This modification could have an impact, or limitation on our group results because the timeframe may be too short to provoke stronger stress reactions and the following psychophysiological adaptation process or delayed adaptations are not comprehensively demonstrated in the group statistic. Due to the clinical routine, the participants performed the test not exactly at the same time—two-thirds of the patients performed the test in the afternoon and one-third in the morning. This may be a contributing factor in terms of our results. Another limitation of our study may be that we have no healthy group sample for comparison of our results, this is concerning in terms of the validity of the PPST. Our results illustrate patients with a likely impaired psychophysiological condition. The participation in the PPST was prescribed by our medical doctors and psychologist for diagnosing stress reactions, we had no inclusion or exclusion criteria to control the variance of our participants, and this might have also an influence on our statistical analysis.

Outlook

For the next future, we intend to combine the PPST as a performance-centered test with an evaluation of primarily emotional challenge / stress in a creative therapeutical setting, such as music therapy, to elicit / provoke different stimuli-specific reactions in patients. So, the patients undergo a wider range of stress experience and the analysis of stress patterns and stress reactions are probably more comprehensive. Since prior research indicated that psychophysiological stress reactions and patterns are individual, we will examine in the next step whether individuals from a healthy control group and special disease groups differently react to the test.

Supporting information

S1 File. Background data which was used for the analysis is publicly available.

Please see corresponding file.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187859.s001

(SAV)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Felix Fischer and Mag. Gregor Liegl for their help in preparing the data and their advice for the statistical analysis and Dr. Immo Curio, Dipl.-Phys. for providing the software of the PPST and the technical support.

References

  1. 1. Henningsen P., Zimmermann T., Sattel , & Sattel H. (2003). Medically Unexplained Physical Symptoms, Anxiety, and Depression: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychosomatic Medicine, 528–533. pmid:12883101
  2. 2. Reid S., Wessely S., Crayford T., & Hotopf M. (2002). Frequent attenders with medically unexplaind symptoms: service use and costs in secundary care. British Journal of Psychiatry, 248–253.
  3. 3. Burch A., & Allen M. (2014). Stress task specific impairments of cardiovascular functioning in obese participants. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 1–8.
  4. 4. Sugaya N., Izawa S., Kimura K., Ogawa N., Yamada K., Shirotsuki K., et al. (2012). Adrenal hormone response and psychophysiological correlates under psychological stress in individuals with irritable bowel syndrome. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 39–44.
  5. 5. Tureba A., & Ritz T. (2013). Stress, asthma, and respiratory infections: Pathways ivolving airway immunology and microbial endocrinology. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 11–27.
  6. 6. Bibbey A., Carroll D., Roseboom T. J., Phillips A. C., & de Rooij S. (2013). Personality and physiological reactions to acute psychological stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 28–36.
  7. 7. Iliceto P., Pompili M., Spencer-Thomas S., Ferracuti S., Erbuto D., Lester D., et al. (2013). Occupational stress and psychopathology in health professionals: An explorative study with the Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) model approach. Stress, 143–152. pmid:22632290
  8. 8. Kreibig S. D., Samson A. C., & Gross J. J. (2013). The psychophysiology of mixed emotional states. Psychophysiology, 799–811. pmid:23730872
  9. 9. Mutsuura H., Kanbara K., Fukunaga M., Yamamoto K., Ban I., Kitamura K., et al. (2009). Depression and Anxiety Correlate Differently with Salivary Free Cortisol in the Morning in Patients with Functional Somatic Syndrome. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 291–298. pmid:19662526
  10. 10. Selye H. (1973). The Evolution of the stress concept. The originator of the concept traces its development from the discovery in 1936 of the alarm reaction to modern therapeutic applications of syntoxic and catatoxic hormones. American Scientist, 692–699. pmid:4746051
  11. 11. Selye H. (1975). Stress and Distress. Comprehensive Therapy, 9–13.
  12. 12. Crum A., Salovey P., & Achor S. (2013). Rethinking Stress: The Role of Mindsets in Determining the Stress Response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 716–733. pmid:23437923
  13. 13. Lazarus R. S., & Folkman S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: NY:Springer.
  14. 14. Kirschbaum C., Pirke K., & Hellhammer D. (1993). “The Trier Social Stress Test“–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 76–81. pmid:8255414
  15. 15. Kirschbaum C., Diedrich O., Gherke J., Wüst S., & Hellhammer D. (1992). Cortisol and behavior: The 'Trier Mental Challenge Test' (TMCT): First evaluation of a new psychological stress test. Perspectives and promises of clinical psychology. In Ehlers A., Fiegenbaum W., Florin I., & Margraf J., Plenum Press (67–78). New York.
  16. 16. Kolotylova T., Koschke M., Bär K.-J., Ebner-Priemer U., Kleindienst N., Bohus M., et al. (2010). Entwicklung des „Mannheimer Multikomponenten-Stress-Test”(MMST) Development of the “Mannheimer Multikomponenten-Stress-Test”(MMST). Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychologie, 64–72. German. pmid:19247924
  17. 17. Hellhammer J., & Schubert M. (2012). The physiological response to Trier Social Stress Test relates to subjective measures of stress during but not before or after the. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 119–124. pmid:21689890
  18. 18. Rief W., Nanke A., Klaiberg A., & Brähler E. (2004). Base rates for panic and depression according to the Brief Patient Health Questionnaire (Brief PHQ): a population-based study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 271–276. pmid:15488257
  19. 19. Fliege H., Rose M., Arck P., Levenstein S., & Klapp B. (2001). Validierung des "Perceived Stress Questionnaire" (PSQ) an einer deutschen Stichprobe. Validation of the “Perceived Stress Questionnaire” (PSQ) within a german sample. Diagnostica, 510–515. German.
  20. 20. Carver C. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol's too long: consider the brief COPE. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 92–100. pmid:16250744
  21. 21. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision (ICD-10). (2015). WHO.
  22. 22. Lazarus R. (1974). Psycholoigcal stress and coping in adaptation and illness. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 321–333. pmid:4618837
  23. 23. Fairburn C., Shafran R., & Cooper Z. (1999). A cognitive behavioral theory of anorexia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1–13. pmid:9922553
  24. 24. Waterink W., & van Boxtel A. (1994). Facial an jaw-elevator EMG activity in relation to changes in performance level during a sustained information processing task. Biological Psychology, 183–98.
  25. 25. Lazarus R. (1963). A Laboratory Approach to the Dynamics of Psychological Stress. Administrative Science Quarterly, 192–213.
  26. 26. Frederikson M., Blumenthal J., Evans D.D., Sherwood A. & Light K.C. (1989). Cardiovascular responses in the laboratory and in the natural environment: Is blood pressure reactivity to laboratory-induced mental stress related to ambulatory blood pressure during everyday life? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 753–762. pmid:2621677
  27. 27. Pollak M. (1994). Heart Rate Reactivity to Laboratory Tasks and in Two Daily Life Settings. Psychosomatic Medicine, 271–276. pmid:8084975
  28. 28. Bussmann J. B., Ebner-Priemer U. W., & Fahrenberg J. (2009). Ambulatory Activity Monitoring. Progress in Measurement of Activity, Posture, and Specific Motion Patterns in Daily Life. European Psychologist, 142–152.
  29. 29. Klapp B. F., & Peters E. M. (2010). Biologische Grundlagen der Anpassung und ihre Entwicklung—Einführung. In Adler H. W.H R, Uexküll, Psychosomatische Medizin. (43–49). Amsterdam: Elsevier (in press).
  30. 30. Hörhold M. (1994). Zur Psychophysiologie der Belastungsregulation: Verlaufsanalysen zum Einfluß psychologischer versus physikalischer Situationsmerkmale und psychologischer versus physiologischer Personenmerkmale. Frankfurt am Main, Berlin, Bern, New York, Paris, Wien: Peter Lang.
  31. 31. Gromen A. (2002). Biopsychologische Regulationsmechanismen bei psychosomatischen Patientinnen ‘mit’ versus ‘ohne’ Rückenschmerzen während psychischer und moderater körperlicher Laborbelastung. Berlin: Inauguraldissertation, Charité–Universitätsmedizin.
  32. 32. Gerhardt M. (2004). Psychophysiologische Parameter einer standardisierten Leistungssituation zum Verständnis des Bewältigungsverhalten psychosomatischer Patienten. Berlin: Inaugurationsdissertation—Charité—Universitätsmedizin.
  33. 33. Feher R. (2013). Analyse der akuten Stressreaktion depressiver Patienten im Vergleich zu einer klinischen Kontrollgruppe in einer standardisierten Belastungssituation. Berlin: Inauguraldissertation, Freie und Humboldt Universität Medizinische Fakultät Charité.