Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Are current chronic fatigue syndrome criteria diagnosing different disease phenotypes?

  • Laura Maclachlan,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Current address: Department of Public Health and Community Medicine, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden

    Affiliation Institute of Cellular Medicine, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

  • Stuart Watson,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom, Northumberland Tyne and Wear NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

  • Peter Gallagher,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

  • Andreas Finkelmeyer,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

  • Leonard A. Jason,

    Roles Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation DePaul University, Chicago, IL, United States of America

  • Madison Sunnquist,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation DePaul University, Chicago, IL, United States of America

  • Julia L. Newton

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    julia.newton@ncl.ac.uk

    Affiliations Institute of Cellular Medicine, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom, Newcastle Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

Abstract

Importance

Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) is characterised by a constellation of symptoms diagnosed with a number of different polythetic criteria. Heterogeneity across these diagnostic criteria is likely to be confounding research into the as-yet-unknown pathophysiology underlying this stigmatised and debilitating condition and may diagnose a disease spectrum with significant implications for clinical management. No studies to date have objectively investigated this possibility using a validated measure of CFS symptoms–the DePaul Symptom Questionnaire (DSQ).

Objective

To examine whether current CFS diagnostic criteria are identifying different disease phenotypes using the DSQ.

Design

Case control study.

Setting

Clinical Research Facility of the Royal Victoria Infirmary, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.

Participants

49 CFS subjects and ten matched, sedentary community controls, excluded for co-morbid depression.

Main outcomes and measures

Self-reported autonomic and cognitive features were assessed with the Composite Autonomic Symptom Score (COMPASS) and Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (COGFAIL) respectively. Objective autonomic cardiovascular parameters were examined using the Task Force® Monitor and a battery of neuropsychological tests administered for objective cognitive assessment.

Results

Self-reported autonomic and cognitive symptoms were significantly greater in CFS subjects compared to controls. There were no statistically significant differences in objective autonomic measures between CFS and controls. There were clinically significant differences between DSQ subgroups on objective autonomic testing. Visuospatial memory, verbal memory and psychomotor speed were significantly different between DSQ subgroups.

Conclusions and relevance

The finding of no significant differences in objective autonomic testing between CFS and control subjects may reflect the inclusion of sedentary controls or exclusion for co-morbid depression. Consistent exclusion criteria would enable better delineation of these two conditions and their presenting symptoms. Findings across CFS subgroups suggest subjects have a different disease burden on subjective and objective measures of function, autonomic parameters and cognitive impairment when categorised using the DSQ. Different CFS criteria may at best be diagnosing a spectrum of disease severities and at worst different CFS phenotypes or even different diseases. This complicates research and disease management and may contribute to the significant stigma associated with the condition.

Introduction

Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) is a disabling and stigmatising illness of unknown aetiology. Its current UK prevalence is estimated to be at least 0.2–0.4%[1]; studies using broader screening procedures have yielded prevalence estimates of over 2%[2]. CFS is a diagnosis of exclusion based on a number of different sets of polythetic diagnostic criteria developed empirically over the last three decades to reflect the many presenting symptoms. The diagnostic and prognostic salience of the different signs and symptoms is unknown and this inclusion of multiple symptoms involving many body systems complicates clinical diagnosis and is likely to add to the stigma associated with the condition.

It is widely recognised that there are limitations to these diagnostic sets and that there is disparity between them[37]. The different sets may identify different categorical groups differing in phenotypic pattern and which can be conceptualised as subtypes of CFS or even different disorders or may relate to a continuum across which different thresholds are arbitrarily met dependent on diagnostic schedule used[8, 9].

The DePaul Symptom Questionnaire (DSQ) was devised by Leonard Jason et al as a development of the CFS Questionnaire[10] to better, and more consistently, assess the “core” symptoms of CFS. Frequency and severity of symptoms, demographics, and medical, occupational, and social history are self-reported over a retrospective six-month period using a 5-point Likert scale to give a “diagnosis” of CFS based on the Fukuda criteria[11], the 2003 Canadian clinical[12] or research[13] criteria and the 2011 Canadian consensus criteria [14] (Table 1). It has been shown to have good inter-rater and test-retest reliability and validity in the differentiation of individuals with CFS, major depressive disorder (MDD), and healthy controls[1517].

Autonomic dysfunction in CFS has consistently been reported in studies[19], as has a potential sequela cognitive dysfunction [20]. However, the nature of these finding lacks uniformity, which may be related to heterogeneity within CFS patients or to systematic differences in sample selection between studies based on differing inclusion criteria, diagnostic rigour or recruitment strategy. Here we report subjective and objective measures of disease severity, autonomic dysfunction and cognitive impairment in CFS subjects and matched comparators, as well as the impact of DSQ categorisation between subgroups, to elicit whether different diagnostic criteria identify a spectrum of disease phenotypes.

Materials and methods

Recruitment

Participants were recruited as part of a Medical Research Council funded study Understanding the pathogenesis of autonomic dysfunction in chronic fatigue syndrome and its relationship with cognitive impairment. Recruitment was conducted between November 2012 and April 2014. CFS subjects were recruited from the Newcastle upon Tyne and regional specialist CFS services. All met Fukuda criteria as diagnosed by physician via clinical interview and confirmed with administration of the DSQ during study screening. Matched sedentary controls were recruited via university volunteer databases, advertisements and word of mouth. Participants with co-morbid hypertension or psychiatric disorder diagnosed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Research Version were excluded. Cardioactive medications were withheld for 72 hours prior to assessment. Patients and controls were matched for age, gender, employment or education status and premorbid IQ (using the validated National Adult Reading Test.

The MRC study was conducted in accordance with the recommendations for physicians involved in research on human subjects adopted by the 18th World Medical Assembly, Helsinki 1964 and later revisions. Informed consent was obtained.

Favourable ethical opinion was obtained from the NRES Committee North East—Newcastle & North Tyneside 2 prior to commencement of the study. Local research and development approval was obtained from Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust’s Research and Development office. All participants provided written, informed consent.

DePaul symptom questionnaire

The DSQ is a self-reported 99-item questionnaire measuring CFS symptoms, demographic information and medical, social and occupational history according to the Canadian Clinical Criteria, the ME International Consensus Criteria, and the Fukuda criteria[16, 21]. It is available in the shared library of Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap), hosted at DePaul University: https://redcap.is.depaul.edu/surveys/?s=tRxytSPVVw. After scoring for symptom frequency and severity, patients are categorised into one of four criteria groups: the Fukuda; the Fukuda+2003 Canadian Clinical; the Fukuda+2003 Canadian Research, or the Fukuda+2003+2011 Canadian (Table 1). Those meeting the Fukuda+2003 Clinical differ from the Fukuda+2003 Research in that they do not meet the more stringent Research criteria requiring the presence of major symptoms.

Other questionnaires

The Fatigue Impact Scale (FIS) assesses the impact that subjective fatigue has on daily functioning[22]. Forty items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale providing a continuous scale of 0–160. A higher score indicates greater impact.

Participants completed the Autonomic Symptom Profile[23] as a self-report measure of autonomic symptoms. However, scoring was performed according to the recently abbreviated and psychometrically improved version of this questionnaire, the Composite Autonomic Symptom Score 31 (COMPASS 31)[24]. Scoring consists of 31 items from six domains–orthostatic intolerance, vasomotor, secretomotor, gastrointestinal, bladder, pupillomotor–each weighted according to number of items and clinical relevance. Weighted individual domain scores are totalled to a maximum of 100, indicating greater symptom load.

The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (COGFAIL)[25] comprises 25 questions measuring the frequency of self-reported failures in perception, memory and motor function in the previous six months and is answered on a 5-point Likert scale giving a possible total score of 100. Higher scores indicate greater impairment.

There is currently no standardised way to measure self-reported activity levels. The short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) gives a measure of physical activity over the preceding seven days[26] and has been shown to be a reliable measure of self-reported physical activity[27]. The questionnaire asks about three types of activity: walking, moderate-intensity activities and vigorous-intensity activities, undertaken during leisure time, domestic and gardening activities, work-related physical activity and transport-related physical activity. The duration (minutes) and frequency (days) of activity is scored to give METs, multiples of resting metabolic rate, equated to levels of activity and described as low, moderate or high.

Participant characteristics

Demographic information, symptom burden, function and diagnostic information were obtained by participant report and questionnaire, as above.

Autonomic assessment

Autonomic assessment was conducted at the Clinical Research Facility at the Royal Victoria Infirmary. The Task Force® Monitor (TFM, CNSystems Medizintechnik, Graz, Austria) was used to record and analyse continuous heart rate (HR) (electrocardiogram (ECG)) and beat-to-beat blood pressure (BP) assessment. It is a reliable and reproducible method of non-invasive autonomic assessment[28, 29].

Participants were instructed to eat a light breakfast, avoid caffeine and alcohol on the day of testing and refrain from nicotine for two hours before assessment. All assessments were performed between 9–10am. TFM recordings were taken during a ten-minute supine rest, a two-minute active stand and a Valsalva manoeuvre.

Cognitive assessment

The battery of neuropsychological tests focussed on memory and concentration, shown to be features of cognitive impairment in CFS[30], and assessed verbal and non-verbal memory, recall, recognition and learning skills. Tests reported here are the Rey Auditory-verbal learning test (AVLT) to assess verbal learning and memory, the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) to assess psychomotor speed, Digit Span for working and short-term memory, Spatial Span for working and short-term dynamic visuospatial memory and the Visual Patterns Test (VPT) to assess short-term fixed visuospatial memory. Tests were conducted under time pressure and commenced immediately after autonomic assessment. Caffeine, alcohol and nicotine were avoided as above.

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed using GraphPad Prism version 6. The difference between CFS patients and control participants in normally-distributed continuous data was analysed using the unpaired t-test, at a 5% and 1% significance level, as indicated. Unpaired continuous skewed data were analysed using the non-parametric Mann Whitney U test. The differences between the four patient sub-groups according to DSQ criteria was analysed using ANOVA or the Kruskal-Wallis test for non-parametric datasets. A 5% significance level is given. For significant ANOVA findings Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc test results are reported. (In addition, as increasingly stricter criteria are applied to the four DSQ diagnostic groups, the assigned sub-group membership can be considered an ordinal variable. Accordingly, Kendall’s tau-b tests were performed to assess the strength of the monotonic relationship between diagnostic category and the various measures.)

Results

Study cohort

Seventy-six potential participants were invited for screening, of whom sixty-nine were eligible. Eight withdrew from the study; one was lost to follow-up and one commenced a new medication resulting in loss of eligibility. In total, 59 participants (CFS n = 49; controls n = 10) underwent autonomic and cognitive testing.

Participant characteristics

Patients and controls were well-matched with no significant differences between age, gender, employment or education status, or pre-morbid IQ (Table 2). Age range was of CFS participants was 23–68 years. Age range of controls was 25–65 years. Co-morbidities were self-reported by affected system and include cardiorespiratory, neurological, gastrointestinal and endocrine disease. The presence of co-morbidities was higher in the CFS group compared to controls (92% versus 70%). Among CFS participants, those meeting the Fukuda+2003 Research and Fukuda+2003+2011 criteria had lower educational attainment and were less likely to be in employment.

Symptom burden

CFS participants self-reported statistically significantly greater fatigue, cognitive impairment and autonomic symptoms (Table 3). IPAQ scores in both groups were lower than those seen in active, healthy volunteers in other studies, who have shown median scores of 4500–6000[31, 32], and reflect the sedentary lifestyle of participants in this study. Nevertheless, controls had significantly higher IPAQ scores than CFS subjects (1230 (IQR 821) v. 203 (1002)) which may represent the cumulative effect of nonstrenuous activity not achievable by CFS patients because of illness burden.

Across DSQ subgroups there are statistically significant differences in FIS and COMPASS 31 scores, and there was a statistical trend for a monotonic increase of self-reported cognitive impairment with increased DSQ category. Participants meeting the Fukuda+2003+2011 criteria had the highest subjective levels of fatigue, cognitive impairment and symptoms of dysautonomia compared to the other CFS diagnostic subgroups. The lowest scores are seen in the group meeting Fukuda alone.

Autonomic nervous system at rest

Objective testing did not reveal any statistically significant differences between CFS and control subjects (Table 4). Resting heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP) were at the lower end of the normal clinical reference range in both groups. Examination of HR and BP variability appeared to show a shift to increased sympathetic modulation in both groups compared to normal reference ranges. Baroreflex control was within normal limits in CFS and control subjects.

thumbnail
Table 4. Autonomic function (heart rate and blood pressure variability and beat statistics, active stand and Valsalva) by DSQ.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186885.t004

There were no statistically significant differences in HR and BP between DSQ subgroups. Fukuda alone subjects had the lowest HR and Fukuda+2003+2011 the highest. Although not statistically significant, mean measures in the Fukuda+2003+2011 group are overall lower for all parameters except TPRI. Measures in the Fukuda alone group are higher compared to other subgroups, also with the exception of TPRI.

Autonomic nervous system in response to standing and Valsalva

Neither mean BP nor nadir on standing was statistically or clinically significantly different and 30:15 ratio–an indicator of parasympathetic activity–was within normal parameters.

The CFS group has a statistically significantly longer Valsalva ratio compared to the control group; however, both groups had an abnormal ratio (<1.21) indicating dysautonomia. Low baseline blood pressure may help to explain these results.

Valsalva ratio was abnormal in all DSQ subgroups indicating dysautonomia. There are statistically significant between-group differences in Phases I and Iii, which may reflect parasympathetic activation by the baroreceptors[33].

Cognitive performance

Results are detailed by domain tested and shown in Table 5.

Memory: Verbal

There was a statistically significant inter-group difference in total recall over trials 1–5 on AVLT. Participants in the Fukuda+2003+2011 group underperformed (score 43.7) compared to the other groups–an indication of a less efficient verbal declarative memory–with those meeting a Fukuda+2003 Research ‘diagnosis’ performing best (53.0). The difference between these groups resulted in the only significant post-hoc test. Furthermore, the Fukuda+2003+2011 group also had the lowest percentage retention on trial A7 (70.5%) and the Fukuda+2003 Research group the highest (84.2%), but this was not statistically significant.

Memory: Visuospatial

There are statistically significant differences across all measures on spatial span and VPT at least at trend level. Participants in the Fukuda+2003+2011 group had the lowest scores in forward spatial span and VPT compared to the other subgroups. For forward spatial span the difference to the Fukuda only group reached statistical significance in post hoc tests. For VPT the difference to the two Fukuda+2003 groups was significant. This suggests this group has a deficit in immediate spatial memory compared to the others[34, 35]. Although the Fukuda+2003+2011 group also has the lowest score in backward spatial span among the patient subgroups and there is a difference between the subgroups in this measure at statistical trend level (p = 0.066), this seems to be driven more by higher scores in the Fukuda+2003 Research group (M = 5.9) than by the poor performance in the Fukuda+2003+2011 group, but none of the post-hoc tests reached statistical significance.

These findings suggest that visuospatial working memory, which allows temporary retention and manipulation of information[36] (spatial span) and short-term visuospatial memory (VPT)[37], differs by diagnostic criteria and may be impaired in participants meeting Fukuda+2003+2011 criteria. There was an effect of group on the number of symbols recorded per second on the DSST, with the fewest symbols completed in the Fukuda+2003+2011 group, which was statistically different from the Fukuda only group in post-hoc tests.

Discussion

CFS subjects reported significantly greater autonomic and cognitive impairment compared to matched, sedentary controls; however, findings did not show significant objective autonomic differences. There were significant differences in subjective cognitive and autonomic measures on DSQ categorisation, as well as clinical differences in objective assessment of autonomic and cognitive function. Possible interpretation of these results is discussed below.

CFS versus controls

The lack of statistically significant objective difference in autonomic function in this cohort of CFS and control subjects differs from previous studies that have demonstrated a difference in autonomic function[19, 38] and may reflect a number of factors. Firstly, sedentary controls were selected for participation. Previously demonstrated autonomic dysfunction in CFS subjects compared to controls may have arisen secondary to inactivity and consequent deconditioning.

Secondly, this study excluded participants with a history of co-morbid depression. Autonomic dysfunction has been found in depression[39]. It is possible that autonomic dysfunction seen in other studies results from co-morbid depression and is not a primary feature of CFS. Consistent exclusion criteria across studies and research centres would enable better delineation of these two conditions and their presenting symptoms.

Finally, it is feasible that autonomic dysfunction experienced by CFS subjects, manifesting as HR and BP abnormalities, is intermittent. Although assessment using the TFM provides a continuous measure it is conducted over approximately 20 minutes implying that intermittent symptoms may not be captured.

DSQ

Autonomic function measured using HR and BP variability assessed over a 10-minute rest suggests differences in the balance between sympathetic and parasympathetic autonomic function with Fukuda+2003 Research showing greater sympathetic activity and Fukuda+2003+2011 lower sympathetic modulation of cardiovascular activity.

LF/HF-sBP is highest in the Fukuda+2003 Research group suggesting a shift towards greater sympathetic activity in this group. It is lowest in the Fukuda+2003+2011 group and suggests lower sympathetic activity, further supported by a low LFnu-sBP indicative of low sympathetic modulation of sBP, and of a higher HFnu-sBP, suggesting greater parasympathetic modulation.

Although not statistically significant, this points towards the presence of different phenotypes across DSQ subgroups, suggesting that AD may differ by diagnostic criteria when considered at a clinical level.

The differences observed between DSQ subgroups may be a reflection of the additive effect in diagnostic criteria. The absence of autonomic symptoms in the Fukuda criteria implies a different, less severe, disease phenotype with fewer features of AD. In contrast, symptoms of AD are present in the 2003 criteria and include ataxia, muscle weakness and OI. Further still, the 2011 criteria have a requirement for a greater symptom burden, which can include widespread migratory pain and hyperalgesia.

The possibility that current criteria include symptoms that are not primary features of CFS, specifically in relation to the Canadian 2011 criteria which encompasses a broad spectrum of symptoms across body systems, and confounds clinical presentation and research, must be considered. The number of symptoms across many physiological symptoms included in the Canadian 2011 criteria may mean that, rather than diagnosing a more severe CFS phenotype, these criteria capture both CFS and other co-morbidities with non-specific symptoms. This may not only affect management and subsequent prognosis, but also give rise to an inaccurate and confused picture of which condition (or conditions) is being researched and serve to exacerbate the stigma associated with the condition.

Furthermore, it is possible that the inclusion of more widespread pain with or without hyperalgesia in the 2011 criteria may be a confounding symptom–particularly in view of the association between pain and both autonomic dysfunction and cognitive impairment (attention, psychomotor speed, verbal and working memory)[4042].

There was only a statistical trend for a monotonic relationship between DSQ subgroups and self-reported cognitive impairment with COGFAIL. Fukuda+2003+2011 subjects reported higher scores than all other subgroups, pointing towards greater impairment in this group.

Comparison of objective measures between DSQ diagnostic subgroups revealed statistically significant between-group differences in verbal memory–as assessed using the AVLT total score, visuospatial memory and psychomotor speed, with the Fukuda+2003+2011 subgroup consistently showing greater impairment, in particular than the Fukuda alone group. These findings mirror subjectively higher scores of cognitive impairment on COGFAIL, greater fatigue on FIS and more autonomic symptoms on COMPASS. Fukuda alone and Fukuda+2003 Clinical appeared to perform better.

Given the findings of the ANOVAs and the patterns of post-hoc results, there appears to be an additive effect in the DSQ criteria where more symptoms result in greater symptom burden and disease severity and points towards a subgroup of patients with greater functional impairment. The results of the monotonic trend tests (Kendall tau-b) often support this notion. These findings contribute to a picture of potentially clinically significant and distinct phenotypic differences between DSQ subgroups and the possibility of CFS as a disease spectrum.

One of the study strengths was that it recruited a highly-motivated cohort of CFS subjects who completed a comprehensive series of investigations enabling, for the first time, in-depth observation of the potential phenotypic differences between DSQ subgroups. There are a number of limitations to acknowledge including the small number of controls and small sample sizes in DSQ subgroups, which means the study is underpowered. In particular the comparisons between the full patient sample and the small control sample are likely to suffer from problems with variance inhomogeneity. Conducting clinical research in this group of patients presents challenges in recruiting adequate numbers and this study would have benefitted from additional control subjects to increase statistical validity. The study results point towards the possibility of a spectrum of disease phenotypes as shown across diagnostic criteria. These phenotypes appear to be quantitative in nature, i.e. of symptom/disease severity, however a small study population of CFS subjects well enough to attend several appointments and therefore likely to represent milder disease severity, mean this is difficult to characterise. Future studies would benefit from exploring the possibility that clusters of participants represent different disease phenotypes to better explore whether they represent discrete, qualitative traits. This study is observational and therefore disease causality cannot be determined.

Conclusions

Findings of no significant differences in objective autonomic parameters between CFS and control groups raises questions about the role of co-morbid depression and sedentary controls and strongly indicates that more consistent inclusion and exclusion criteria across studies is key to furthering understanding of CFS pathophysiology.

This is the first study investigating objective differences in autonomic and cognitive features across DSQ subgroups. It suggests that CFS–as classified using current diagnostic criteria–may constitute a disease spectrum, with different severities. This has potentially significant repercussions for further understanding of the underlying aetiopathogenesis of this debilitating condition, contributing to the sometimes conflicting and mixed results seen across studies, and implications for response to clinical management.

This study suggests that there are differences in objective autonomic parameters across groups of CFS subjects. Damage to the autonomic nervous system appears to take the form of an initial sympathetic over-modulation followed, in more severe disease, by sympathetic underactivity and increased parasympathetic modulation, as seen with subjects meeting the Fukuda+2003 Research and Fukuda+2003+2011 criteria respectively. This supports the theory that abnormalities in the autonomic nervous system are a potentially important feature of CFS and hold promise for better understanding the underlying pathophysiology of this condition.

This study highlights the need for international consensus with regard to diagnosis. The recent development of the Institute of Medicine criteria will go some way to achieving this but it is vital that there is consistency in diagnostic approach when performing research in this condition where novel therapies are so urgently needed.

Acknowledgments

Medical Research Council, ME Research UK

References

  1. 1. National Institute of Health and Care Excellence. Chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis (or encephalopathy) 2007 [cited 2014 2.6.14]. Available from: http://www.nice.org.uk/nicemedia/live/11824/36193/36193.pdf.
  2. 2. Reeves WC, Jones JF, Maloney E, Heim C, Hoaglin DC, Boneva RS, et al. Prevalence of chronic fatigue syndrome in metropolitan, urban, and rural Georgia. Population health metrics. 2007;5(5):5. pmid:17559660; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1904178.
  3. 3. Brurberg KG, Fonhus MS, Larun L, Flottorp S, Malterud K. Case definitions for chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis (CFS/ME): a systematic review. BMJ open. 2014;4(2):e003973. pmid:24508851; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3918975.
  4. 4. NHS Choices. Chronic fatigue syndrome—Diagnosis 2015 [cited 2015 6/8]. Available from: http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/Chronic-fatigue-syndrome/Pages/Diagnosis.aspx.
  5. 5. King C, Jason LA. Improving the diagnostic criteria and procedures for chronic fatigue syndrome. Biological psychology. 2005;68(2):87–106. pmid:15450690.
  6. 6. Morris G, Maes M. Case definitions and diagnostic criteria for Myalgic Encephalomyelitis and Chronic fatigue Syndrome: from clinical-consensus to evidence-based case definitions. Neuro endocrinology letters. 2013;34(3):185–99. pmid:23685416.
  7. 7. Clayton EW. Beyond myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome: an IOM report on redefining an illness. Jama. 2015;313(11):1101–2. pmid:25668027.
  8. 8. Jason LA, Brown A, Clyne E, Bartgis L, Evans M, Brown M. Contrasting case definitions for chronic fatigue syndrome, Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome and myalgic encephalomyelitis. Evaluation & the health professions. 2012;35(3):280–304. pmid:22158691; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3658447.
  9. 9. Jason LA, Helgerson J, Torres-Harding SR, Carrico AW, Taylor RR. Variability In Diagnostic Criteria For Chronic Fatigue Syndrome May Result In Substantial Differences In Patterns Of Symptoms And Disability. Evaluation & the health professions. 2003;26(1):3–22. pmid:12629919
  10. 10. Jason L, Ropacki M, Santoro N, Richman J, Heatherly W, Taylor R, et al. A Screening Instrument for Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Journal Of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. 1997;3(1):39–59.
  11. 11. Fukuda K, Straus SE, Hickie I, Sharpe MC, Dobbins JG, Komaroff A. The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Annals of internal medicine. 1994;121(12):953–9. pmid:7978722.
  12. 12. Carruthers BM, Jain AK, De Meirleir KL, Peterson DL, Klimas NG, Lerner AM, et al. Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome: Clinical working case definition, diagnostic and treatments protocols. Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. 2003;11:7–115.
  13. 13. Jason LA, Evans M, Porter N, Brown M, Brown A, Hunnell J, et al. DePaul University Fatigue Questionnaire 2010 [cited 2015 7/4]. Available from: http://www.iacfsme.org/Portals/0/pdf/kingquestionnaire.pdf.
  14. 14. Carruthers BM, van de Sande MI, De Meirleir KL, Klimas NG, Broderick G, Mitchell T, et al. Myalgic encephalomyelitis: International Consensus Criteria. Journal of internal medicine. 2011;270(4):327–38. pmid:21777306; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3427890.
  15. 15. Hawk C, Jason LA, Torres-Harding S. Reliability of a chronic fatigue syndrome questionnaire. Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. 2007;13(4):41–66.
  16. 16. Jason LA, So S, Brown AA, Sunnquist M, Evans M. Test–retest reliability of the DePaul Symptom Questionnaire. Fatigue: Biomedicine, Health & Behavior. 2015;3(1):16–32. pmid:26973799
  17. 17. Brown AA, Jason LA. Validating a measure of myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome symptomatology. Fatigue: Biomedicine, Health & Behavior. 2014;2(3):132–52. pmid:27213118
  18. 18. Jason LA, Evans M, Porter N, Brown M, Brown A, Hunnell J, et al. The development of a revised Canadian myalgic encephalomyelitis chronic fatigue syndrome case definition. American Journal of Biochemistry and Biotechnology. 2010;6(2):120–35.
  19. 19. Van Cauwenbergh D, Nijs J, Kos D, Van Weijnen L, Struyf F, Meeus M. Malfunctioning of the autonomic nervous system in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome: a systematic literature review. European Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2014;44(5):516–26. pmid:24601948.
  20. 20. Michiels V, Cluydts R. Neuropsychological functioning in chronic fatigue syndrome: a review. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2001;103(2):84–93. pmid:11167310.
  21. 21. Jason LA SM, Brown A,. Factor Analysis of the DePaul Symptom Questionnaire: Identifying Core Domains. Journal of neurology and neurobiology. 2015;1(4). pmid:27088131
  22. 22. Fisk JD, Ritvo PG, Ross L, Haase DA, Marrie TJ, Schlech WF. Measuring the functional impact of fatigue: initial validation of the fatigue impact scale. Clinical infectious diseases: an official publication of the Infectious Diseases Society of America. 1994;18 Suppl 1:S79–83. pmid:8148458.
  23. 23. Suarez GA, Opfer-Gehrking TL, Offord KP, Atkinson EJ, O'Brien PC, Low PA. The Autonomic Symptom Profile: a new instrument to assess autonomic symptoms. Neurology. 1999;52(3):523–8. pmid:10025781.
  24. 24. Sletten DM, Suarez GA, Low PA, Mandrekar J, Singer W. COMPASS 31: a refined and abbreviated Composite Autonomic Symptom Score. Mayo Clinic proceedings. 2012;87(12):1196–201. pmid:23218087; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3541923.
  25. 25. Broadbent DE, Cooper PF, FitzGerald P, Parkes KR. The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1982;21(1):1–16.
  26. 26. Anonymous. Guidelines for Data Processing and Analysis of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ). 2005.
  27. 27. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE, et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003;35(8):1381–95. pmid:12900694.
  28. 28. Fortin J, Habenbacher W, Gruellenberger R, Wach P, Skrabal F. Real-time monitor for hemodynamic beat-to-beat parameters and power spectra analysis of the biosignals. Proceedings of the 20th annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. 1998;1:360–3.
  29. 29. Fortin J, Haitchi G, Bojic A, Habenbacher W, Grullenberger R, Heller A, et al. Validation and Verification of the Task Force® Monitor. External paper: Results of clinical studies for FDA 510(k)no: K014063 2001.
  30. 30. Cockshell SJ, Mathias JL. Cognitive functioning in chronic fatigue syndrome: a meta-analysis. Psychological medicine. 2010;40(8):1253–67. pmid:20047703.
  31. 31. Wolin KY, Heil DP, Askew S, Matthews CE, Bennett GG. Validation of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire-Short Among Blacks. J Phys Act Health. 2008;5(5):746–60. pmid:18820348
  32. 32. Kurtze N, Rangul V, Hustvedt BE. Reliability and validity of the international physical activity questionnaire in the Nord-Trondelag health study (HUNT) population of men. BMC medical research methodology. 2008;8(63):63. pmid:18844976; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2577099.
  33. 33. He J, Hollingsworth KG, Newton JL, Blamire AM. Cerebral vascular control is associated with skeletal muscle pH in chronic fatigue syndrome patients both at rest and during dynamic stimulation. NeuroImage Clinical. 2013;2:168–73. pmid:24179772.
  34. 34. Gallagher P, Gray JM, Kessels RP. Fractionation of visuo-spatial memory processes in bipolar depression: a cognitive scaffolding account. Psychological medicine. 2014;45(3):1–14. pmid:25065455.
  35. 35. Gallagher P, Gray JM, Watson S, Young AH, Ferrier IN. Neurocognitive functioning in bipolar depression: a component structure analysis. Psychological medicine. 2014;44(5):961–74. pmid:23800475.
  36. 36. Cambridge Brain Sciences. Spatial Span [cited 2015 16/6]. Available from: http://www.cambridgebrainsciences.com/browse/memory/test/spatial-span-ladder.
  37. 37. Della Sala S, Gray C, Baddeley AD, Wilson L. Visual patterns test: a test of short-term visual recall: Thames Valley Test Company; 1997.
  38. 38. Meeus M, Goubert D, De Backer F, Struyf F, Hermans L, Coppieters I, et al. Heart rate variability in patients with fibromyalgia and patients with chronic fatigue syndrome: a systematic review. Seminars in Arthritis & Rheumatism. 2013;43(2):279–87. pmid:23838093.
  39. 39. Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Gray MA, Felmingham KL, Brown K, Gatt JM. Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: a review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(11):1067–74. pmid:20138254.
  40. 40. Gandhi RA, Marques JL, Selvarajah D, Emery CJ, Tesfaye S. Painful diabetic neuropathy is associated with greater autonomic dysfunction than painless diabetic neuropathy. Diabetes care. 2010;33(7):1585–90. pmid:20587724; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2890363.
  41. 41. Moriarty O, McGuire BE, Finn DP. The effect of pain on cognitive function: a review of clinical and preclinical research. Progress in neurobiology. 2011;93(3):385–404. pmid:21216272.
  42. 42. Mathias CJ. Autonomic diseases: clinical features and laboratory evaluation. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 2003;74 Suppl 3:iii31–41. pmid:12933912; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1765633.