Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Objective quantification of the food proximity effect on grapes, chocolate and cracker consumption in a Swedish high school. A temporal analysis

  • Billy Langlet ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    billy.langlet@ki.se

    Affiliation Division of Applied Neuroendocrinology, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Petter Fagerberg,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Applied Neuroendocrinology, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Andrew Glossner,

    Roles Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Internationella Engelska Gymansiet Södermalm, Internationella Engelska Skolan, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Ioannis Ioakimidis

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Applied Neuroendocrinology, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

Abstract

Close food proximity leads to increased short-term energy intake, potentially contributing to the long-term development of obesity. However, its precise effects on eating behaviour are still unclear, especially with food available for extended periods of time. This study involved two similar high school student groups (15–17 years old), which had ad libitum access to grapes, chocolates and crackers during an hour-long experimental session. In the distal condition the foods were placed 6 meters away from the students (n = 24), in contrast to the proximal condition (n = 17) were the food was placed near the students. The identification of the type and the quantification of the amount of each food selected, for each individual serving, was facilitated through use of food scales and video recording. In the proximal condition individuals served themselves grapes and crackers more often and consumed more chocolate than in the distal condition. In total, participants in the proximal condition ingested significantly more energy (726 kcal vs. 504 kcal; p = 0.029), without reporting higher fullness. Food proximity also affected the temporal distribution of servings, with the first five minutes of the sessions corresponding to 53.1% and 45.6% of the total energy intake for the distal and proximal conditions, respectively. After the first five minutes, the servings in the distal condition were strongly clustered in time, with many students getting food together. In the proximal condition however, students displayed an unstructured pattern of servings over time. In conclusion, this study strengthens past evidence regarding the important role of food proximity on individual energy intake and, for the first time, it associates continuous food proximity to the emergence of unstructured eating over time. These conclusions, expanded upon by future studies, could support the creation of meaningful intervention strategies based on spatially and temporally controlled food availability.

Introduction

Recent decades have seen a huge rise in obesity, with world prevalence having more than doubled in the adult population since 1980 [1]. The serious negative health effects of obesity are widely recognised [2], making obesity a significant financial burden for modern health care systems [3]. Weight gain occurs when energy intake is higher than energy expenditure [4], with small modifications in daily energy balance leading to significant weight changes over time [5]. High-energy foods, available at minimal physical effort, are certainly contributing to energy imbalance [6,7] and increased food availability is probably connected to both more frequent meal occurrences and increased meal size [8,9].

The determinants of food selection and consumption in naturalistic environments remain unclear [10,11], being difficult to quantify reliably, due to the predominant use of self-reporting [12] and the lack of quantitative data on eating behaviour patterns across a day [13]. However, recent research show positive correlations between availability of palatable foods on an individual's kitchen counter and their weight [14].

In controlled settings, short-term individual food intake can be modified by manipulating visual food cues [15], food packaging and plate sizes [1618] and one’s dining companions [19,20]. Additionally, increased food availability is positively correlated with spontaneous eating [21] and increased meal sizes [22], even if the precise effects are again unclear [23]. Furthermore, both adults [24] and children [25] seem to eat more when food is available at a close distance.

Another potential behavioural factor affecting energy balance is the temporal distribution of eating over time, usually studied in the format of meal timing and frequency across a day [26]. The exact role of regularity on food intake and long term energy balance remains ambiguous, but there is some evidence that unstructured eating might contribute to the unbalancing of energy intake [27]. However, these studies often focus on the generalised effects of meal skipping or time-shifting [28] and it is often noted that novel methodologies for the collection of proper temporal data are required [29]. Similarly, most food proximity studies focus on group measures and only report cumulative effects at discrete time points, without accounting for temporal variations of individual eating behaviour [24,25,30], potentially ignoring the temporal effects of prolonged direct food availability on overall intake. Once more, for the temporal analysis of those effects on an individual level, more sophisticated measuring methodologies are required [28].

On the other hand, temporal analysis in single-meal individual eating behaviour, when quantified through the microstructural behavioural analysis [31] has proven valuable for the definition of individual eating styles associated with the development of obesity [32]. Similar methods have also been adopted very successfully in clinical settings for the treatment of eating disorders [33] and obesity [34]. Furthermore, past methodologies of temporal parallel analysis of weight-loss and video data used in the lab to fully describe single meals [35], have recently been adapted to the naturalistic analysis of adolescent eating behaviour during school lunches [36]. Similar techniques can now be reliably used to further analyse eating behaviour changes due to external food cue manipulations, such as food proximity.

By objectively quantifying human eating behaviour in a naturalistic setting, this study aimed at describing the underlying factors that drive human eating behaviour and can contribute to the development of obesity. The current study took place in a Swedish high school, where data was collected on the ingestive behaviour of students who were free to consume grapes, chocolate and crackers during a 60-minute work task. To achieve this, a previously developed technological tool was deployed (i.e., the Mandometer® [33,34]), collecting continuous weight-loss data from common serving trays, with parallel video recordings, introducing a methodology for the combined analysis of serving sizes and serving occurrences across time on an individual level. Based on previous reports [24,25], we hypothesised that increased proximity to the food would result in increased energy intake over time per individual, expecting less structured temporal distribution of servings over time. The hypothesis was tested by comparing two groups of high school students of similar age and weight, who participated in testing sessions differing only in the spatial placement of the served food items.

Material & methods

Recruitment & subjects

Participants of the experiment were recruited from two first year natural science classes of a high school situated in central Stockholm. The recruitment took place in Feb 2015, as part of a bigger trial organised in the school through the EU project SPLENDID [37], with the same student sample participating in behaviourally monitored lunch sessions earlier during the test days [36]. A total of 41 out of 53 notified students agreed to participate in the experiment, providing written assent forms for themselves and written consent forms from their legal guardians. Randomisation among the two conditions was made between the two participating classes, resulting in 24 students assigned to the distal and 17 students to the proximal conditions (Table 1). Participation was non-discriminative, since every student was allowed to participate irrespective of their background, BMI or sex. Minimum sufficient sample sizes (n = 14) were calculated for a power of 0.80 for independent group comparisons, using expected effect size from a relevant report [25]. The presented protocol was approved by the Stockholm Regional Ethics Board and the presented practises fully follow the guidelines for human research in the Declaration of Helsinki [38].

thumbnail
Table 1. Group characteristics for the distal and proximal condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.t001

Procedure

Experimental preparation.

The study followed a simple between-subject design. The two conditions were: i) distal, where food was situated at least 6 m away from the participants (Fig 1A) and ii) proximal, where food was situated at arm’s length from participants (Fig 1B). The two groups of students, each assigned to a single condition, were tested on separate school days, on two consecutive weeks. Two days prior to the testing the weight and height of the participants were measured by researchers. During the testing days, three hours before the initiation of the experimental sessions, the students participated in behaviourally monitored (but otherwise unhindered) school lunches with access to identical types of food, in a buffet setting, following the common school practises [36].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental setup.

The snack station positioning and the room in which the study was conducted, before receiving participants for the two tested conditions. A: distal and B: proximal.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.g001

The two conditions were tested in the same room, which, for the duration of the trials, was only accessible by the researchers and the participants of the study and were video-recorded by 5 separate digital camcorders (Samsung, Suwon, South Korea) fully covering the room area. The required snack stations were prepared in advance, based on the needs of each condition.

Experimental sessions.

The day of testing, after lunch, the participants followed their regular school schedule until 3:00 pm when they returned to the study area for the presented experiment. Upon arrival, the participants were freely (i.e., based on their personal preference, not in randomised fashion) divided into work groups of 3 or 4 students each and were placed at separate tables. At the beginning of the session (before given access to the food) the students answered an appetite questionnaire, containing questions about their perceived fullness, hunger and desire to eat. Each group was given a work task and students were informed that they could serve themselves food with no restrictions on the number of servings or serving sizes throughout the 60-minute work-sessions. Each student was provided with three small plastic serving cups of 100, 100 and 200 ml capacities for chocolate, crackers and grapes, respectively. The only restriction was that students were required to serve only themselves, in order to avoid the possibility of a single student carrying excess food for their whole workgroup. Food was removed at the end of the sessions and the leftover food items in each participant’s cup was weighed by researchers. The students then filled in the appetite questionnaire again, together with a comfortability question concerning their participation in the experimental session and the session was terminated.

Work task.

The provided work task was identical for both testing sessions and it was not mentally or psychologically challenging, as it didn’t include any type of task performance measures and had no “failure” conditions. Specifically, the students were encouraged to freely interact with a smartphone, aiming at evaluating the functionality and the design of a novel app [39] and allowed the students freedom to socialise (inside their workgroup) and consume food throughout the session. The participants were also allowed to drink water ad libitum throughout the trials. While all the workgroups managed to finish the task before the end of the allocated 60 min period, the students were required to remain at their assigned positions until the end of the lecture period.

Materials

Snack stations.

Each station consisted of a kitchen-trolley equipped with three Mandometer® scales (Fig 1A and 1B). Grapes, chocolate and crackers were served in separate, transparent plastic bowls. Each bowl was placed on a Mandometer® scale, allowing separate weight measurements of each food type throughout the session. In all cases, the station bowls were fully refilled with food by researchers, when roughly one third of the bowl content was consumed in order to create a visual sensation of ad libitum food availability across the sessions.

In the distal condition, three snack stations were placed in a visible spot next to the main entrance of the classroom, at a distance of at least 6 meters from each workgroup desk (Fig 1A). In the proximal condition (Fig 1B) one snack station was placed adjacent to each workgroup desk (5 in total), within arm’s length of the participants.

Served food items.

Three types of food items with differing macronutrient composition were provided (Table 2): i) green grapes (seedless, served without stalks), ii) chocolate lentils and iii) rice crackers. The nutritional value of chocolate and crackers were derived from the manufacturer’s labelling, while the nutritional value of grapes was derived from the database provided by Livsmedelsverket, the national food agency of Sweden (The National Food Agency food database, version 2016-02-17) [40].

thumbnail
Table 2. Macronutrient composition of the three provided food types.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.t002

The Mandometer®.

The weight and the timing of each serving (i.e., each time a participant retrieved food from one of the food bowls) was recorded using the Mandometer® v4 (Mikrodidakt, Lund, Sweden), a portable weighing scale linked to a small computer. The device records weight reduction over time at a sampling rate of 1Hz, providing raw weight data series in XML format.

Questionnaires.

An appetite questionnaire was presented before and after the test session in order to estimate potential food intake after lunch and to evaluate appetitive differences between the groups. The intake between lunch and the experiment was answered in free text, while the remaining questions prompted participants to rate their hunger, fullness and desire to eat between 0-100mm on a Visual Analogue Scales (VAS), anchored by five-word descriptors; “Not at all”, “Little”, “Fairly”, “Very” and “Extremely” at 0, 25, 50, 75 and 100mm, respectively (see S1 File).

A comfortability question was used to evaluate perceived burden of the study, the participants rated their comfort on a 9-point Likert scale, with answers from 1: “not comfortable at all” to 9: “Very comfortable” (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Likert scale.

Comfortability question presented to participants on a Likert scale, after the experiment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.g002

The appetite and the comfortability questions were specifically constructed for the present study, based on the general principles of VAS [41] and Likert [42] scales. The appetite questionnaire was deployed for screening purposes only (i.e., to contextualise potential outlier values in the objectively recorded data) and was not one of the main outcome variables of the study. Similarly, the comfortability question was used to evaluate the acceptance of the research methodology by the target group of students. While not formally standardised, similar questions have been used in past research, both in controlled and naturalistic settings, in similarly-aged or older populations ([36,43]).

Data handling

Data processing.

The videos and Mandometer® data were manually transferred to a PC after the sessions. The videos were annotated, using The Observer® XT event log software (Noldus, Wageningen, the Netherlands). The time-stamp events were; experiment start, experiment stop, grape serving, chocolate serving, cracker serving, social snack (if a participant ate from another person's food) and other (used for unexpected behaviours, followed by a comment on the behaviour). During the experiment, no social snack or other event occurred. The Mandometer® weight-loss data series were manually corrected, in order to remove recording artefacts (e.g., pressure put on the plate when handling the food, similar to [44]). Afterwards, the serving events from the video and weight-loss data from the Mandometer® were matched and cross-validated, coupling each individual serving to a participant through the video. Finally, each participant’s weighed leftovers were subtracted from the quantified weight of the final respective serving.

The output of this analysis was a detailed, temporal description of the servings (weight and type of food) for each individual across the session. Note that this method enabled us to identify and measure the precise weight of each of the different food types at every visit to the snack station. Thus, the serving of each component is treated as a separate serving event, even if it occurred during the same visit to the snack station. If, for example, during one visit to the snack station, a participant served themselves 30g of food (consisting of 10g/10g/10g of grapes/chocolate/crackers, respectively), we report 3 different serving events of 10g each.

Statistical analysis.

The presented statistical analyses and figures were done using Sigmaplot v13 (Systat Software Inc., San Jose, California) and R 3.2.3 [45]. The group characteristics and the questionnaire responses for the participating classes were compared with independent T-tests. Independent 2-way ANOVAs (post-hoc testing: Tukey’s) were used to compare: i) the energy intake of each food for individual participants across the testing conditions, ii) the average individual energy content per serving, across food types and testing conditions and iii) the number of servings for each individual across food types and testing conditions. Finally, the temporal distribution of servings across the testing conditions was tested with Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. The significance threshold of all statistical tests was set at 0.05 and all the presented values are mean (SD).

Results

The characteristics of the students participating in the study were not significantly different between the two testing conditions (Table 1; p = 0.160 and p = 0.287 for age and BMI respectively).

Cumulative energy intake per participant

Table 3 gives an overview of the mean energy intake of each food type per participant and the total energy intake per participant, in each condition (absolute food weights are presented in S1 Table).

thumbnail
Table 3. Energy intake per participant in the two experimental conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.t003

The comparison of the cumulative energy intake per participant, throughout the 60-min session, revealed a significant effect (p = 0.029) of the tested conditions (Table 3), with participants in the proximal condition ingesting, on average, 222.7 additional kcal (Fig 3). The post-hoc analysis (Table 3) between the two conditions for each of the consumed foods revealed no significant differences in the cumulative energy intake per individual due to grapes or crackers, (p = 0.913 and p = 0.246, respectively), but a significantly higher cumulative energy intake due to chocolate consumption in the proximal condition (p = 0.012).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Mean energy intake per participant, per food type.

Mean energy intake of grapes, chocolate and crackers per participant, after 60 minutes, in the distal and proximal condition, respectively. * p < 0.05 (post-hoc testing, 2-way ANOVA).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.g003

Serving characteristics per participant

Table 4 gives an overview of the differences across the two conditions in: i) the mean number of servings per food type, per individual, ii) the mean energy content of each serving, per food type, per individual (absolute food weights are presented in S2 Table).

thumbnail
Table 4. Serving event characteristics per participant in the two experimental conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.t004

Overall, there was a significant effect of both condition (p < 0.001) and food type (p < 0.001) on the number of servings per individual (p < 0.001, condition effect). Post-hoc testing revealed that participants served themselves grapes and crackers more often in the proximal condition (p = 0.006 and p = 0.011, respectively), while there was no significant difference in individual chocolate serving numbers (p = 0.150). An interesting observation is that, while in the distal condition 38% (8 of 9 being women) of the participating students did not eat any chocolate, only 12% (2 of 2 being women) didn’t eat any chocolate in the proximal condition. The above observation was not statistically tested due to insufficient sample sizes.

Furthermore, the participants served themselves more chocolate per serving in the proximal condition (p = 0.037), but there was no significant difference of energy content per serving of grapes and crackers (p = 0.430 and p = 0.656, respectively), as presented in Fig 4.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Total energy content (kcal) per serving per individual in the two tested conditions, for each food item.

* p < 0.05 (post-hoc testing, 2-way ANOVA).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.g004

The presented values do not include the leftovers in the serving cups, which were 2.5 kcal, 33.2 kcal and 0.0 kcal in the distal condition and 1.1 kcal, 33.8 kcal and 2.2 kcal in the proximal condition for the grapes, chocolate and crackers, respectively (see leftover column in S1 Dataset).

In summary, in comparison to the distal condition, the participants in the proximal condition ingested foods of higher energy across the 60-minute sessions, when they served themselves grapes and crackers more often, while consuming larger amounts of chocolate per serving.

Temporal analysis of servings

In both the distal and proximal conditions there was an initial burst of energy intake (Fig 5) with 53.1% and 45.6% of the total energy intake occurring in the first 5 minutes of each session, when 46.2% and 26.8% of the total number of servings occurred, respectively. The subsequent serving sizes across the session were reduced in both conditions, which also (Fig 5) differ in regard to the temporal grouping of the serving occurrences. Thus, after the first 5 min, in the distal condition servings appear grouped around specific time points, while in the proximal condition servings are spread across the session more evenly. A Kolmogorov–Smirnov test on the distribution of time differentials between subsequent servings (Δt = tx—tx-1, with tx: the time of the current serving and tx-1: the timing of the previous one), indicated that neither of the two conditions followed a normal distribution (p < 0.001), and also that there was a significant difference between conditions (p = 0.024). These results agree with the researchers’ observations at the time of the testing sessions, who perceived “continuous” servings during the proximal condition and “episodic” serving in the distal one.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Temporal distribution of serving events.

The time point and energy load for each serving event of grapes, chocolate and crackers, in each condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.g005

Subjective group measures

At the beginning of the experimental sessions, only two participants (one in each condition) reported any food intake after lunch. Their exclusion from the analysis did not modify the reported results, thus they are included in the presented analysis. Additionally, the two participating groups rated their perceived hunger, fullness and desire to eat similarly (p = 0.64, p = 0.82 and p = 0.55, respectively). Interestingly (Table 5), regardless of the energy intake differences, the two groups did not perceive any significant differences in any of these feelings at the end of the sessions either (p = 0.46, p = 0.97 and p = 0.69 for hunger, fullness and desire to eat, respectively).

thumbnail
Table 5. Subjective appetite measures in the two conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.t005

At the end of the sessions, the participants in the two groups reported that they felt comfortable participating in the test sessions, with only one participant, across both groups, rating his/her comfort 5 out of 9 (on a Likert-scale), while 95% rated their comfort as 7 or above.

Discussion

In the present study, we successfully tested the hypothesis that increased food proximity during a work task would lead to increased energy intake, through a differentiated pattern of servings across the testing sessions. In order to achieve that, we deployed a novel methodology in naturalistic conditions, based on the parallel analysis of weight-loss data from Mandometer® and video extracted behavioural information, by adapting our own methodologies, previously used for single meal analysis in controlled [35] and naturalistic environments [36]. The distal and proximal conditions were identical, apart from the spatial placement of the available food items, and were tested in two independent samples of high school students of comparable BMI and age characteristics. The two independent student groups were also similar in regard to the quantity and the type of food which they consumed during the earlier lunch [36], while the majority of them (39 out of the 41 participants) did not report any food intake between lunch and the experimental sessions. Finally, the two groups had similar group ratings of perceived hunger, fullness and desire to eat at the beginning of the experimental sessions (Table 5) and the participating students felt comfortable taking part in the experiment, pointing towards high sample homogeneity in the performed group comparisons.

Considering the main outcomes of this study, the students participating in the proximal condition, ingested more energy over a period of 60 minutes (726kcal per individual) comparing to the students that participated in the distal condition (504kcal per individual). This was mainly caused by a significant increase of chocolate consumption in the proximal condition (397kcal vs 254kcal in the distal condition), while the cumulative energy intake for grapes and crackers was similar across the conditions (Table 3 and Fig 3). Interestingly, the significantly increased energy intake in the proximal condition did not result in significantly different subjective scores of fullness, hunger and desire to eat at the end of the session. Overall, these results support the notion that external food cues [1520], and especially food availability [21,22] and food proximity have an important effect on the observed eating behaviour and the cumulative energy intake, at least short-term [24,25], potentially affecting energy balance throughout the day and, if continued over time, facilitate the development of obesity [5].

Looking closer into the characteristics of the average servings per individual across the testing sessions, we found that food proximity caused increased frequency of servings for both grapes and crackers (Table 4), which, however, was not enough to result in significant cumulative changes in energy intake for these food types. Also, the students with the food placed close to them during the scheduled work task ate more chocolate per serving (260kcal vs 187kcal per serving for the proximal vs the distal condition; Fig 4), resulting in the observed cumulative energy intake increase. In both conditions, almost half of the total energy intake was ingested in the first 5 minutes of the experimental session. However, there was a distinct difference in the temporal distribution of the subsequent servings between the conditions (Fig 5). Participants served themselves in regular intervals, or “bursts”, when the food was placed further away (i.e., distal), while they served themselves in an unstructured, “continuous” fashion when the foods were readily available (i.e., proximal). The “burst” serving behaviour in the distal condition was also noticed by the researchers on-site, with many participants visiting the snack stations around the same time, rather than by themselves.

The findings of the present study are mostly in line with previously reported results. For example, in a study conducted in an office setting, the difference of food unit consumption was quantified across a whole work day, when bowls of chocolate were placed either on the work desk (proximal), or 2m away (distal), resulting in 37.5% intake increase in the proximal condition [24]. However, the above study does not offer any objective information for the temporal distribution of the servings or individual serving characteristics, but expands upon the perceived “ease of access” of the proximal food.

Our findings also partially agree with previous reports examining the effects of food proximity on the consumption of foods of differing energy density [25,46]. In a previous study([46]) participants ingested significantly more from both types of food items (i.e., popcorns and apples) when they were placed in close proximity, albeit for a limited time period (6 min), while in the study by Musher-Eizenman [25], again, both the high- and low- energy food items were consumed more when proximal, but the duration of food availability is not reported. This difference in the duration of the availability of the food might be responsible for the discrepancy in our results, since in the presented study, the total energy intake does not differ for the two foods of lower energy density (grapes and crackers). Overall, our study supports the notion that unstructured eating, in this case facilitated by continuous food availability due to increased proximity, can lead to increased energy intake over time [28]. Past studies, however, are not directly comparable to the presented work, as they are based on subjective measures of food intake [29] and they have mainly focused on irregular daily meal occurrence [26], rather than controlled efforts to describe the temporal pattern of eating.

As stated before [30], one should be careful regarding the limitations of short experimental protocols (e.g., 6 min of food availability in [46]), avoiding generalisation of the results for longer time periods. This also points toward the importance of continuous data collection for understanding food selection and ingestion in certain settings, with our study being the first, to our knowledge, providing detailed reports on these measures. On the other hand, that does not reduce the usefulness of time-restricted study protocols, which might be better suited for modelling eating behaviour in time-restrained environments (e.g., a school meal/class break under strict school schedule [47]).

The lack of additional perceived fullness after the increased energy intake in the proximal condition in our study, supports our previously stated opinion [48] about the disconnection between the objectively quantified behaviour and perceived subjective measures, at least in conditions when “mindless” [49] or “automatic” [50] eating is facilitated. These findings should further discourage the use of subjective measures (e.g, satiety, hunger and fullness) as sole predictors or descriptors of energy intake in research, in agreement with past suggestions [43,51,52].

The greatest strength of the present study is that, in contrast to similar studies examining the effects of various external factors on eating [15,17,18,20,24,25], the deployed methods allowed for detailed continuous recording of servings per participant over time, which enabled us to compare the time distribution of servings and energy intake between the two conditions. The identical nature of every aspect of the protocol and the sample homogeneity across the two conditions are also important, allowing for proper comparisons between the independent groups.

A factor which can potentially be regarded as a weakness of our protocol is the specific setting of the study. In the everyday school practice students do not usually have access to food items during formal work tasks, making the setting of the study mostly relevant to non-supervised school work, usually taking place later in the school day, at least for the selected target group. The sample size in this study is also relatively small, and while the power of the presented comparisons was adequate, we could not test some potentially interesting differences between conditions, such as the observed tendency of females to avoid chocolate altogether in the distal but not in the proximal condition.

Thus, future controlled and semi-controlled research should aim to quantify in detail additional parameters which might interact with the presented effects, focusing on sex (male vs female), population (e.g., obese vs normal weight), or setting (e.g., office-scape, family environment, etc.) differences. On the other hand, in naturalistic settings, the logical next step is the deployment of comparable measuring techniques over extended periods of time, in order to gather further quantitative information on the distribution of eating occurrences, together with more exact data on the energy loads of individual food items. Such an achievement would close the gaps of knowledge concerning the contribution of different eating occurrences on obesity development [10,11], remedying the identified inconsistencies in self-reported energy intake [12]. To that effect, new technological advancements are required (e.g., note the effort in current research projects like SPLENDID [53] and BigO [54]) and one can envision a comparable system, where the video recordings are replaced by automatic food item registration (e.g., QR code scanning [55] etc.). Additional advancements in data analysis (e.g., [56] or [57]) can facilitate automatic extraction of the recorded information, facilitating a “hands-off” (but not less detailed) approach to the analysis of the eating behaviour in different populations. In the future that can lead towards the deployment of more meaningful intervention and prevention schemes, carefully controlling the timing and the proximity of food servings, in order to limit unstructured occurrences of eating.

Conclusions

In the present study, food proximity, especially of chocolate, appears to be responsible for increased cumulative energy intake per participant when two independent groups of high school students were tested in identical naturalistic conditions. Additionally, the proximity of the food seems to affect the pattern of servings across time, causing increased frequency of servings for grapes and crackers and, in general, leading to “continuous” rather than “burst” serving patterns. Overall, these findings provide a more comprehensive description of the reported effects of food proximity on energy intake, compared to past studies [24,25]. They also indicate the importance of controlling the accessibility of food in studies examining other external factors, such as social interactions (e.g., [20,58]), in respect to their effect on food intake. In the future, detailed temporal analysis might prove a powerful tool to analyse eating behaviour in naturalistic environments with continuous access to food for longer periods of time (e.g., an office environment [24], a kitchen environment [14], etc.), allowing the collection of more detailed information about the environmental effects on eating behaviour.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. Subject measures.

Data on sex, age, BMI, questionnaire answers and food item leftovers, for each participant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.s001

(XLSX)

S2 Dataset. Event log of experiment.

Data on the size and time of each serving, for each participant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.s002

(XLSX)

S1 File. Before and after meal questionnaire.

The before and after meal questions used in the study as presented to the participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.s003

(PDF)

S1 Table. Food weight ingested per participant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182172.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Anna Anvret and Modjtaba Zandian for their help during data collection. We would also like to thank the students and teachers, who made this study possible.

References

  1. 1. WHO | Obesity and overweight [Internet]. WHO. [cited 2016 Oct 27]. Available from: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs311/en/
  2. 2. Kopelman PG. Obesity as a medical problem. Nature. 2000 Apr 6;404(6778):635–43. pmid:10766250
  3. 3. Sturm R. The effects of obesity, smoking, and drinking on medical problems and costs. Health Aff Proj Hope. 2002 Apr;21(2):245–53.
  4. 4. Bray GA, Kim KK, Wilding JPH. Obesity: a chronic relapsing progressive disease process. A position statement of the World Obesity Federation. Obes Rev Off J Int Assoc Study Obes. 2017 May 10;
  5. 5. Hall KD, Sacks G, Chandramohan D, Chow CC, Wang YC, Gortmaker SL, et al. Quantification of the effect of energy imbalance on bodyweight. Lancet Lond Engl. 2011 Aug 27;378(9793):826–37.
  6. 6. Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT, Moodie ML, et al. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet. 2011 Aug 27;378(9793):804–14. pmid:21872749
  7. 7. de Graaf C, Kok FJ. Slow food, fast food and the control of food intake. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2010 May;6(5):290–3. pmid:20351697
  8. 8. Duffey KJ, Popkin BM. Energy density, portion size, and eating occasions: contributions to increased energy intake in the United States, 1977–2006. PLoS Med. 2011 Jun;8(6):e1001050. pmid:21738451
  9. 9. Popkin BM, Duffey KJ. Does hunger and satiety drive eating anymore? Increasing eating occasions and decreasing time between eating occasions in the United States. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010 May;91(5):1342–7. pmid:20237134
  10. 10. Watowicz RP, Anderson SE, Kaye GL, Taylor CA. Energy Contribution of Beverages in US Children by Age, Weight, and Consumer Status. Child Obes Print. 2015 Aug;11(4):475–83.
  11. 11. Just DR, Wansink B. Fast food, soft drink and candy intake is unrelated to body mass index for 95% of American adults. Obes Sci Pract. 2015 Dec;1(2):126–30. pmid:27774255
  12. 12. Lichtman SW, Pisarska K, Berman ER, Pestone M, Dowling H, Offenbacher E, et al. Discrepancy between Self-Reported and Actual Caloric Intake and Exercise in Obese Subjects. N Engl J Med. 1992 Dec 31;327(27):1893–8. pmid:1454084
  13. 13. Hutchison AT, Heilbronn LK. Metabolic impacts of altering meal frequency and timing—Does when we eat matter? Biochimie. 2016 May;124:187–97. pmid:26226640
  14. 14. Wansink B, Hanks AS, Kaipainen K. Slim by Design: Kitchen Counter Correlates of Obesity. Health Educ Behav Off Publ Soc Public Health Educ. 2016 Oct;43(5):552–8.
  15. 15. Wansink B, Painter JE, North J. Bottomless bowls: why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obes Res. 2005 Jan;13(1):93–100. pmid:15761167
  16. 16. Wansink B, Kim J. Bad popcorn in big buckets: portion size can influence intake as much as taste. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005 Oct;37(5):242–5. pmid:16053812
  17. 17. Wansink B, van Ittersum K. Portion size me: plate-size induced consumption norms and win-win solutions for reducing food intake and waste. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2013 Dec;19(4):320–32. pmid:24341317
  18. 18. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Kral TVE, Meengs JS, Wall DE. Increasing the portion size of a packaged snack increases energy intake in men and women. Appetite. 2004 Feb;42(1):63–9. pmid:15036784
  19. 19. Péneau S, Mekhmoukh A, Chapelot D, Dalix A-M, Airinei G, Hercberg S, et al. Influence of environmental factors on food intake and choice of beverage during meals in teenagers: a laboratory study. Br J Nutr. 2009 Dec;102(12):1854–9. pmid:19682398
  20. 20. Clendenen VI, Herman CP, Polivy J. Social facilitation of eating among friends and strangers. Appetite. 1994 Aug;23(1):1–13. pmid:7826053
  21. 21. Fay SH, White MJ, Finlayson G, King NA. Psychological predictors of opportunistic snacking in the absence of hunger. Eat Behav. 2015 Aug;18:156–9. pmid:26102429
  22. 22. Fisher JO, Birch LL, Zhang J, Grusak MA, Hughes SO. External influences on children’s self-served portions at meals. Int J Obes 2005. 2013 Jul;37(7):954–60.
  23. 23. Roe LS, Kling SMR, Rolls BJ. What is eaten when all of the foods at a meal are served in large portions? Appetite. 2016 Apr 1;99:1–9. pmid:26767612
  24. 24. Wansink B, Painter JE, Lee Y-K. The office candy dish: proximity’s influence on estimated and actual consumption. Int J Obes 2005. 2006 May;30(5):871–5.
  25. 25. Musher-Eizenman DR, Young KM, Laurene K, Galliger C, Hauser J, Wagner Oehlhof M. Children’s sensitivity to external food cues: how distance to serving bowl influences children’s consumption. Health Educ Behav Off Publ Soc Public Health Educ. 2010 Apr;37(2):186–92.
  26. 26. Garaulet M, Gómez-Abellán P. Timing of food intake and obesity: a novel association. Physiol Behav. 2014 Jul;134:44–50. pmid:24467926
  27. 27. Berg C, Forslund HB. The Influence of Portion Size and Timing of Meals on Weight Balance and Obesity. Curr Obes Rep. 2015 Mar;4(1):11–8. pmid:26627086
  28. 28. Bellisle F. Meals and snacking, diet quality and energy balance. Physiol Behav. 2014 Jul;134:38–43. pmid:24657181
  29. 29. Leech RM, Worsley A, Timperio A, McNaughton SA. Understanding meal patterns: definitions, methodology and impact on nutrient intake and diet quality. Nutr Res Rev. 2015 Jun;28(1):1–21. pmid:25790334
  30. 30. Hunter JA, Hollands GJ, Couturier D-L, Marteau TM. Impact of altering proximity on snack food intake in individuals with high and low executive function: study protocol. BMC Public Health. 2016 Jun 13;16:504. pmid:27297225
  31. 31. Kissileff HR, Guss JL. Microstructure of eating behavior in humans. Appetite. 2001 Feb;36(1):70–8. pmid:11270360
  32. 32. Ioakimidis I, Zandian M, Bergh C, Södersten P. A method for the control of eating rate: A potential intervention in eating disorders. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(3):755–60. pmid:19587188
  33. 33. Bergh C, Callmar M, Danemar S, Hölcke M, Isberg S, Leon M, et al. Effective treatment of eating disorders: Results at multiple sites. Behav Neurosci. 2013 Dec;127(6):878–89. pmid:24341712
  34. 34. Ford AL, Bergh C, Södersten P, Sabin MA, Hollinghurst S, Hunt LP, et al. Treatment of childhood obesity by retraining eating behaviour: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2010 Jan 6;340:b5388.
  35. 35. Ioakimidis I, Zandian M, Ulbl F, íLund C, Bergh C, SöDersten P. Food Intake and Chewing in Women. Neurocomput. 2012 May;84:31–38.
  36. 36. Langlet B, Anvret A, Maramis C, Moulos I, Papapanagiotou V, Diou C, et al. Objective measures of eating behaviour in a Swedish high school. Behav Inf Technol. 2017 May 6;0(0):1–9.
  37. 37. Maramis C, Diou C, Ioakeimidis I, Lekka I, Dudnik G, Mars M, et al. Preventing obesity and eating disorders through behavioural modifications: the SPLENDID vision. Appetite. 2016 Jun 1;101:234.
  38. 38. WMA Declaration of Helsinki—Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects [Internet]. 2013 [cited 2017 Jan 18]. Available from: http://www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/b3/
  39. 39. Moulos I, Maramis C, Ioakimidis I, Boer J van den, Nolstam J, Mars M, et al. Objective and Subjective Meal Registration via a Smartphone Application. In: Murino V, Puppo E, Sona D, Cristani M, Sansone C, editors. New Trends in Image Analysis and Processing—ICIAP 2015 Workshops [Internet]. Springer International Publishing; 2015 [cited 2017 Jan 16]. p. 409–16. (Lecture Notes in Computer Science). Available from: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-319-23222-5_50
  40. 40. Search for nutrients—Livsmedelsverket [Internet]. [cited 2017 Jan 18]. Available from: http://www7.slv.se/SokNaringsinnehall/Home/ToggleLanguage
  41. 41. Aitken RCB. A Growing Edge of Measurement of Feelings: Measurement of Feelings Using Visual Analogue Scales. Proc R Soc Med. 1969 Oct 1;62(10):989–93. pmid:4899510
  42. 42. Likert R. A Technique for the Measurement of Attitudes. Arch Psychol. 1932;22(140):55.
  43. 43. Zandian M, Ioakimidis I, Bergh C, Brodin U, Södersten P. Decelerated and linear eaters: effect of eating rate on food intake and satiety. Physiol Behav. 2009 Feb 16;96(2):270–5. pmid:18992760
  44. 44. Ioakimidis I, Zandian M, Eriksson-Marklund L, Bergh C, Grigoriadis A, Södersten P. Description of chewing and food intake over the course of a meal. Physiol Behav. 2011 Oct 24;104(5):761–9. pmid:21807012
  45. 45. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012. Available from: http://www.R-project.org/
  46. 46. Privitera GJ, Zuraikat FM. Proximity of foods in a competitive food environment influences consumption of a low calorie and a high calorie food. Appetite. 2014 May;76:175–9. pmid:24560689
  47. 47. Van Hook J, Altman CE. Competitive Food Sales in Schools and Childhood Obesity: A Longitudinal Study. Sociol Educ. 2012 Jan 1;85(1):23–39. pmid:22611291
  48. 48. Ioakimidis I, Zandian M, Ulbl F, Bergh C, Leon M, Södersten P. How eating affects mood. Physiol Behav. 2011 Jun 1;103(3–4):290–4. pmid:21315752
  49. 49. Wansink B. From mindless eating to mindlessly eating better. Physiol Behav. 2010 Jul 14;100(5):454–63. pmid:20470810
  50. 50. Cohen D, Farley TA. Eating as an automatic behavior. Prev Chronic Dis. 2008 Jan;5(1):A23. pmid:18082012
  51. 51. Ferriday D, Bosworth ML, Lai S, Godinot N, Martin N, Martin AA, et al. Effects of eating rate on satiety: A role for episodic memory? Physiol Behav. 2015 Dec 1;152(Pt B):389–96. pmid:26143189
  52. 52. Rolls BJ, Roe LS. Effect of the volume of liquid food infused intragastrically on satiety in women. Physiol Behav. 2002 Aug;76(4–5):623–31. pmid:12127002
  53. 53. European Commission: CORDIS: Projects & Results Service: Personalised Guide for Eating and Activity Behaviour for the Prevention of Obesity and Eating Disorders [Internet]. [cited 2017 Jan 23]. Available from: http://cordis.europa.eu/project/rcn/111354_en.html
  54. 54. European Commission: CORDIS: Projects & Results Service: Big data against childhood Obesity [Internet]. [cited 2017 Jan 23]. Available from: http://cordis.europa.eu/project/rcn/206067_en.html
  55. 55. Sanz-Valero J, Álvarez Sabucedo LM, Wanden-Berghe C, Santos Gago JM. QR Codes: Outlook for Food Science and Nutrition. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2016;56(6):973–8. pmid:26047907
  56. 56. Papapanagiotou V, Diou C, Langlet B, Ioakimidis I, Delopoulos A. A parametric Probabilistic Context-Free Grammar for food intake analysis based on continuous meal weight measurements. In: 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC). 2015. p. 7853–6.
  57. 57. Dehais J, Anthimopoulos M, Mougiakakou S. Food Image Segmentation for Dietary Assessment. In: Proceedings of the 2Nd International Workshop on Multimedia Assisted Dietary Management [Internet]. New York, NY, USA: ACM; 2016 [cited 2017 Jan 23]. p. 23–28. (MADiMa ‘16). Available from: http://doi.acm.org/10.1145/2986035.2986047
  58. 58. Cruwys T, Bevelander KE, Hermans RCJ. Social modeling of eating: a review of when and why social influence affects food intake and choice. Appetite. 2015 Mar;86:3–18. pmid:25174571