Skip to main content

Magnetic orientation of the Common Toad: establishing an arena approach for adult anurans

Abstract

Background

Magnetic orientation is a taxonomically widespread phenomenon in the animal kingdom, but has been little studied in anuran amphibians. We collected Common Toads (Bufo bufo) during their migration towards their spawning pond and tested them shortly after displacement for possible magnetic orientation in arena experiments. Animals were tested in two different set-ups, in the geomagnetic field and in a reversed magnetic field. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study testing orientation of adult anurans with a controlled magnetic field of a known strength and alignment.

Results

After displacement, toads oriented themselves unimodally under the geomagnetic field, following their former migration direction (d-axis). When the magnetic field was reversed, the distribution of bearings changed from a unimodal to a bimodal pattern, but still along the d-axis. The clustering of bearings was only significant after the toads reached the outer circle, 60.5 cm from their starting point. At a virtual inner circle (diameter 39 cm) and at the start of the experiment, orientation of toads did not show any significant pattern.

Conclusions

The experimental set-up used in our study is suitable to test orientation behaviour of the Common Toad. We speculate that toads had not enough time to relocate their position on an internal map. Hence, they followed their former migration direction. Bimodality in orientation when exposed to the reversed magnetic field could be the result of a cue conflict, between magnetic and possibly celestial cues. For maintaining their migration direction toads use, at least partly, the geomagnetic field as a reference system.

Background

Periodic migrations are an integral part of the life history of many animals. Northern populations of Robins (Erithacus rubecula) migrate to the south [1], Equatorial Sandhoppers (Talorchestia martensii) migrate along a Y-axis perpendicular to the shoreline [2] and Alpine Newts (Triturus alpestris) migrate to their spawning pond [3]. All these and several other animal species are able to use magnetic cues for orientation [4, 5]. Even though the phenomenon of magnetoreception in animals is well known, some fundamental issues, ranging from the underlying biophysical processes to the ability of navigation, are still poorly understood [68].

The most detailed studies in amphibian orientation biology were conducted with the Eastern Red Spotted Newt (Notophthalmus viridescens). These newts are able to orient themselves in the homeward direction after displacements of up to 45 km [9]. Magnetic orientation of the Eastern Red Spotted Newt is light-dependent [10, 11] and experiments suggest that the inclination of the geomagnetic field is used in a magnetic map [9, 12].

For anurans, only very limited information on magnetic orientation is available [13]. Tadpoles of the Bullfrog (Lithobates catesbeianus) and the Iberian Green Frog (Pelophylax perezi) could be trained to swim along a Y-axis, using the magnetic field as a cue [14, 15]. In both species magnetic orientation by tadpoles is also light-dependent [16, 17].

Adults of our target species, the European Common Toad (Bufo bufo), are able to locate their home pond even if it is filled up with soil [18]. Sinsch [19] provided the first evidence for a magnetic sense: he showed that toads were not able to initially orient towards their spawning pond after displacements over a few hundred meters when they had a bar magnet attached to their heads.

The use of arenas is a standard approach in orientation studies: Animals are placed in the centre of a circular test area and the point where they reach the perimeter is usually taken as measure of their orientation tendencies. Once orientation has been established, the magnetic field and other orientation cues can be manipulated and any changes in behaviour recorded [16, 20, 21]. This approach has been successfully applied to urodeles and larval anurans [10, 16, 2224]. In experiments with amphibians, arena diameter usually ranged from 43 cm [25] to 80 cm [26]. Generally, arena experiments are more difficult to perform with adult anurans, because many species known for their impressive breeding migrations are large bodied, move faster than newts and salamanders and hence require larger arenas. Moreover, they tend to show escape behaviour that interferes with the realization of orientation trials.

We tested Common Toads in an arena situated outside of their home range, in order to investigate whether they would orient towards their breeding pond, and how their behaviour would be influenced by a change in direction of the magnetic field. To the best of our knowledge this study is the first experiment examining orientation of adult anurans with a controlled alignment and strength of the magnetic field.

Methods

Testing site

Experiments were carried out after sunset between 20:30 and 00:30 hours, between 28 March and 2 April 2010 in stable weather conditions (without rain or strong wind), at the car park of the Federal Research and Training Centre for Forests, Natural Hazards and Landscape (48°12'27" N, 16°13'45" E; 225 m a. s. l.), situated in Vienna, Austria.

Experimental animals

Only males of the Common Toad (Bufo bufo) were used for the experiments. This species is well known for its explosive breeding behaviour [27]. Many toads migrate after sunset during a period of approximately two weeks in spring. If streets intersect the migration route, traffic can cause high mortality. Therefore, barriers such as drift fences and amphibian tunnels are frequently built to reduce the death toll at roads. At our study site, a permanent drift fence constructed of wooden planks is positioned so as to direct the toads towards two tunnels along a mean direction of 32.5° (ranging from 5° to 60°), when north is set to zero (Figure 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Testing site. Location of the pond in Vienna with the surrounding park and the direction towards the testing site. Toads are diverted by the drift fence until they can cross the street through the tunnels.

The pond is located approximately 200 m from the fence and at a distance of about 2.5 km from the testing site. The general migration direction from the hibernation area to the pond was observed in the years 2009 and 2010 and estimated at approximately 318° (with a possible range of 285° to 351°), while the direction from the testing site to the pond was 85°.

We collected experimental animals at the drift fence during their migration to the spawning site (48°12'32" N, 16°15'47" E; 300 m a. s. l.) (Figure 1). During transport in a car to the testing site the toads were kept in closed buckets (filled with water to a level of 2 cm) to prevent access to chemical or visual cues. Animals were tested at the latest 3:49 hours after they arrived at the testing site.

The arena

The toads were tested one by one in a circular arena (diameter 121 cm) consisting of a plastic wall (60 cm high) and a wooden floor. Wall and bottom were covered with black opaque plastic sheets. The toads could see the sky, whereas the horizon was not visible. Directions were painted on the wall of the arena with a white pen at five degrees intervals.

Electromagnetic coils

Helmholtz-coils (21 windings per coil; approximately 50 A in the coils) were used to alter the alignment of the geomagnetic field [28] (Figure 2). When the coils were turned on the direction of magnetic north was rotated to 180°. The vertical field was not modified. Field strength was measured with a 3 D fluxgate magnetic field sensor (FLC3-70; Stefan Mayer Instruments). Three lead-acid batteries functioned as power supply; a current limiter and a resistor were used to hold the current constant at the required level.

Figure 2
figure 2

Experimental set-up. Experimental set-up with Helmholtz-coils. At the start of each trial the toad was placed in the centre of the arena and covered with a pot (release device).

Testing procedure

At the beginning of each trial, a toad was placed in the centre of the arena and covered with the release device, a cylindrical opaque clay pot (diameter 20 cm). The toad was held under the release device to allow it to recover from handling. After 4 min the release device was lifted with rope and pulley, thereby releasing the toad into the arena. The behaviour of the toad (jumping and the directional choice at the wall) was recorded with observation positions being alternated after each trial to avoid bias. The point where a toad reached the wall for the first time was defined as the directional choice at this level. If a toad failed to reach the wall of the arena within 7 min, or if it was jumping rather than walking, it was excluded from further analysis. After every two trials alignment of the horizontal field was changed by 180°. Each toad was subjected to only one trial. Between trials the arena surface was wiped with paper towels to eliminate possible olfactory cues.

When the release device was lifted trials were recorded using an infrared camera (iSlim 321R; Genius). With these recordings we analysed the heading (alignment of the snout-vent axis) at the start of the trial (initial orientation) and the directional choice where the toad crossed a virtual inner circle (diameter 39 cm), which was electronically overlaid. The point at which the toad contacted the wall of the arena was recorded by the observer.

Supplementary data

Data collected for each trial also included weather conditions (air temperature measured to the next 0.1°, cloudiness estimated to the next 5% and humidity measured to the next 0.1%), time of day, trial time (time a toad needed to reach the wall after the releasing device was lifted), and the toad's body length (measured to the nearest millimetre with a slide ruler). For the supplementary data standard deviations (SD) were calculated.

Data analysis

Data were analysed using standard circular statistics [29, 30]. Mean direction was calculated by vector addition and rounded to the nearest 5° (accuracy of observation). The Rayleigh-test was used to test for a non-random distribution. To test for bimodal orientation angles were doubled and multiples were reduced modulo 360° prior to analysis [29]. For each mean direction the mean vector length (r) was measured. The 95% confidence interval was determined for each significant mean vector. These analyses were carried out in Microsoft® Excel® 2004 for Mac (Version 11.5.9). The statistical program R were used to plot the data [31].

To test for differences between bearings in the natural and the reversed magnetic field, the absolute deviations of the bearings of both distributions from the mean direction under the natural magnetic field (MDN) were calculated. Distributions of deviations were tested for departure from normality with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov-test. Then, one-sided t-tests were applied to examine whether absolute deviations from the MDN were significantly higher in the reversed field. Both tests were carried out using SPSS 13 for Mac OS X.

Results

Experimental animals

In total 62 toads with a mean body length of 67 mm (SD = 6 mm) were tested in the arena experiment. One toad jumped and three did not reach the wall after 7 min and were therefore excluded from analysis. Thus, in each set-up (natural and reversed magnetic field) 29 toads were tested successfully. Toads needed on average 3 min and 16 s (SD = 1 min and 31 s) to reach the wall after the release device was lifted (mean trial time).

Experimental conditions

The strength of the geomagnetic field at the testing site was 40 μT, with an inclination of 63.4°. The declination for the first July in the testing year in Vienna was about 3°10' [32]. The strength of the reversed field differed by less than 2.9% from the geomagnetic field. The mean temperature was 7.8°C (SD = 3.3°C), the mean cloudiness was 45% (SD = 35%) and the mean humidity was 68.4% (SD = 5.6%). No successful trial was performed under complete cloud cover.

Orientation in the natural magnetic field

For initial orientation, no significant trend was detectable under the natural condition (Figure 3a, Table 1). Also at the inner circle a random distribution was observed (Figure 3b, Table 1). The distribution of bearings where toads contacted the arena wall was unimodal and the mean direction coincided with the mean of the former migration direction along the drift fence (d-axis, [33]) (Figure 3c, Table 1).

Figure 3
figure 3

Results. Bearings and mean directions of the initial orientation (a), orientation at the inner circle (b) and orientation at the wall of the arena (c); in the figure the radius of the circle corresponds to a mean vector length (r) equal to one. Graphs in the upper row show the results obtained under natural conditions, graphs in the lower row (highlighted) those obtained with the reversed magnetic field (n = 29 in all cases). Dashed lines indicate the 95% confidence intervals in cases of significant orientation (P < 0.05). Under natural conditions the magnetic North (mN) coincided with the true north direction (nN), whereas in the reversed field mN was opposite to nN. Coloured arrows indicate the direction to the pond (P), the direction from the hibernation area to the pond (HA) and the mean direction along the drift fence (D). In the case of the reversed field, corresponding arrows are placed on the opposite side, in paler colours, to illustrate the directions when following only the magnetic field.

Table 1 Mean directions of toads in the natural and the reversed magnetic field

Orientation in the reversed magnetic field

With the reversed magnetic field, initial orientations and the distribution at the inner circle were indistinguishable from random (Figure 3a, b, Table 1). In contrast, at the wall, the distribution of bearings was bimodally distributed, with the d-axis included in the 95% confidence interval (Figure 3c, Table 1), and failed to reach significance for a unimodal orientation (unimodal direction: 305°; r = 0.28; Rayleigh-test for unimodal distribution: P = 0.10, n = 29).

Comparison of distributions

In no case distributions of deviations from the MDN differed significantly from normality (Kolmogorov-Smirnov-test: df = 29, P > 0.35). The differences between distributions at the start and at the inner circle were not significant (Table 1). At the wall in the reversed field, absolute deviations from the MDN were significantly higher than in the natural field (Table 1).

Discussion

The arena we used was large enough for the Common Toads to manifest outcomes of orientation. Initial orientation, as well as the directions of movement at a short distance (39 cm) from the release point, were random, but a directional preference was evident when the toads reached the wall of the arena. A larger arena would perhaps allow to obtain clearer results; in displacement experiments with Common Toads in the field, Sinsch [19] included only individuals which had moved at least 5 m in the statistical analyses. With an increase in the size of the arena, however, manipulations of the magnetic field would become increasingly cumbersome.

The bearings at the wall of the arena were widely scattered, with the mean vector coinciding with the former migration direction along the drift fence. We tentatively interpret this result to show that toads oriented in their former migration direction. The variance of bearings may reflect at least partly their different prior migration directions up to or along the drift fence. It may appear implausible that toads should follow the course of the drift fence, rather than orienting in the direction from the hibernation area to the pond. The fence, however, had been installed for many years; thus, toads that migrate not for the first time may have learned the path to the tunnel and integrated it in an internal map.

Maintenance of the former migration direction has been found in some orientation studies with amphibians [33], whereas other experiments demonstrated the ability to orient to the breeding pond [9, 12, 13, 26, 34]. In the Common Toad, both outcomes have been obtained [19, 35]; hence, these inconsistent results probably reflect differences in experimental design. Our approach differs in two ways from arena experiments that documented orientation towards the breeding pond: We collected the toads during their spawning migration, not at the breeding pond, and we allowed only a short recovery time between transport and orientation trials. Our rationale for taking toads during their migration was that we surmised migrating toads to be highly motivated to reach the pond, whereas toads already present at the pond might eventually lose the motivation to return due to exhaustion in their struggle for matings [36]. In preliminary trials at another testing site (900 m southeast from the spawning pond), toads that we had collected on the street exhibited orientation towards the breeding pond; reanalysis of these data showed, however, that this direction did not differ significantly from their likely former migration direction [37].

According to the magnetic map hypothesis [7, 38], animals have to find their position on an internal map before they are able to choose the new direction towards their target. We speculate that, in our experiment, toads did not have sufficient time to assess their position in the interval between capture and testing, which ranged from a few minutes to 4 hours. With Common Toads displaced 3 km by Sinsch [19], it took up to three days before they headed homewards. In arena trials conducted by Buck [35] with Common Toads collected during their spawning migration, animals tested directly after displacement of 350 m maintained their migration direction, whereas a series with toads tested 24 hours after displacement resulted in orientation towards their breeding pond. Time dependence of orientation is also known in other vertebrates: Gray-Cheeked Thrushes (Catharus minimus), Swainson's Thrushes (Catharus ustulatus) and Greater Mouse-Eared Bats (Myotis myotis) need to recalibrate their magnetic compass with twilight cues for successful homeward orientation responses [39, 40].

In the reversed magnetic field, bearings shifted from a unimodal to a bimodal distribution, along the same axis. This finding indicates an effect of the magnetic field on the orientation of the toads. Bimodality may result from a cue-conflict between the magnetic field and another cue. Individual preferences in using a multiple cue system have been reported in the orientation behaviour of anurans [41, 42]. Hence, it is possible that some toads in our experiment followed magnetic cues in the reversed magnetic field, whereas others may have used celestial cues.

Because of its explosive breeding behaviour, high abundance, slow locomotion and high fidelity to its spawning ponds, the Common Toad is an excellent experimental animal in which to study orientation of anurans. The experimental set-up we developed opens possibilities to distinguish between multiple cues and to clarify the time-dependency of orientation systems. In future studies similar experiments with a longer resting time at the testing site should be conducted. Studies of magnetic orientation by juvenile toads, migrating away from their natal pond and by larvae trained to a Y-axis could lead to an integral picture of the life history of the Common Toad.

Conclusion

We show that orientation of adult Common Toads can be investigated with a standard arena approach during their spawning migration. The distribution of directional choices changed when the horizontal component of the magnetic field was reversed. Bimodality in orientation under the altered magnetic field may have been caused by a cue conflict between magnetic and celestial cues.

References

  1. Lack D, Gillmor R: The life of the robin. 1946, London: HF & G. Witherby

    Google Scholar 

  2. Pardi L, Ugolini A, Faqi AS, Scapini F, Ercolini A: Zonal recovery in equatorial sandhoppers: interaction between magnetic and solar orientation. Behavioural adaptation to intertidal life. Edited by: Chelazzi G, Vannini M. 1988, New York: Plenum Press, 151: 79-92. ASI Series

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  3. Diego-Rasilla FJ, Luengo RM, Phillips JB: Magnetic compass mediates nocturnal homing by the alpine newt, Triturus alpestris. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2005, 58: 361-365. 10.1007/s00265-005-0951-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R: Magnetic orientation and magnetoreception in birds and other animals. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 2005, 191: 675-693. 10.1007/s00359-005-0627-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ugolini A: Equatorial sandhoppers use body scans to detect the earth's magnetic field. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 2006, 192: 45-49. 10.1007/s00359-005-0046-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W: Magnetoreception. Bioessays. 2006, 28: 157-168. 10.1002/bies.20363.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Lohmann KJ, Lohmann CMF, Putman NF: Magnetic maps in animals: nature's GPS. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2007, 210: 3697-3705. 10.1242/jeb.001313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wiltschko R, Gehring D, Denzau S, Gunturkun O, Wiltschko W: Interaction of magnetite-based receptors in the beak with the visual system underlying 'fixed direction' responses in birds. Frontiers in Zoology. 2010, 7: 24-10.1186/1742-9994-7-24.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Fischer JH, Freake MJ, Borland SC, Phillips JB: Evidence for the use of magnetic map information by an amphibian. Animal behaviour. 2001, 62: 1-10. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Deutschlander ME, Borland SC, Phillips JB: Extraocular magnetic compass in newts. Nature. 1999, 400: 324-325. 10.1038/22450.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Phillips JB, Borland SC: Behavioural evidence for use of a light-dependent magnetoreception mechanism by a vertebrate. Nature. 1992, 359: 142-144. 10.1038/359142a0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Phillips JB, Adler K, Borland SC: True navigation by an amphibian. Animal Behaviour. 1995, 50: 855-858. 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80146-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Sinsch U: Orientation and navigation in Amphibia. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology. 2006, 39: 65-71. 10.1080/10236240600562794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Freake MJ, Borland SC, Phillips JB, Guyer C: Use of a magnetic compass for Y-axis orientation in larval bullfrogs, Rana catesbeiana. Copeia. 2002, 2002: 466-471. 10.1643/0045-8511(2002)002[0466:UOAMCF]2.0.CO;2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Diego-Rasilla FJ, Phillips JB: Magnetic compass orientation in larval Iberian green frogs, Pelophylax perezi. Ethology. 2007, 113: 474-479. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01334.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Freake MJ, Phillips JB: Light-dependent shift in bullfrog tadpole magnetic compass orientation: evidence for a common magnetoreception mechanism in anuran and urodele amphibians. Ethology. 2005, 111: 241-254. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01067.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Diego-Rasilla FJ, Luengo RM, Phillips JB: Light-dependent magnetic compass in Iberian green frog tadpoles. Naturwissenschaften. 2010, 97: 1077-1088. 10.1007/s00114-010-0730-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Heusser H: Zur Laichplatzorientierung der Erdkröte Bufo bufo L. Mitteilungen der Naturforschenden Gesellschaft Schaffhausen. 1964, 28: 101-112.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Sinsch U: Orientation behaviour of toads (Bufo bufo) displaced from the breeding site. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 1987, 161: 715-727. 10.1007/BF00605013.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Diego-Rasilla F, Luengo R, Phillips JB: Use of a magnetic compass for nocturnal homing orientation in the palmate newt, Lissotriton helveticus. Ethology. 2008, 114: 808-815. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01532.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Muheim R, Edgar NM, Sloan KA, Phillips JB: Magnetic compass orientation in C57BL/6J mice. Learning & Behavior. 2006, 34: 366-373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Diego-Rasilla FJ, Luengo RM: Heterospecific call recognition and phonotaxis in the orientation behavior of the marbled newt, Triturus marmoratus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2004, 55: 556-560. 10.1007/s00265-003-0740-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Schlegel PA: Magnetic and other non-visual orientation mechanisms in some cave and surface urodeles. Journal of Ethology. 2008, 26: 347-359. 10.1007/s10164-007-0071-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Diego-Rasilla F, Luengo R: Acoustic orientation in the palmate newt, Lissotriton helveticus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2007, 61: 1329-1335. 10.1007/s00265-007-0363-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Rodríguez-García L, Diego-Rasilla F: Use of a magnetic compass for Y-axis orientation in premetamorphic newts (Triturus boscai). Journal of Ethology. 2006, 24: 111-116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Sinsch U: Initial orientation of newts (Triturus vulgaris, T. cristatus) following short-and long-distance displacements. Ethology Ecology and Evolution. 2007, 19: 201-214. 10.1080/08927014.2007.9522562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Heusser H: Die Lebensweise der Erdkröte Bufo bufo (L.); Wanderungen und Sommerquartiere. Revue Suisse de Zoologie. 1968, 75: 927-982.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Berger W, Butterweck H-J: Die Berechnung von Spulen zur Erzeugung homogener Magnetfelder und konstanter Feldgradienten. Electrical Engineering (Archiv fur Elektrotechnik). 1956, 42: 216-222.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Batschelet E: Circular statistics in biology. 1981, New York: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  30. Fisher NI: Statistical analysis of circular data. 1995, Cambridge: University Press

    Google Scholar 

  31. R Development Core Team: R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2010, Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing

    Google Scholar 

  32. Zentralanstalt für Meteorologie und Geodynamik: Magnetische Deklination: Werte für die Jahresmitte, 1. Juli 2010. 2010, Wien: Österreichischer Geophysikalischer Dienst

    Google Scholar 

  33. Endler JA: Kinesthetic orientation in the California newt (Taricha torosa). Behaviour. 1970, 37: 15-23. 10.1163/156853970X00204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Diego-Rasilla FJ: Homing ability and sensitivity to the geomagnetic field in the alpine newt, Triturus alpestris. Ethology Ecology & Evolution. 2003, 15: 251-259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Buck T: Untersuchungen zur Biologie der Erdkröte Bufo bufo L. unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der Erscheinungsformen und Mechanismen des Phänomens der Orientierung. 1988, University of Hamburg

    Google Scholar 

  36. Hettyey A, Vági B, Hévizi G, Török J: Changes in sperm stores, ejaculate size, fertilization success, and sexual motivation over repeated matings in the common toad, Bufo bufo (Anura: Bufonidae). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2009, 96: 361-371. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2008.01126.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Landler L, Gollmann G: Zur Orientierung von Amphibien am Erdmagnetfeld: Hypothesen, Experimente und offene Fragen. Schriften des Vereins zur Verbreitung naturwissenschaftlicher Kenntnisse.

  38. Freake MJ, Muheim R, Phillips JB: Magnetic maps in animals: a theory comes of age?. The Quarterly Review of Biology. 2006, 81: 327-347. 10.1086/511528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Cochran WW, Mouritsen H, Wikelski M: Migrating songbirds recalibrate their magnetic compass daily from twilight cues. Science. 2004, 304: 405-408. 10.1126/science.1095844.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Holland RA, Borissov I, Siemers BM: A nocturnal mammal, the greater mouse-eared bat, calibrates a magnetic compass by the sun. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010, 107: 6941-6945. 10.1073/pnas.0912477107.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Adler K: Individuality in the use of orientation cues by green frogs. Animal Behaviour. 1980, 28: 413-425. 10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80050-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Dall'Antonia P, Sinsch U: In search of water: orientation behaviour of dehydrated natterjack toads, Bufo calamita. Animal Behaviour. 2001, 61: 617-629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

These experiments would not have been possible without the support of many people. Our sincere thanks go to: Frank Landler for being the master builder of nearly all the equipment we used; Maria Landler-Gartmayer for passing vacations to support our research; the Federal Research and Training Centre for Forests, Natural Hazards and Landscape, Franz Kweton-Vogl and Michael Pfeffer for providing the testing site in Mariabrunn; Helmuth Horvath for essential advice how to build serviceable Helmholtz-coils and align the magnetic field; Roland Grössinger (Technische Universität Wien, Institut für Festkörperphysik) for the loan of the magnetometer; Horst Schneider for constructing the current limiter; the "Wiener Umweltschutzabteilung Magistratsabteilung 22" for authorizing investigations through their permit (MA 22 - 3726/2009); Albert Müller, Alfred Burian, Andrea Maier, Christine Wu, Felix Kohlmeister, Iris Starnberger, Marko Ascher, Markus Pail and Sara-Maria Schnedl for assistance in the field; Markus Pail for assistance with the video analysis; Alfred Burian, Birgit Gollmann and Sara-Maria Schnedl for helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript; Ulrich Sinsch and three anonymous reviewers for constructive criticism that helped to improve our presentation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Günter Gollmann.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

LL and GG conceived and organized this study, LL conducted the experiments and statistical analyses, LL and GG interpreted the results and wrote the paper. This work was submitted by LL to the Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Mag. rer. nat.; GG was supervisor of this thesis. Both authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Authors’ original file for figure 3

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Landler, L., Gollmann, G. Magnetic orientation of the Common Toad: establishing an arena approach for adult anurans. Front Zool 8, 6 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-8-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-8-6

Keywords