Introduction

Increasing evidence indicates that the highly polymorphic genes of the major histocompatibility complex (MHC) influence odour and mating preferences in house mice and humans [1]. MHC genes encode cell‐surface glycoproteins (class I and II molecules) that bind short peptides and present them to T lymphocytes. Through this mechanism, MHC genes control the immunological self/non‐self discrimination, and subsequently, tissue rejection and immune recognition of infectious diseases. Thus, it is suspected that the extraordinary polymorphism of MHC loci is maintained by balancing selection from infectious diseases, though direct evidence for this hypothesis is lacking [2]. Surprisingly, the best evidence indicates that MHC polymorphisms are driven by sexual selection. Studies in house mice indicate that both males and females prefer MHC‐dissimilar mates [35], who they apparently recognize by odour cues [6]. Studies in humans have also found MHC‐associated odour [7] and mating preferences [8]. Such disassortative mating preference could explain the diversity of MHC genes, though several questions remain unanswered about how the MHC influences odour production and why MHC‐dependent mating preferences evolved.

Hypotheses about the functional significance

Three non‐mutually exclusive hypotheses have been proposed to explain the function of MHC‐dependent mating preferences (reviewed in [1]). First, MHC‐dependent mate preference may function to produce certain MHC combinations or increased heterozygosity in offspring to increase resistance to infectious diseases. Although increased MHC heterozygosity is associated with increased resistance to HIV [9] and hepatitis [10], experimental infections with single infections in mice have not supported this idea [2]. Second, MHC‐dependent mate preference may enable hosts to provide a ‘moving target’ against rapidly evolving parasites that adapt to their host's MHC genotypes [1]. These two ideas can be called the ‘parasite hypotheses’. Third, MHC‐ dependent mate preferences may function to avoid inbreeding (the ‘inbreeding avoidance hypothesis’ [11,12]. Inbreeding has extremely negative fitness effects in wild house mice [13], therefore, inbreeding avoidance through genetic kin recognition would be advantageous. Humans, like other mammals, avoid inbreeding through familiarity (the Westermark effect) [14], and there is evidence that odour cues are used for recognizing kin [15].

Odour and mating preferences in humans

Two studies found MHC‐associated odour preferences, and one study found MHC‐dependent mating preferences. First, Wedekind et al. [7] found that women prefer the odour of MHC‐dissimilar men. Forty‐nine female and 44 male students were typed for their HLA‐A, ‐B and ‐DR. The men wore a T‐shirt for two nights. On the following day, the women were asked to judge the odours of six T‐shirts each, and the shirts’ odours were judged as more ‘pleasant’ when they had been worn by men whose MHC genotype was different from that of the judging woman. In contrast, the odours were judged less pleasant when the MHC genotype of the odour‐producing males and that of the judging women were similar. This difference in odour assessment was reversed when the women were taking oral contraceptives. Furthermore, the odours of MHC‐dissimilar men more frequently reminded the women of their own present or former partners than did the odours of MHC‐similar men. Although this is no direct evidence that odour influences mating preferences in humans (but see [16]), this study suggests that the MHC or linked genes influence human mate choice.

Second, Wedekind and Füri [17] tested whether odour preferences are aimed at producing offspring with certain MHC allele combinations (since certain combinations may offer increased resistance against pathogens) or simply increased heterozygosity. The former (but not the latter) possibility would specifically support the parasite hypotheses, since a preference for specific allele combinations that are beneficial under given environmental conditions would not be expected by the inbreeding avoidance hypothesis. The study was also designed to test for gender effects, and to get a first estimate of the amount of variance in pleasantness scorings that is correlated to the MHC. This time, 58 women and 63 men, all typed for their HLA‐A, ‐B and ‐DR, were asked to score the odours of six T‐shirts, worn always by the same two women and four men. The pleasantness scorings correlated again negatively with the degree of MHC similarity between smeller and T‐shirt wearer in men and in women who were not using the contraceptive pill (but not in pill users). Depending on the T‐shirt wearer, the amount of variance in the scorings of odour pleasantness that was explained by the degree of MHC similarity (=r2) varied between nearly 0 and 23%. The six T‐shirt‐wearers differed significantly from each other in the degree to which pleasantness scorings correlated to the MHC. There was no significant effect of gender in the correlation between pleasantness and MHC similarity: the highest r2 was actually reached with one of the male odours sniffed by male smellers. Men and women who were reminded of their own mate/ex‐mate when sniffing a T‐shirt had significantly fewer MHC‐alleles in common with this T‐shirt wearer than expected by chance. This suggests again that the MHC or linked genes influence human mate choice. This study found no significant influence of the MHC on odour preferences when the degree of similarity between T‐shirt wearer and smeller was statistically controlled. This negative finding suggests that body odour preferences are mainly influenced by the degree of similarity or dissimilarity at the MHC in the Swiss study population. The observed preferences would increase heterozygosity in the progeny, without producing specific combinations at the MHC. As mentioned above, a more specific choice of particular alleles would have provided strong evidence in favour of the parasite hypothesis, as opposed to the inbreeding avoidance hypothesis. However, this second study did not provide such evidence.

Third, Ober et al. [8] conducted a large study on American Hutterites, a reproductively isolated community of Austrian‐German ancestry, and found direct evidence for MHC‐disassortative mating preferences. Married couples were less likely to share MHC loci than expected by chance, even after inbreeding taboos were statistically controlled. However, Hedrick and Black [18] did not find such an effect in South Amerindians. Although the latter study had a lower statistical power (the sample size in their field study was much smaller than in the study by Ober et al. [8] while the heterogeneity on the MHC seemed to be larger, and fewer alleles had been analysed), the possibility remains that there might be population differences with respect to mating preferences. A study by Paterson and Pemberton [19] tested for MHC‐dependent mate preferences in another mammal, the Soay sheep (Ovis aries). In contrast to the many studies in mice and the studies on humans mentioned above, they could not find any indication of such mate preferences in an unmanaged population on a Scottish island. Their analysis was conservative with respect to their conclusion, and their sample size was large, which strongly suggests that MHC‐dependent mate choice is not universal among mammals.

Hypotheses about the mechanism of odour production

There is much evidence that MHC genes influence individual odour in laboratory mice and rats (reviewed in [20]), and several hypotheses have been proposed to explain how. First, since MHC molecules occur in the urine and sweat, they may provide the odourants [21]. This is unlikely since MHC molecules are large, involatile proteins, and furthermore, denaturation of proteins in urine does not destroy the distinguishability of MHC‐mediated odours by mice [22]. Second, MHC molecules bind to allele‐specific subsets of peptides, and their volatile metabolites, such as carboxylic acids, may provide the odourants. Class I MHC molecules bind peptides that are hydrophilic, highly evolutionarily conserved, universally expressed and derived from hydrophobic proteins, whereas Class II‐ bound peptides are more conserved than their source proteins but less conserved than class I‐bound peptides [23]. Singer et al. [24] found that the relative concentrations of volatile carboxylic acids were characteristic of the urinary odour of different MHC‐congenic inbred mouse strains. Third, MHC genes may alter odour by shaping specific populations of microbial flora, although the evidence for this idea is inconsistent [2527]. Fourth, MHC molecules may change their conformation to bind volatiles, instead of peptides, and carry them to scent glands [28]. Finally, when taken together, the evidence suggests that MHC‐bound peptides are metabolized and made volatile by microbes [20]).

Human chemical communication is not well understood and somewhat controversial. It is known that human skin has two types of glands that produce odour: (i) sebaceous glands, which are located all over the body, secrete an odourless oily liquid that is broken down by bacteria into volatile molecules, mostly fatty acids, and (ii) apocrine glands in the axillary region, which play an important role in odour production [29]. Protein carriers (lipocalins) bind and transport odourants to the axillae where they are metabolized and made volatile by bacteria [30]. Besides work on the MHC, there are other findings that indicate human odours play a role in sexual behaviour: (i) humans have a functional vomernasal organ (a chemical sensory system in mammals used to detect pheromones) [31], (ii) pheromones influence women's reproductive synchrony [32,33], and (iii) women prefer the odour of physically symmetrical men [34]. Interestingly, all of these studies on odour communication in humans, including studies on MHC genes, odour, and mating preferences, are rather controversial. Perhaps people are sceptical to the idea of chemical communication in our species because it goes on ‘right under our nose’, not necessarily with any conscious processing.

Correspondence and offprint requests to: Claus Wedekind, Institute of Cell, Animal and Population Biology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH9 3JT, UK.

Claus Wedekind is currently supported by the ‘Berner Hochschulstiftung’ and the Cloëtta Foundation. Dustin Penn is supported by NIH (gm39578) and NSF (IBN 9222177).

References

1

Penn D, Potts W. The evolution of mating preferences and major histocompatibility genes.

Am Nat
1999
;
153
:
145
–164

2

Apanius V, Penn D, Slev PR, Ruff LR, Potts WK. The nature of selection on the major histocompatibility complex.

Crit Rev Immunol
1997
;
17
:
179
–224

3

Yamazaki K, Boyse EA, Miké V et al. Control of mating preferences in mice by genes in the major histocompatibility complex.

J Exp Med
1976
;
144
:
1324
–1335

4

Egid K, Brown JL. The major histocompatibility complex and female mating preferences in mice.

Anim Behav
1989
;
38(3)
:
548
–549

5

Potts WK, Manning CJ, Wakeland EK. Mating patterns in seminatural populations of mice influenced by MHC genotype.

Nature
1991
;
352
:
619
–621

6

Yamazaki K, Yamaguchi M, Baranoski L, Bard J, Boyse EA, Thomas L. Recognition among mice: Evidence from the use of Y‐maze differentially scented by congenic mice of different major histocompatibity types.

J Exp Med
1979
;
150
:
755
–760

7

Wedekind C, Seebeck T, Bettens F, Paepke AJ. MHC‐dependent mate preferences in humans.

Proc R Soc Lond B
1995
;
260
:
245
–249

8

Ober C, Weitkamp LR, Cox N, Dytch H, Kostyu D, Elias S. HLA and mate choice in humans.

Am J Hum Genet
1997
;
61
:
497
–504

9

Carrington M, Nelson GW, Martin MP et al. HLA and HIV‐1: heterozygote advantage and B*35–Cw*04 disadvantage.

Science
1999
;
283(5408)
:
1748
–5172

10

Thursz MR, Thomas HC, Greenwood BM, Hill AVS. Heterozygote advantage for HLA class‐II type in hepatitis B virus infection (letter) (published erratum appears in Nat Genet 1998; 18: 88).

Nat Genet
1997
;
17
:
11
–12

11

Potts WK, Manning CJ, Wakeland EK. The role of infectious disease inbreeding and mating preferences in maintaining MHC diversity: an experimental test.

Phil Trans R Soc Lond
1994
;
346
:
369
–378

12

Brown JL, Eklund A. Kin recognition and the major histocompatibility complex: an integrative review.

Am Nat
1994
;
143
:
435
–461

13

Meagher S, Penn D, Potts WK. Male–male competition magnifies inbreeding depression in wild house mice.

Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
, In press

14

Wolf AP.

Sexual Attraction and Childhood Association: a Chinese Brief for Edward Westermark
. Stanford University Press, Stanford,
1995

15

Porter RH, Moore JD. Human kin recognition by olfactory cues.

Physiol Behav
1981
;
27
:
493
–495

16

Herz RS, Cahill ED. Differential use of sensory information in sexual behavior as a function of gender.

Hum Nat
1997
;
8
:
275
–289

17

Wedekind C, Füri S. Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?

Proc R Soc Lond B
1997
;
264
:
1471
–1479

18

Hedrick PW, Black FL. HLA and mate selection: no evidence in South Amerindians.

Am J Hum Genet
1997
;
61
:
505
–511

19

Paterson S, Pemberton JM. No evidence for major histocompatibility complex‐dependent mating patterns in a free‐living ruminant population.

Proc R Soc Lond B
1997
;
264
:
1813
–1819

20

Penn D, Potts W. How do major histocompatibility complex genes influence odor and mating preferences?

Adv Immunol
1998
;
69
:
411
–435

21

Singh PB, Brown RE, Roser B. MHC antigens in urine as olfactory recognition cues.

Nature
1987
;
327
:
161
–164

22

Singer AG et al. Chemistry of odor types in mice: Fractionation and bioassay.

J Chem Ecol
1993
;
19
:
569
–579

23

Yeager M, Carrington M, Hughes AL. Class I and class II MHC bind self peptide sets that are strikingly different in their evolutionary characteristics.

Immunogenetics
2000
;
51(1)
:
8
–15

24

Singer AG, Beauchamp GK, Yamazaki K. Volatile signals of the major histocompatibility complex in male mouse urine.

Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1997
;
94
:
2210
–2214

25

Howard JC. H‐2 and mating preferences.

Nature
1977
;
266
:
406
–408

26

Schellinck HM, Brown RE, Slotnick BM. Training rats to discriminate between the odors of individual conspecifics.

Anim Learn Behav
1991
;
19(3)
:
223
–233

27

Schellinck HM, Brown RE. Why does germfree rearing eliminate the odors of individuality in rats but not in mice. In: Doty RL, Müller‐Schwarze D (eds).

Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, VI.
Plenum Press: New York,
1992
;
237
–241

28

Pearse‐Pratt R, Schellinck H, Brown R, Roser B. Evolutionary and immunological implications of the role of the MHC in olfactory signalling. In: Doty RL, Müller‐Schwarze D (eds).

Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, VI.
Plenum Press: New York,
1992
;
167
–175

29

Stoddart DM.

The Scented Ape: The Biology and Culture of Human Odour
. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,  1990

30

Spielman AI, Zeng X, Leyden JL, Preti G. Proteinaceous precursors of human axillary odor: isolation of two novel odor binding proteins.

Experientia
1995
;
51
:
40

31

Monti‐Blonch L, Jennings‐White C, Dolberg DS, Berliner DL. The human vomernasal system.

Psychoneuroendocrinology
1994
;
19
:
673
–686

32

McClintock MK. Menstrual synchrony and suppression.

Nature
1971
;
229
:
224
–245

33

Stern K, McClintock MK. Regulation of ovulation by human pheromones.

Nature
1998
;
392
:
177
–179

34

Gangestad SW, Thornhill R. Menstrual cycle variation in women's preferences for the scent of symmetrical men.

Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
1998
;
265
:
927
–933

Comments

0 Comments
Submit a comment
You have entered an invalid code
Thank you for submitting a comment on this article. Your comment will be reviewed and published at the journal's discretion. Please check for further notifications by email.