Skip to main content

The Emergence of Complex Language

  • Chapter

Abstract

It has now become rather common to accept that any theory of language is constrained by evolutionary and comparative considerations, as much as considerations of the evolution of language should be constrained by linguistic considerations. This is to say: when and as we develop a theory of what language is like and why it is that way, it is useful to look at the archaeological record, as well as at other (non-linguistic) minds, in order to see what makes language special. At the same time, evolutionary hypotheses on the emergence of language benefit from some independent theoretically informed grasp of what the empirical properties of languages are.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Arsenijevic, B. and Hinzen, W. (2012). On the absence of X-within-X recursion in the design of human grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 43:3, to appear summer 2012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. 1934/1990. Theory of language: the representational function of language. Trans. D. F. Goodwin. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1999). The origins of complex language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D., Fotakopoulou, O. and Boroditsky, L. (2010). Space and time in the child’s mind: evidence for a cross-dimensional asymmetry. Cognitive Science 34: 387–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2005). Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36(1): 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2008). Approaching UG from below. In U. Sauerland and H.-M. Gärtner, (eds), Interfaces + Recursion = Language?, Berlin and New York: de Gruyter, pp. 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2010). Poverty of stimulus: unfinished business. Lecture presented in the Lecture Series ‘Sprache und Gehirn — Zur Sprachfähigkeit des Menschen’, University of Mainz, Summer 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. and Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 489–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. and N. Chater (2009). Language acquisition meets language evolution, Cognitive Science: 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2010). How to qualify for a cognitive upgrade: executive control, glass ceilings, and the limits of simian success. This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crow, T. (2009). A theory of the origin of cerebral asymmetry: epigenetic variation superimposed on a fixed right-shift. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 15(3): 289–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond J. (1992). The third chimpanzee: the evolution and future of the human animal. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, N. and Levinson, J. (2009). The myth of language universals: language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32: 429–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2010). Language evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. and Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (2010). What Darwin got wrong. London: Profile Books Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett, J. A. (1992). Tools: The paleolithic record. In S. Jones, R. D. Martin and D.

    Google Scholar 

  • R. Pilbeam (eds), Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 350–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. and Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298, 22 November 2002: 1569–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinzen, W. (2006). Mind design and minimal syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hinzen, W. (2010). Emergence of a systemic semantics through minimal and under-specified codes. In C. Boeckx and A.-M. di Sciullo (eds), The biolinguistic enterprise. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 417–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krause, J., et al. (2007). The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals. Current Biology 17: 1908–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, S. and Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGonigle, B. O. and Chalmers, M. (2007). Ordering and executive functioning as a window on the evolution and development of cognitive systems. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 19(2): 241–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGonigle, B. O., Chalmers, M. and Dickinson, A. (2003). Concurrent disjoint and reciprocal classification by Cebus Apella in serial ordering tasks: evidence for hierarchical organization. Animal Cognition 6: 185–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars, P. and Stringer, C. (eds) (1989). The human revolution: modelling the early human mind. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mukherji, N. (2009). The primacy of grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(2): 109–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 704–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. and Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: what’s special about it?, Cognition 95: 201–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quine, W. V. O. (1953). From a logical point of view. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roeper, T. (2009). The prism of grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • The Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium 2005. Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome. Nature 437(1), September 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tattersall, I. (2004). What happened in the origin of human consciousness? The Anatomical Record (Part B: New Anat.) 276B: 19–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. (2005). Metacognition and the evolution of language. In H. Terrace and P. Metcalfe (eds), The missing link in cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 84–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). The origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2012 Wolfram Hinzen

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hinzen, W. (2012). The Emergence of Complex Language. In: McFarland, D., Stenning, K., McGonigle-Chalmers, M. (eds) The Complex Mind. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230354456_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics