INTRODUCTION

Anxiety disorders like posttraumatic stress disorder are characterized by deficits in the ability to inhibit fear. Fear inhibition can be studied using fear extinction, a procedure in which subjects are repeatedly presented a fear-eliciting conditioned stimulus (CS, eg, a noise that was previously paired with shock) in the absence of aversive outcome until fear responses decline. Extinction recall is assessed the following day by measuring fear responses in the presence of the extinguished CS. Fear extinction forms the basis of exposure therapy, the current psychological treatment of choice for anxiety disorders. Investigating the behavioral and neurobiological processes that underpin fear extinction is thought to be a promising avenue through which novel means of augmenting the efficacy of exposure therapy can be identified (Graham and Milad, 2011).

Although a comprehensive cross-species model of the mechanisms underlying fear extinction has been developed, this model is grossly limited by the fact that it has been based on studies that have almost exclusively been conducted in males. Indeed, fear extinction has only been studied in females recently. This research has revealed that female rats exhibit markedly different extinction recall depending on when they were extinguished during the 4-day estrous (menstrual) cycle (Chang et al, 2009; Milad et al, 2009; Graham and Milad, 2013; Rey et al, 2014; Gruene et al, 2015). Specifically, female rats exhibit low fear responses at extinction recall only when extinguished during proestrus, when levels of the sex hormones estradiol and progesterone are high. Conversely, they exhibit return of fear at extinction recall when extinguished during metestrus, a period of low estradiol and progesterone levels.

Although it is clear that fluctuations in ovarian sex hormones modulate fear extinction, the exact role of each hormone is unknown. In the naturally cycling rat, following a period of low estradiol and progesterone, increases in estradiol precede and then overlap with increases in progesterone. Consequently, research with naturally cycling females cannot easily dissociate the distinct effects of these different hormonal phases on fear extinction. A common approach used by previous research aimed at understanding other effects of the estrous cycle is to surgically remove the gonads, thus eliminating the primary endogenous source of sex hormones. The hormones of interest can then be systematically replaced in a pattern that mimics the cyclic changes in these hormones. For example, Woolley and McEwen (1993) demonstrated that estradiol treatment alone in ovariectomized (OVX) female rats led to increased hippocampal dendritic spine densities within 48 h, an effect that persisted for 6 days. When progesterone was administered 2 days after estradiol treatment, even greater increases in spine density were observed within 6 h, but within 24 h spine density declined to basal levels. These results mirrored cyclic fluctuations in spine density in intact rats, where spine density is lowest during metestrus and greatest during proestrus. Woolley and McEwen (1993) inferred that in the naturally cycling rat, estradiol increases spine density, whereas progesterone initially augments and then rapidly attenuates estradiol’s effects. This was supported by additional findings demonstrating that blocking progesterone receptor activation during proestrus prevented the subsequent decline in dendritic spine density in intact rats.

The study by Woolley and McEwen (1993), in addition to providing insight to the physiological consequences of fluctuating ovarian sex hormones, established an ecologically valid means of assessing the hormonal mechanisms underlying functional changes across the estrous cycle. The aim of the present study was to apply their method to identify how estradiol and progesterone interact to produce rapid cyclic changes in fear extinction in naturally cycling female rats. In our initial experiments, we examined the consequences of Woolley and McEwen’s (1993) hormone treatment protocol on fear extinction in OVX rats. Following fear-conditioning OVX rats received estradiol, alone or in combination with progesterone. In experiment 1, we determined whether estradiol alone could facilitate fear extinction when it was administered 48 h before extinction training. In experiment 2, we assessed the impact of progesterone, administered 6 or 24 h before extinction training, on fear extinction in estradiol-treated rats. Finally, in experiment 3, we translated our findings from OVX to intact rats by assessing the impact of preventing progesterone receptor activation during proestrus in naturally cycling rats subsequently extinguished during metestrus. Combined, these approaches provide novel insights to the nature and time-course of estradiol and progesterone’s effects on fear extinction in female rats.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Experimentally naïve Sprague–Dawley-derived female rats, aged 10–12 weeks, obtained from a commercial supplier (Animal Resources Centre, Perth, Australia), were used. Rats were housed in groups of eight in plastic cages (67 × 30 × 22 cm3) in a 20–22 °C colony room. They were maintained on a 12 h light–dark cycle (lights on at 0700 h) with food and water available ad libitum. Procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Ethics Committee at UNSW Australia and followed guidelines outlined in The Australian Code Of Practice For The Care and Use Of Animals For Scientific Purposes (7th edition, 2004).

Surgery

In experiments 1 and 2 rats received bilateral ovariectomy (OVX). Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (Laser Animal Health, Australia) mixed with oxygen. Bupivacaine (Cenvet, Australia) was applied to the skin before a dorsal midline incision was made caudal to the posterior border of the ribs. The abdominal wall muscles were separated and the periovarian fat was exposed to isolate the ovary, fallopian tube, and uterine horn. The fallopian tube was clamped and the ovary and oviduct removed. The uterine horn and periovarian fat were returned to the abdominal cavity and the incision was sutured and closed with wound clips. Immediately after surgery rats were injected i.m. with 0.15 ml each of 100 mg/ml cephazolin and 300 mg/ml benacillen, and s.c. with 0.05 ml of rimadyl (all from Cenvet, Australia). Rats were given 4–5 days recovery before behavioral training, during which time they were handled and weighed daily.

Estrous Cycling

In experiment 3, vaginal smears were conducted daily (between 0800 and 0010 h) to determine estrous cycle phase in naturally cycling rats. This commenced 4 days before fear conditioning and ended the day of testing to ensure regular cycling throughout the experiment. A cotton-tip moistened with 0.9% saline was inserted into the vaginal canal and rapidly twisted to collect epithelial cells, which were transferred to a microscope slide, dyed with KwikDiff Stain Kit (Thermofisher, Australia), and inspected under a light microscope (× 10). Rats have a 4-day cycle with distinct vaginal cytology in each phase (estrus, metestrus, diestrus, and proestrus). Phases were identified by the presence of leukocytes (metestrus/diestrus), nucleated cells (proestrus), and cornified cells (estrus), as outlined by Becker et al (2005).

Hormone and Drug Administration

In experiments 1 and 2, OVX rats received two s.c. injections of β-Estradiol (Sigma-Aldrich, Sydney, Australia; 10 μg in 150 μl sesame oil), or an equivalent volume of sesame oil. In experiment 2, rats received a single s.c. injection of progesterone (Sigma-Aldrich; 500 μg in sesame oil), or an equivalent volume of sesame oil, administered after the second estradiol injection. In experiment 3, naturally cycling rats received two s.c. injections of the progesterone receptor antagonist RU486 (Sigma-Aldrich, Australia; 2 mg in 0.2 ml sesame oil), or an equivalent volume of sesame oil. The estradiol and progesterone doses were identical to those used by Woolley and McEwen (1993) and create physiological hormone levels that mimic those of naturally cycling rats during proestrus. The RU486 dose was used by Woolley and McEwen (1993) to block progesterone receptor activity during proestrus.

Apparatus

Four Med Associates experimental chambers (24 × 30 × 21 cm3) were used for conditioning, extinction training, and recall. Chambers were housed in separate wooden cabinets to minimize external auditory and visual stimulation; ventilation fans provided low, constant background noise (~58 dB). An infrared video camera mounted on the rear wall of the cabinets recorded the behavior of each rat inside the chamber. The chambers were divided into two sets that differed in visual and tactile features. The first set (designated context A) had front and rear walls and ceilings constructed of clear Perspex. The sidewalls were stainless steel, and one was embedded with a high-frequency speaker. The floor consisted of stainless steel rods (4 mm diameter, spaced 16 mm apart center to center), connected to a shock generator, with a stainless steel tray of corn cob bedding 3.5 cm beneath. These chambers were illuminated by infrared light from the camera. The second set (designated context B) differed from context A in that the front walls were covered with patterned paper (2.5 cm wide vertical black and white stripes), and the ceilings and stainless steel rod floor were overlaid with a sheet of opaque Perspex, below which was an empty stainless steel tray. White light from a table lamp positioned on top of the chambers provided illumination.

The CS was a white noise (4 dB above background noise) delivered through the sidewall speaker, and the US was a scrambled foot-shock (1 s, 0.6 mA) delivered through the floor.

Procedure

Handling and context pre-exposure

Rats were handled for 4–5 min each day for three consecutive days; after handling on each day rats were individually placed in context A for 10 min.

Fear conditioning

Conditioning was identical in all experiments. Rats were placed in context A and after a 2 min adaptation period the CS was presented for 10 s and coterminated with the shock US. Rats received three CS-US pairings (intertrial interval 85–135 s, with an average of 110 s). Conditioning occurred before hormone administration in experiments 1 and 2, and during diestrus in experiment 3.

Hormone replacement/drug administration

In all experiments hormone/drug administration began the day after fear conditioning to avoid introducing potentially confounding effects on the acquisition or consolidation of learned fear. In experiment 1, two groups received two injections of estradiol or vehicle, spaced 24 h apart. In experiment 2, three groups received two injections of estradiol, and one group received vehicle injections, spaced 24 h apart. Two days after the second estradiol or vehicle injection, two of the estradiol-treated groups received progesterone (groups ‘E+P 6 h’ and ‘E+P 24 h’), whereas the third estradiol-treated group (group ‘estradiol’) and the vehicle group (group ‘vehicle’) received vehicle. In experiment 3, rats received two injections of RU486 (group ‘RU486’) or vehicle (groups ‘proestrus’ and ‘metestrus’) at 1300 and 1800 h during proestrus, the day after conditioning.

Extinction training

Extinction training in all experiments was identical. Rats were placed in context B and after a 2 min adaptation period, received 6 × 2 min CS presentations, with an intertrial interval of 2 min. In experiment 1, extinction training occurred 48 h after the second injection. In experiment 2, extinction training occurred 6 h (group E+P 6 h), or 24 h (group E+P 24 h) after the progesterone administration. Half of groups vehicle and estradiol received extinction training 6 h, and the other half 24 h, after the final vehicle injection. After determining that there were no differences dependent on the time of extinction training (Supplementary Figure S1), data were collapsed to create two groups (‘vehicle’ and ‘estradiol’). Thus, this design controlled for the different amounts of time elapsing between conditioning and extinction recall in the E+P 24 h and the vehicle/estradiol groups. In experiment 3, groups RU486 and metestrus were extinguished 2 days after RU486/vehicle injections during metestrus, whereas group proestrus was extinguished on the morning of proestrus before vehicle administration.

Extinction recall

The test procedure was identical in all experiments. Rats were placed in context B, and following a 1-min adaptation period, received a single 2 min CS presentation. In experiments 1 and 2, rats were tested for extinction recall 24 h after extinction training. In experiment 3, groups RU486 and metestrus were tested 24 h, and group proestrus was tested 72 h, after extinction training. Thus, all rats were conditioned and tested during diestrus, and the time between conditioning and extinction recall was equivalent across groups. For an overview of the experimental timeline for all experiments, see Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1 Experimental Timeline for Experiments 1 and 2
Table 2 Experimental Timeline for Experiment 3

Scoring

Rats were scored for freezing, defined as the absence of movement except that required for respiration (Fanselow, 1980), during extinction training and recall. Freezing was scored using a time-sampling procedure whereby every 3 s the animal was scored as ‘freezing’ or ‘not freezing’. A percentage score was calculated for each animal to determine the proportion of total observations spent freezing. A second observer blind to the experimental condition of rats scored a random sample (40%) of data. The inter-rater reliability across all experiments was high (average r=0.92).

Statistical analyses

In experiment 1, pre-CS freezing (during the adaptation periods before extinction training and recall) and CS-elicited freezing during extinction recall were analysed using independent samples t-tests, and in experiments 2 and 3, a one-way analysis of variance. Freezing during within-session extinction training was analyzed using a repeated measures analysis of variance with group as a between-subjects factor. Main effects of group were further analyzed using Student–Newman–Keuls method. Three statistical outliers were removed from the analysis — two due to their pre-CS freezing at extinction recall being >10 STDEVs away from the mean (from the estradiol group in experiment 1, and the E+P 6 h group in experiment 2), and one due to its CS-elicited freezing during recall being>4 STDEVs away from the mean (from the metestrus group in experiment 3).

RESULTS

Experiment 1

There were no differences between the two groups in pre-CS freezing before extinction training (t(20)=0.84, P=0.41; Figure 1a). During extinction training, there was a significant effect of extinction trial (F(5,100)=29.58, P<0.0001), and no effect of group or trial-by-group interaction (F-values<1), indicating that both groups exhibited comparable conditioning and rates of extinction (Figure 1a). Both groups exhibited comparable, low, pre-CS freezing during the adaptation period before extinction recall, (t(20)=0.4, P=0.69), however the groups differed significantly in CS-elicited freezing (t(20)=2.23, P=0.038), with estradiol-treated rats exhibiting significantly lower freezing than vehicle-treated rats (Figure 1b).

Figure 1
figure 1

(a) Mean (±SEM) pre-CS, and CS-elicited, freezing for groups vehicle (n=11) and estradiol (n=11) during extinction training in experiment 1. (b) Mean (±SEM) pre-CS, and CS-elicited, freezing during test for groups during extinction recall in experiment 1. *Estradiol<vehicle (P<0.05).

PowerPoint slide

Experiment 2

There were no group differences in pre-CS freezing before extinction training (F<1; Figure 2a). During extinction training, there was a significant effect of extinction trial (F(5,275)=53.87, P<0.0001), and no effect of group or trial-by-group interaction (F-values<1), indicating that all groups exhibited comparable conditioning and rates of extinction (Figure 2a). All groups exhibited comparable, low, pre-CS freezing during the adaptation period before extinction recall, (F<1), however the groups differed significantly in CS-elicited freezing (F(3,58)=11.39, P<0.0001; Figure 2b). Post hoc tests indicated that group estradiol exhibited significantly lower freezing relative to group vehicle (replicating the findings from experiment 1) as well as group E+P 24 h, whereas group E+P 6 h exhibited significantly lower freezing than all other groups (P-values<0.05).

Figure 2
figure 2

(a) Mean (±SEM) pre-CS, and CS-elicited, freezing for groups vehicle (n=16), estradiol (n=16), E+P 6 h (n=13), E+P 24 h (n=14) during extinction training in experiment 2. (b) Mean (±SEM) pre-CS, and CS-elicited, freezing for groups during test for extinction recall in experiment 2. *E+P 6 h<estradiol (P<0.05), **estradiol and E+P 6 h<vehicle and E+P 24 h (P<0.05).

PowerPoint slide

To ensure that the differences in CS-elicited freezing at extinction recall between the E+P 6 h and E+P 24 h groups were not merely due to the difference in time interval between fear conditioning and extinction recall, we conducted several post-hoc tests. First, we conducted a 2 × 2 analysis of variance on the vehicle and estradiol sub-groups. In this analysis, the first factor corresponded to drug condition (vehicle or estradiol) and the second factor corresponded to time interval between final injection and extinction training (6 or 24 h). This analysis revealed a significant main effect of drug condition (F(1,32)=7.01, P=0.013), with no main effect of time interval or interaction between the two factors (P-values>0.6). Thus, in vehicle and estradiol-treated rats, time interval alone did not influence CS-elicited freezing at extinction recall (Supplementary Figure S1B). Furthermore, we compared the E+P 24 h group to the two estradiol sub-groups (ie, E 6 h, which had the same conditioning-extinction recall interval as the E+P 6 h group, and E 24h, which had the same conditioning-extinction recall interval as the E+P 24 h group), and in both cases, there was a significant difference (P-values<0.04). Together, this suggests that the impaired extinction recall in the E+P 24 h group relative to the E+P 6 h group was not due to the difference in time interval between conditioning and extinction recall.

Experiment 3

There were no group differences in pre-CS freezing before extinction training (F<1; Figure 3a). During extinction training, there was a significant effect of extinction trial (F(5,135)=12.68, P<0.0001), and no effect of group or trial-by-group interaction (F-values<1), indicating that all groups exhibited comparable conditioning and rates of extinction (Figure 3a). All groups exhibited comparable, low, pre-CS freezing during the adaptation period before extinction recall, (F<1), however the groups differed significantly in CS-elicited freezing (F(2,29)=4, P=0.03; Figure 3b). Post hoc tests indicated that the proestrus and RU486 groups exhibited significantly lower freezing relative to the metestrus group (P<0.05).

Figure 3
figure 3

(a) Mean (±SEM) pre-CS, and CS-elicited, freezing for groups metestrus (n=9), proestrus (n=10), and RU486 (n=11) during extinction training in experiment 3. (b) Mean (±SEM) pre-CS, and CS-elicited, freezing for groups during test for extinction recall in experiment 3. *Proestrus and RU486<metestrus (P<0.05).

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

The present experiments demonstrate that estradiol alone facilitates fear extinction in OVX rats. This effect was potentiated by progesterone when extinction training occurred 6 h following progesterone administration. Conversely, estradiol-induced facilitation of fear extinction was abolished by progesterone if extinction training occurred 24 h following progesterone administration. The present experiments are the first to systematically isolate the impact of the various hormonal changes across the estrous cycle on fear extinction. On the basis of our findings in OVX rats, we infer that in the naturally cycling female, increases in estradiol may be responsible for the enhancement in fear extinction during proestrus. Conversely, the impact of increasing progesterone levels is likely time-dependent: augmenting estradiol’s facilitation of fear extinction within 6 h, but rapidly reversing this within 24 h. This inference is supported by the outcome of the third experiment, which demonstrated that preventing progesterone receptor activation during proestrus rescued the impairment in extinction recall in rats extinguished during metestrus. Together, these experiments suggest that during the estrous cycle, whereas estradiol mediates enhancements in fear extinction observed during proestrus, progesterone ultimately mediates the impairments in fear extinction observed during metestrus.

The present findings on fear extinction are broadly consistent with past research illustrating that progesterone antagonizes estradiol’s effects in other domains of learning and memory. For example, in estradiol-treated OVX rats, progesterone augmented the beneficial effect of estradiol on spatial memory when administered 90 min before test, but reversed estradiol’s effects when administered 24 h before test (Sandstrom and Williams, 2001). Likewise, chronic estradiol administration enhanced spatial memory in OVX middle-aged rats, and this was prevented by co-administration of chronic progesterone (Bimonte-Nelson et al, 2006). Similarly, in OVX mice, a low dose of progesterone co-administered with estradiol prevented the enhancement in object recognition caused by estradiol alone (Harburger et al, 2009). In addition to its impact on estradiol-induced increases in dendritic spines, progesterone has been shown to reverse several other physiological consequences of chronic estradiol administration, including enhanced neuroprotection following brain injury (Rosario et al, 2006), and increases in neurotrophic factors BDNF, NGF, and NT3 (Bimonte-Nelson et al, 2004). Progesterone also prevents acute physiological effects of estradiol, including rapid phosphorylation of extracellular-signal regulated kinase (Harburger et al, 2009), and CREB (Murphy and Segal, 2000). The exact mechanisms by which progesterone antagonizes estradiol’s behavioral and physiological effects are unclear, although in cultured hippocampal cells progesterone prevents estradiol-induced dendritic spine formation through conversion to its metabolite, the neurosteroid tetrahydroprogesterone (Murphy and Segal, 2000). As tetrahydroprogesterone potentiates GABAA receptor activity, it is possible that GABA-mediated increases in neuronal inhibition may reverse estradiol-facilitated increases in neuronal excitation. It remains to be tested whether similar downstream processes mediate progesterone’s reversal of estradiol-facilitated extinction.

We and others have previously demonstrated that estradiol and progesterone facilitate fear extinction when administered just before or immediately after extinction training (Milad et al, 2009; Zeidan et al, 2011; Graham and Milad, 2013). This facilitation is very likely due to non-genomic effects of these hormones on cell signaling and excitability via membrane bound receptors. In the present experiments, however, extinction training took place 48 h after estradiol administration and either 6 or 24 h after progesterone administration. This is beyond the time points by which these hormones are metabolized and it is therefore unlikely that the facilitation in fear extinction was due to an acute impact of estradiol and/or progesterone on signaling processes underlying long-term memory formation (eg, phosphorylation of extracellular-signal regulated kinase and CREB, or upregulation of neurotrophic factors). Instead, our results strongly suggest that the influence of estradiol and progesterone on fear extinction in OVX rats was due to the longer-term, genomic consequences of these hormones, and in turn, this raises the possibility that fluctuations in fear extinction during the estrous cycle may be mediated, at least in part, by similar mechanisms. One candidate genomic mechanism relates to hormone-induced alterations in basal dendritic spine density. Our experiments provide indirect evidence for this proposition; extinction training was precisely timed to occur during periods of low, intermediate, or high densities of dendritic spines, based on well-established prior research by Woolley and McEwen (1993), and our findings demonstrated that extinction recall was exactly proportional to the expected spine densities during extinction training. That is, optimal extinction recall was achieved by matching extinction training to the time point at which spine densities were expected to be greatest (ie, 6 h post-progesterone administration in estradiol-treated rats), whereas return of fear during extinction recall was observed when extinction training was matched to the time point at which spine densities were expected to be lowest (ie, 24 h post-progesterone administration in estradiol-treated rats). Moreover, by blocking progesterone receptor activation during proestrus, a manipulation which has been demonstrated to prevent the cyclic collapse of dendritic spines, extinction recall in metestrus rats (that normally exhibit return of fear) was spared. Dendritic spines have a role in long-term memory formation (Leuner and Shors, 2004), and it has recently been demonstrated that fear extinction causes new spine formation within the prefrontal cortex, the levels of which are positively correlated with extinction recall (ie, reduced fear expression; Lai et al, 2012). Although the relationship between hippocampal spine density and fear extinction has not been investigated, it could be speculated that hormonal-induced increases or decreases in basal dendritic spine density could create a neuronal environment that is more or less permissive, respectively, for the consolidation of fear extinction. This possibility needs to be examined in future research that combines behavioral and morphological analyses.

Fear extinction is the laboratory basis of exposure therapy for anxiety disorders, and identical cyclic fluctuations in fear extinction to those observed in female rats have been documented to occur throughout the menstrual cycle in healthy women (eg, Milad et al, 2010; Graham and Milad, 2013) as well as in women with posttraumatic stress disorder (Glover et al, 2012). The implication of these findings is that exposure therapy may be less effective in women during periods of low hormonal levels. It has been suggested that estradiol, on its own or in combination with progesterone, may be a potentially useful pharmacological adjunct to augment the beneficial effects of exposure therapy in women (Milad et al, 2010; Glover et al, 2012; Graham and Milad, 2013). Indeed, in women who have not undergone hysterectomy, long-term estradiol treatment (as would need to occur for more complex anxiety disorders requiring multiple exposure sessions) must be accompanied by progesterone. The present findings suggest that although optimal therapeutic benefit could be achieved via an adjunct that includes both estradiol and progesterone, unless the timing of treatment relative to hormone administration is carefully controlled, progesterone may antagonize the effects of estradiol and prevent its benefit altogether. We have already demonstrated that a single dose of estradiol alone enhances fear extinction in healthy women (Graham and Milad, 2013); further studies are now needed to assess whether co-administration of estradiol and progesterone has similar biphasic effects on fear extinction in humans. Such studies may prove useful in identifying the most effective means by which hormone-based pharmacological adjuncts could be implemented to optimize treatments for anxiety disorders.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

This work was supported by an Australian Research CouncilDiscovery Early Career Research Award (DE140100243) anda UNSW Australia early career research grant to BMG. The authors declare no conflict of interest.