INTRODUCTION

The Prefrontal–Amygdala Circuit

The concept that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) controls and inhibits the amygdala and other limbic structures, termed ‘the reptilian brain’, was proposed many years ago (McLean, 1955). Abundant preclinical data indicate that areas of the PFC exert inhibitory control over the amygdala. A series of experiments in rats have shown that the medial PFC inhibits activity in the basolateral amygdala by stimulating inhibitory interneurons in the amygdala (Rosenkranz and Grace, 1999, 2002). Many other such studies that have shown this phenomenon in rodents (al Maskati and Zbrozyna, 1989; Halasz et al, 2002; Jinks and McGregor, 1997; McDonald and Mascagni, 1996; Morgan and LeDoux, 1995; Zbrozyna and Westwood, 1991). In primates, damage to the lateral PFC causes a loss of inhibitory control in attention tasks (Dias et al, 1996; Stefanacci and Amaral, 2002), whereas damage to orbital frontal cortex (OFC) causes a loss of inhibitory control in ‘affective’ processing and increased aggression (Izquierdo et al, 2005). In the macaque monkey, whereas both lateral and medial areas within OFC have strong association with limbic regions, lateral OFC has specific connections to the amygdala (Carmichael and Price, 1995).

In human beings, functional brain imaging provides an approach in assessing the relationship between prefrontal and amygdala function by examining the correlation coefficients between activity in the two structures. Human studies with 18fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG)-positron emission tomography (PET) and fMRI have suggested a significant correlation between these two structures, but have primarily been carried out in healthy subjects. The directionality of these correlations has been inconsistent, with some studies showing positive correlations between areas of PFC and amygdala (Pezawas et al, 2005), and other studies showing negative correlations (Hariri et al, 2000, 2003). Specifically, one fMRI study during a perceptual task involving viewing pictures of frightening faces showed significant negative correlation between rostral anterior cingulate, but negative correlations between amygdala and caudal anterior cingulate (Pezawas et al, 2005), whereas another study with a similar task showed a negative correlation between right PFC and amygdala activity (Hariri et al, 2000). Significant negative correlations were also identified between the response of the left amygdala and those of the right OFC as well as the anterior cingulate (Hariri et al, 2003). An fMRI study of surprised faces show increased activation of the right ventral amygdala when the subject interpreted the face negatively, whereas a positive interpretation of the face yielded activation of OFC (Kim et al, 2003). A subsequent study by the same group showed activation of lateral OFC in response to negative vs positive sentences, whereas medial OFC was activated in response to positive vs negative sentences (Kim et al, 2004), suggesting that subregions of OFC may play different roles in relation to amygdala function.

Studies of patients with affective disorder (Drevets et al, 1992) and post-traumatic stress disorder (Shin et al, 2005) have tended to find negative correlations between specifically medial PFC and amygdala in patients but not controls, suggesting coupling only in psychopathology. Recent data suggest a role for serotonin in modulating connectivity between PFC and the amygdala, with subjects carrying the ‘short’ allele of the serotonin transporter gene (a gene possibly conferring a risk for mood disorders), showing greater fMRI amygdala/medial OFC ‘coupling’ during an emotional picture viewing task than those with only the ‘long’ allele (Hariri et al, 2006; Heinz et al, 2005; Pezawas et al, 2005).

Subregions of the Amygdala in Emotion

Subregions of the human amygdala have been shown to have specific functions, roughly organized on a dorsoventral dimension. The ventral amygdala includes primarily the basolateral complex (BLC) and the dorsal nucleus of the Central Nucleus (CN). Whalen et al (2001) suggest that the human dorsal vs ventral designation within the amygdala provides a means for incorporating numerous results from the animal literature offering compelling evidence that the BLC (located ventrally in the human) can be dissociated behaviorally from the central nucleus (located dorsally in the human) and is the component of the amygdala predominantly involved in emotion modulation. Further, they note that whereas expressions of fear appear to activate the BLC and CN (ie ventral and dorsal amygdala), anger may involve the CN to a lesser degree (than the BLC) because less additional information concerning the stimulus is required (Whalen et al, 2001). Furthermore, fMRI studies by this group have shown that specifically the ventral amygdala is in response to emotional stimuli (Kim et al, 2003, 2004). The ventral amygdala (basolateral amygdala in rats)–OFC circuit role in associative encoding and aversive odor was also supported in rat studies of amygdala activity in OFC (Saddoris et al, 2005; Schoenbaum et al, 2003).

The amygdala has been implicated not only in the processing of negative emotion in general, but also more specifically in the production of aggressive behavior in animal studies. Electric stimulation of the lateral nucleus of the amygdala in cats results in predatory attack behavior (Gregg and Siegel, 2001). In prairie voles, the medial nucleus of the amygdala has been shown to be involved in the regulation of aggression towards intruders (Wang et al, 1997). In primates, ablation of the amygdala bilaterally leads to increased social affiliation and decreased aggression (Emery et al, 2001; Meunier et al, 1999). These data have been taken to demonstrate a central role for the amygdala in the production of aggression, and led to the successful use of unilateral amygdalectomy in the treatment of a small number of cases of pathological aggression in human beings (Sachdev et al, 1992).

Borderline Personality Disorder as a Prototype of Emotion Dysregulation

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is an illness, characterized by the symptom of emotional dysregulation and disinhibited anger, which often leads to aggressive behavior. The model of altered prefrontal–amygdala connectivity provides a model for the primary symptom in BPD, disinhibition of emotion. To date, however, ours is the first study in BPD examining the relationship between prefrontal regions and amygdala in BPD.

A number of studies have reported abnormal PFC in BPD, as well as in impulsive aggressive subjects with a variety of personality disorders. An 18FDG-PET study reported reduced glucose metabolism in BPD patients compared to healthy controls in PFC, and anterior cingulate bilaterally (De La Fuente et al, 1997). In response to serotonergic challenge, specifically impulsive-aggressive BPD patients demonstrate decreased metabolism in anterior cingulate and PFC, compared to controls (New et al, 2002; Siever et al, 1999b; Soloff et al, 2003). Numerous studies have demonstrated decreased serotonergic responsiveness in impulsive aggressive patients with personality disorders (Coccaro, 1989; Dougherty et al, 1999; New et al, 2004; O'Keane et al, 1992; Virkkunen et al, 1994), and impulsive aggression has been shown to respond the treatment with SSRIs (Coccaro and Kavoussi, 1997). We have recently reported gray matter reduction in anterior cingulate (BA 24) in a large sample of BPD patients (n=50) compared with healthy controls (n=50) (Hazlett et al, 2005). In an analysis of a large sample of BPD-(IED) impulsive aggressive subjects, we found that male subjects with IED-BPD have hypometabolism widely across the frontal lobe compared to healthy men, healthy women and women with BPD in response to placebo (New et al, under review). This finding extends our prior finding of decreased rGMR in response to m-CPP in IED-BPD in anterior and increase in posterior cingulate compared to controls in a larger sample (New et al, 2002). We find similar evidence of anterior cingulate decreases and posterior cingulate increases in activation after serotonergic stimulus. We report these findings in a separate manuscript from the present one as they are conceptually quite different. The present study is a correlational analysis, examining the relationship of activity in different brain regions, whereas our replications study closely follows the analysis of our previous publication (New et al, 2002), additionally exploring the role of aggression subtype and sex on the findings.

To date, only few neuroimaging studies have evaluated amygdala volume or activity in BPD and none specifically in aggressive subjects. An early study of amygdala volume in BPD showed that total amygdala volume tended to be reduced in female BPD subjects compared to controls (Driessen et al, 2000). Two subsequent studies also reported decreased amygdala volume in BPD compared to controls in relatively small samples (Schmahl et al, 2003; Tebartz van Elst et al, 2003), although a recent study showed no difference in amygdala volume compared to controls (Brambilla et al, 2004) and a VBM extension study showed decreases only in the left hippocampus/amygdala complex (Rusch et al, 2003; Tebartz van Elst et al, 2003). A recent larger study employing a software package ‘BRAINS’ showed no difference in amygdala volume in BPD compared to controls, although those BPD patients with a concurrent major depressive episode had larger amygdala volumes compared to those without (Zetzsche et al, 2006).

Functional imaging studies of BPD are also limited in number. One study of six female BPD patients and six healthy volunteers (Herpertz et al, 2001) showed that BPD patients had greater cerebral blood flow (BOLD) signal in the amygdala bilaterally during unpleasant pictures compared with neutral pictures than healthy controls. Another study reported greater left amygdala activation in BPD patients to facial expressions of emotion (vs a fixation point) compared with healthy controls (Donegan et al, 2003).

Taken together, these studies provide support for a model in which the amygdala is linked to emotional processing, but it does not act in isolation; instead functions within a network of brain regions that together modulate the complex manifestations of emotion. This reciprocal interaction predicts that if cortical control of the thalamo-amygdala pathway is reduced, emotional responses will be dysregulated (LeDoux, 1994). Based on this literature, we hypothesized that in BPD, an amygdala uncoupled from the prefrontal regulation might be associated with loss of behavioral control. In addition, the numerous data showing abnormalities in serotonergic function in impulsive aggressive patients with personality disorders led us to examine the affect of a serotonergic agent on differential amygdala connectivity with the prefrontal cortex. We further hypothesized that these changes might be more marked for the ventral than dorsal amygdala. Using 18FDG-PET, we tested the PFC-amygdala balance theory by comparing inter-regional correlations between all 13 ipsilateral prefrontal Brodmann areas and amygdala regions in impulsive aggressive BPD patients compared with healthy controls as measured at rest and after a serotonergic stimulus. We predicted more robust correlations between medial OFC (BA 11, 12, and 47) and specifically ipsilateral ventral amygdala in healthy controls compared to patients. We examined ipsilateral correlations as evidence suggests that the reciprocal PFC–amygdala connections both in non-human primates (Ghashghaei and Barbas, 2002) and in human beings (Di Virgilio et al, 1999) are predominantly ipsilateral. In addition, we examined group differences in amygdala volume and metabolic activity at baseline and after m-CPP. Data on regional metabolism in PFC and cingulate on a subset of patients included in this study (13 IED-BPD; 13 controls) have been published previously (New et al, 2002).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Twenty-six patients (17 men (35.7, SD=7.9 years), nine women (30.7, SD=8.6), range=20–48; 19 (right-handed), four (left-handed), four (mixed)) meeting DSM-IV criteria for BPD and Intermittent Explosive Disorder-modified (IED) as defined by the Module for Intermittent Explosive disorder (Coccaro et al, 1998) were included. Patients with a history of schizophrenia, a psychotic disorder, or bipolar (Type I) affective disorder were excluded. Patients with current major depressive disorder were excluded. Patients with past or current PTSD were accepted into the study, as a relatively high rate of PTSD in community samples of BPD has been reported (Swartz et al, 1990). Three (all male) of the 26 BPD patients met criteria for current PTSD and one female patient met criteria for past PTSD. We studied borderline patients with impulsive aggression to find a more homogeneous group of subjects with severe symptoms; this resulted, however, in our having a higher portion of male subjects than is usually reported in BPD samples. All subjects were medication-free >6 weeks (22/27 never-medicated). Twenty-four age- and sex-matched healthy subjects were also studied (15 men (31.7, SD=7.9 years); nine women (34.0, SD=11.2) range=21–58;19 (right-handed), two (left-handed), three (mixed)). One RH 31-year-old male control was inadequately imaged on the m-CPP day owing to technical difficulties and was used only in baseline analyses. Subjects were screened for severe medical or neurological illness, head injury, or past substance dependence, as well as substance abuse in the prior 6 months. All subjects had a negative urine toxicology screen, and females a negative pregnancy test on each scan day. Participants provided written informed consent in accordance with IRB guidelines. Patients were recruited through advertisement in local newspapers (90%) and referrals from psychiatric clinics at the Bronx VAMC and Mount Sinai (10%). For the 26 subjects recruited into the patient group, 164 subjects were screened. Patients were excluded, in order of frequency for not fully meeting BPD, current substance abuse, medical problems, pregnancy, and/or current major depression. One subject declined participation because of the radioactivity and another declined an intravenous line. In the control group, 121 candidates responded to advertisement and 97 were excluded because of the presence of an Axis I or II diagnosis in themselves or a first-degree relative.

Diagnoses were made through interviews by a psychologist using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (First et al, 1996) and the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (Pfohl et al, 1997), respectively followed by a consensus meeting. Subjects and (when available with the patient's consent) a family member were interviewed. All patients met DSM-IV criteria for BPD, except one subject who met 4/5 criteria needed for a BPD diagnosis by his report and full criteria by family-member report. Trait aggression was assessed using the ‘Module for Intermittent Explosive Disorder-Modified’ (Coccaro et al, 1998). All subjects completed the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI) (Buss and Durkee, 1957), the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS-7b) (Barratt, 1965), the Affective Lability Scale (ALS) (Harvey et al, 1989) and the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (Bernstein and Fink, 1998).

All BPD patients had: significant physical and/or verbal aggression, meeting criteria for IED (κ=0.92). All patients met the ‘impulsiveness’ criterion for BPD (κ=0.78) and 3/27 subjects met the ‘self-damaging’ BPD criterion (κ=0.90). Controls met none of the above-defined criteria. Handedness was determined with the Edinburgh-handedness-scale (Oldfield, 1971). Table 1 shows group means for symptom domains.

Table 1 Clinical Assessments in IED-BPD and Controls

Procedure

On two separate occasions (1–4 weeks apart), each participant received m-CPP or placebo in a double-blind counterbalanced manner. After an overnight fast, an intravenous line was inserted (for blood sampling and injection of m-CPP/placebo/18FDG). 0.08-mg/kg of m-CPP/placebo was given by slow push, immediately followed by 5 mCi of 18FDG. The subject remained resting in a sound-attenuated, dimly-lit room for the 35-min tracer-uptake period. Following uptake, subjects were positioned in the PET scanner for a 45-min data-acquisition period. This method has been described in detail in previous reports (New et al, 2002).

PET scans were carried out as described elsewhere (Haznedar et al, 1997; New et al, 2002) (GE2048 head-dedicated scanner, resolution 4.5 mm in plane, 5.0 mm axially). Fifteen slices at 6.5-mm intervals were obtained in two sets to cover the entire brain. Slice counts of 1.5–3 M counts are typical. Scans were reconstructed with a blank and a transmission scan using the Hanning filter. The same individually molded thermoplastic facemask was used for each scan to minimize head-movement during image acquisition and to assist in PET/MRI coregistration. PET images were obtained in nanocuries/pixel and standardized as rGMR by dividing each pixel by the mean value for the entire brain (defined by brain-edge from coregistered MRI). Although this limits interpretations of single structure absolute activity, this method is widely used when evaluating hypotheses related to patterns of metabolic rate across brain areas and was used in earlier imaging studies of serotonin activation (Mann et al, 1996; New et al, 2002; Siever et al, 1999a; Soloff et al, 2000). PET–MRI coregistration used the algorithm of Woods et al (1993). Brain edges were visually traced on all MRI axial slices with inter-tracer reliability of 0.99 on 10 subjects.

Regions of Interest Approach

We assessed rGMR within BAs by tracing coronal slices based on a digitized brain atlas with 33 coronal slice maps of BAs defined by microscopic examination of an entire postmortem brain, a technique detailed elsewhere (Buchsbaum et al, 2001, 2002; Hazlett et al, 2000; Mitelman et al, 2005). To assess the effect of m-CPP on rGMR, the dependent measure for PET analyses on drug effect was expressed as difference scores (m-CPP-placebo) for rGMR within each BA, calculated by subtracting placebo counts for each region of interest in each subject from the corresponding rGMR from the m-CPP scan.

The amygdala was outlined on coronal MRI sections using previously published methods (Haznedar et al, 2000) (ICC=0.82, area measured on three slices at the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles of anteroposterior distance). Outlining of the amygdala began at its largest extent (approximately the center in the anteroposterior dimension) where clear boundaries between gray matter and surrounding white matter are visible. At this mid-section, the amygdaloid complex is roughly elliptical in shape, and anatomical margins are defined by the cornu ammonis and the white matter of gyrus ambiens in the medial aspect, the cornu inferius of the lateral ventricle in the ventral aspect, the temporal lobe white matter laterally, and the gyrus semilunaris in the dorsal aspect. Using an edge contrast-enhancing technique (gradient filter) (Haznedar et al, 2000), we were able to visualize better the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus and boundaries between the hippocampus and the amygdala. The posterior portions of the amygdaloid complex were outlined by using the ventricular recess, hippocampus, and gyrus semilunaris as reference points (Figure 1). Anteriorly, the amygdaloid complex gray matter is more heterogeneous and hard to identify. We outlined from the midsection forward using gradient filtering and excluded the entorhinal cortex, which may include the inferior amygdala. The outlining ended at the first coronal MRI section on which there was visible white matter between the amygdala, ambiens, and white matter of the entorhinal cortex. This procedure may have omitted the very anterior end of the amygdaloid complex, but it had the advantage of excluding other extraneous structures from our analysis. Following the suggestion of Kim et al (2003) we divided the amygdala into a top and bottom half, based on the vertical distance on the mid-coronal slice and applied the MRI-traced template to resliced and coregistered coronal PET slices. Volume is expressed as absolute volume in mm3 and relative to whole brain volume. The data were assessed for movement artifact by measuring the ratio of the area of the middle PET slice (obtained by a radial edging algorithm which draws the edge at 62% of the maximum value) to the area of the middle MRI slice (obtained by hand-tracing); head-movement during the longer scan would tend to blur the image and enlarge the area of activity. No group difference for movement artifact was detected (normals=0.967±0.031, patients=0.964±0.037, t=0.30).

Figure 1
figure 1

Tracing the amygdala. (a) Coronal MRI: anterior–posterior dimension of amygdala. (b) Sobel-gradient filter to enhance gray/white boundaries. (c) Amygdala outlined.

Statistical Analysis

Pearson's correlation coefficients were employed for frontal BAs and regions of the amygdala with significance level of p<0.05. The Kullback's χ2 test for correlational matrices was employed to test group differences in correlational matrices (Kullback, 1967). The Kullback test provides one single p-value for comparing two correlation matrices each from a separate group of subjects. This avoids the Type I error associated with multiple correlation testing because only one comparison is made. Univariate tests can then be supplied as post hoc tests to locate major sources of matrix differences. Mixed-factorial repeated-measures ANOVAs were employed to examine group differences in amygdala volume and metabolism. In addition, Pearson's correlation coefficients were used to test correlation between amygdala activity and the ALS, BIS, BDHI and CTQ scores. All correlations are reported for p<0.05, two-tailed.

ANOVAs examining amygdala volume and function were conducted both including and excluding the subjects meeting PTSD, as PTSD has been associated with both increased (Protopopescu et al, 2005; Shin et al, 2004), and decreased amygdala activity (Britton et al, 2005); amygdala volume in PTSD has been shown not to be different from controls (Bremner, 2002; Wignall et al, 2004).

RESULTS

Correlations PFC and Amygdala

Placebo condition: correlations PFC and amygdala

The correlations between the ventral amygdala and orbitofrontal BA 11, BA 12, and BA 47 were positive and significant in controls on the right; whereas on the left, they were positive but not significant in BA 11, BA 12, and BA 47 (Table 2a). In contrast, the IED-BPD group showed no significant correlations between right or left ventral amygdala and OFC (BA 11, 12, 47) (Table 2a). In addition to OFC, controls showed positive correlations between subgenual BA 25 and ventral amygdala on the right and between BA 44 and ventral amygdala bilaterally. Controls showed no significant positive correlations between any frontal BAs and dorsal amygdala in either hemisphere. Patients showed no significant positive correlations between frontal BAs and either dorsal or ventral amygdala at rest. In patients, there were significant negative correlations between the dorsal and ventral amygdala and BAs 6, 8, 9, 10, 32, and 46 bilaterally.

Table 2 Correlations Frontal Brodmann Areas and Amygdala: Placebo and m-CPP-Placebo Condition

To test for group differences in the correlation matrices for ventral/dorsal amygdala with frontal BAs for each hemisphere, a Kullback's χ2 test was conducted (Kullback, 1967). Controls differed significantly from patients in correlations between prefrontal BAs and dorsal and ventral amygdala in both hemispheres (right ventral amygdala: df=91, Kullback's χ2=236.853, p<0.0001; right dorsal amygdala: df=91, Kullback's χ2=193.939, p<0.001; left ventral amygdala, df=91, Kullback's χ2=194.290, p<0.001; left dorsal amygdala, df=91, Kullback's χ2=184.355, p<0.001) (see Figure 2). The most striking group differences occurred in correlations between right ventral amygdala and orbital BAs 11, 12, and 47. Figure 3 shows scatter plots of rGMR for correlations between these orbital BAs in the right hemisphere and ventral amygdala.

Figure 2
figure 2

Correlations orbital floor and amygdala: placebo condition, This is a visual map of significant positive and negative as well as non-significant correlations between rGMR in frontal Brodmann areas on the orbital surface of the brain and rGMR in ipsilateral amygdala in healthy subjects on the left and in impulsive aggressive borderline personality disorder subjects on the right. Significant positive correlations at p<0.05 are shown in red and significant negative correlations in blue.

Figure 3
figure 3

Scatterplots right hemisphere OFC/ventral amygdala. Scatterplots of correlations of orbital Brodmann areas and amygdala rGMR for right BA 11, 12, 47 and ventral amygdala illustrating distribution in controls and in IED-BPD.

m-CPP-placebo condition: correlations PFC and amygdala

In response to m-CPP, controls showed significant positive correlations between ventral amygdala and BA 12 and 25 on the left. In addition, controls showed significant positive correlations for m-CPP-placebo rGMR between dorsal amygdala and left BA 11 and 12 and right BA 11 and 25. In controls, the frontal pole correlated negatively with dorsal amygdala in response to m-CPP in right BA 6 and 8 (see Table 2b). Patients, in contrast had positive correlations between dorsal amygdala and dorsolateral PFC BA 44,45 and 46 on the left and between ventral amygdala and dorsolateral PFC in BA 44 and 45. In addition, patients showed a positive correlation between BA 12 and left ventral amygdala and a negative correlation between dorsal amygdala and BA 8 on the left.

Also for m-CPP-placebo, controls differed significantly from patients in correlations between the prefrontal BAs and ipsilateral dorsal and ventral amygdala in both hemispheres (right ventral amygdala: df=91, Kullback's χ2=190.04, p<0.001; right dorsal amygdala: df=91, Kullback's χ2=248.71, p<0.001; left ventral amygdala, df=91, Kullback's χ2=169.10, p<0.001; left dorsal amygdala, df=91, Kullback's χ2=194.40, p<0.001) (see Figure 4).

Figure 4
figure 4

Correlations orbital floor and amygdala: m-CPP- Placebo Condition. This is a visual map of significant positive and negative as well as non-significant correlations between rGMR for m-CPP minus placebo in frontal Brodmann areas on the orbital surface of the brain and rGMR for m-CPP minus placebo in ipsilateral amygdala in healthy subjects on the left and in impulsive aggressive borderline personality disorder subjects on the right. Significant positive correlations at p<0.05 are shown in red and significant negative correlations in blue.

Group Differences in Amygdala Volume and Metabolism

Although our hypothesis in this study was that the coupling of metabolic activity between frontal BAs and ventral amygdala would be disrupted, we also tested simple group differences in amygdala volume and metabolism.

Amygdala volume

Because of evidence for specificity of ventral amygdala in human emotion, we divided the amygdala into dorsal and ventral components. A group (control, patient) × hemisphere (R, L) × region (dorsal, ventral) repeated-measures ANOVA with dorsal and ventral amygdala volume as the dependent variable showed no main effect of group or interaction with group. The same result was found with relative amygdala volume (amygdala volume/whole brain volume). It should be noted that the total amygdala volumes found in our healthy subjects fall well within the range of that found by others. Specifically, we found total amygdala volumes for healthy control subjects (mathematically identical of the sum of the top and bottom for each hemisphere) to be: right hemisphere: 1365±198 mm3 for women, 1397±208 mm3 for men; left hemisphere: 1198±254 mm3 for women, 1365±225 mm3 for men. This is within the range for normal human amygdala volumes found by others ranging from 1050 to 1600 mm3 for the right amygdala and 1140 to 1400 mm3 for the left amygdala (Convit et al, 1999; Driessen et al, 2000; Szabo et al, 2003).

Similarly, when the same analysis was conducted excluding subjects meeting criteria for PTSD, no significant effect of group in relative volume was detected. The groups did not differ in whole brain volume (controls mean 1190, SD=150 mm3, patients 1180, SD=125 mm3, t=0.04, p=NS) (see Table 3).

Table 3 Amygdala Volume

Amygdala metabolism: placebo

A group (control, patient) × hemisphere (R, L) × region (dorsal, ventral) repeated-measures ANOVA with placebo rGMR in the dorsal/ventral amygdala as the dependent variable showed no main effect of group or significant interaction with group. Similarly, there was no interaction involving group for rGMR in the whole amygdala. The same result was found when excluding the subjects with PTSD.

Amygdala metabolism (m-CPP-placebo)

A group (control, patient) × hemisphere (R, L) × region (dorsal,ventral) repeated-measures ANOVA with rGMR m-CPP−placebo difference scores within regions of the amygdala as the dependent variable showed no interaction involving group. The same result was found when excluding the subjects with PTSD.

Correlations of volume/metabolism with clinical symptoms

To examine the relationship between metabolism and symptom dimensions, we examined correlations between volume and metabolism and clinical measures of impulsivity, aggression, and affective instability.

Amygdala volume

We observed a negative correlation between BIS score and right relative ventral amygdala volume in controls (n=22, r=−0.45, p<0.001), but not patients (n=23, r=−0.27, p=NS) (between-group test of correlation differences was not significant). In patients, we found a negative correlation between impulsivity scores and left ventral amygdala volume (n=23, r=−0.42, p<0.004), not present in controls (n=22 r=−0.08, p=NS) (between-group test of correlation differences was not significant). These correlations survive Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons for which a p-value <0.004, two-tailed, is significant.

Amygdala activity

In patients, but not controls, we observed a negative correlation between aggression and affective lability scores for placebo rGMR in right ventral and dorsal amygdala (BDHI: ventral, r=−0.67, dorsal r=0.63; ALS: ventral, r=−0.67, dorsal r=−0.68). For drug-placebo, in controls but not patients, we observed negative correlations between ALS scores and the ventral amygdala (controls r=−0.64) (Bonferroni corrected significance, p<0.002, two-tailed) (between group test of correlation differences was not significant).

DISCUSSION

Implications of Group Differences in Fronto-Amygdala Correlations

The most striking finding that we report is the highly significant normal-BPD group differences in correlation patterns between frontal BA and ipsilateral amygdala metabolic activity (m-CPP minus placebo as well as the resting condition). Healthy controls, both at rest and in response to a serotonergic probe, show positive correlations between OFC (BA 11, 12) and right ventral amygdala, as was predicted from the model of the ventral amygdala as the component of human amygdala most closely associated with frontal lobe emotion modulation (Kim et al, 2003; Somerville et al, 2004; Whalen et al, 1998). This correlational approach is based on the assumption that significant correlations may reveal an important functional relationship between the structures as discussed by Katz et al (1996). Correlations in healthy controls support the idea of intact coupling between PFC, particularly the ventrolateral region (BA 11, 47 and more dorsally in BA 44), and right ventral amygdala, a tight coupling which may be the neural substrate for downregulation of the amygdala in response to aversive stimuli. The absence of such tight coupling in BPD patients, indicated by the lack of significant correlations between OFC and amygdala, suggests a disconnect between OFC and amygdala, which may explain the failure of BPD patients to downregulate the amygdala in response to aversive stimuli. Furthermore, the BPD patients appear to have lost anatomic specificity of ventral vs dorsal amygdala, showing nonsignificant correlations with OFC and modest negative correlations between amygdala and widespread regions of the frontal lobe.

The directionality of correlations between amygdala and PFC is inconsistent in the literature. Our studies are consistent with findings of Pezawas et al, in which rostral areas of anterior cingulate showed positive correlations with amygdala, at least under certain conditions (Kim et al, 2004; Pezawas et al, 2005). However, other studies have demonstrated negative correlations between amygdala and medial OFC (Hariri et al, 2000, 2003). Previous fMRI studies showing negative correlations between amygdala and OFC have employed event-related designs showing acute reactions to aversive stimuli (Dougherty et al, 2004; Shin et al, 2005). Our study, in contrast, employs 18FDG-PET, which reflects an average of activity of a 30-min epoch. As FDG uptake appears to reflect metabolic activity at axon terminals (Sokoloff, 1982), positive correlations would arise between areas that receive input causing firing and its area of efferent connection (Katz et al, 1996). A limitation of the correlational approach using FDG is that it does not discriminate between inhibitory and excitatory connections.

Another reason for inconsistencies in the literature arise from the specific brain regions examined. Findings of a negative correlation between the regions of PFC and amygdala have been found with the rostral anterior cingulate (Pezawas et al, 2005), as well as in ventral regions of PFC (Hariri et al, 2000, 2003). Our finding of positive correlations between particularly lateral OFC and ventral amygdala in healthy subjects (not found in our borderline group) is consistent with regional specificity of the connection between amygdala and PFC shown in primate studies (Carmichael and Price, 1995), and some human studies however (Kim et al, 2004).

The ventral amygdala has been suggested to have greater interconnection with OFC whereas the dorsal amygdala has greater interconnection with the cingulate and other limbic areas (Aggleton and Saunders, 2000). In our data, cingulate areas left 24 and right 25 were both significantly correlated with ventral but not dorsal amygdala, consistent with the general pattern of greater ventral amygdala–cortical links, but not with a distinction between cingulate and OFC connections with the amygdala.

We show no significant group differences in rGMR between BPD patients and controls in the amygdala, which may suggest that the primary abnormality in BPD relates to the failure of the PFC to ‘come on line’ in response to amygdala activation. Group differences in correlations in response to serotonergic stimulation show healthy controls with positive correlations between orbitofrontal areas (BA 11, bilaterally; left BA 12) and dorsal amygdala (Table 2b). This is consistent with evidence that the dorsal amygdala (central nucleus) is rich in neuronal fibers positive for serotonin and serotonin-transporter in primates (Freedman and Shi, 2001). Borderline patients show positive correlations between dorsal and ventral amygdala and dorsolateral BAs.

Implications of Lack of Group Differences in Amygdala Volume and Activity

Consistent with larger studies in the literature employing ‘BRAINS’ to delineate the amygdala, our hand-tracing of amygdala in a large sample show no difference in amygdala volume between BPD and controls. Although some studies have shown group differences, these studies have employed exclusively female subjects, where as we include both men and women and have selected BPD subjects with impulsive aggression. We explored our data to examine whether entering sex into the ANOVA would yield significant results and found no main effect of sex or interaction between sex and diagnosis in amygdala volume.

Clinical correlation with volume show some laterality in that inverse correlations were seen in patients between right ventral amygdala and measures of impulsivity, but in left ventral amygdala in controls. Prior studies of amygdala volume decreases in BPD, however, have shown a bilateral effect. Clinical correlations in amygdala activity revealed inverse correlations with affective instability and aggression with right ventral and dorsal amygdala in BPD, whereas in controls, a negative correlation between right ventral amygdala and affective lability was observed. This supports our evidence that there is a loss of anatomic specificity between the ventral and dorsal amygdala in BPD. There was no relationship between aggression scores and left ventral amygdala volume in controls in part because the variance in aggression measures within the controls group was very limited.

This study has a number of limitations in design and analysis. The lack of group differences in activation in amygdala may relate to the study design, which did not employ a behavioral task. As there was a pharmacological challenge, the interaction with an impulse control task would have required four scans instead of two, and this would have provided logistical and subject volunteerism problems. The responsivity of the amygdala may not be adequately probed with a resting scan or with a pharmacologic challenge. Future studies involving behavioral provocation during functional scanning will elucidate this. In addition, our statistical analysis employed a broad assessment of correlations between all prefrontal Brodmann areas in each hemisphere as well as the ventral and dorsal amygdala. However, as functional imaging studies, present and future, are producing a large number of activation areas from exploratory mapping, the full presentation of the correlation matrix seemed justified to provide findings for other exploratory imaging studies.

Another limitation of the present study is the lack of precision of our top-bottom parcellation of the amygdala. The amygdala ROI has a volume of about 1300 mm3 in our tracings. The full-width half-maximum resolution of our scanner measured at the center of the ring is 4.5 mm in plane and 6.5 mm in the z-axis. This yields a volume of 131 mm3 and the amygdala volume is thus approximately 10 times as great. The mean height of our amygdala tracing on the coronal ROI is 14.3 mm, and encompasses two PET slices. Nevertheless, top and bottom amygdala values are one-half as high, and partial volume effects limit our statistical power to detect differences. In addition, our division into top and bottom half is geometrical and does not weight the lateral basal portion. MRI images provide inadequate resolution to easily trace individual amygdala nuclei, again reducing statistical power.