INTRODUCTION

The two monoamine oxidase (MAO) isoenzymes, A and B, are encoded by different genes closely aligned on the X-chromosome (Xp11. 23) (Lan et al, 1989), with 70% identity in amino-acid sequence (Bach et al, 1988). MAO-A has a high affinity for serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) and norepinephrine (NE), whereas MAO-B prefers β-phenylethylamine as substrate (Shih et al, 1999a). The metabolism of other monoamines, such as dopamine (DA), is contributed by both isoforms in most animal species (Bortolato et al, 2008; Shih and Thompson, 1999b).

Numerous lines of evidence suggest that MAO-A has a critical role in emotional reactivity and social behavior (Brummett et al, 2008; Buckholtz and Meyer-Lindenberg, 2008; Caspi et al, 2002; Jacob et al, 2005; Manuck et al, 2000; Meyer-Lindenberg et al, 2006); in particular, the activity of this enzyme has been found to be inversely correlated with the severity of antisocial traits (Alia-Klein et al, 2008). Notably, total congenital deficiency of MAO-A results in Brunner syndrome, a rare X-linked recessive disorder chacterized by aggressive conduct and criminal acts (such as armed violence, rape, and arson) in the affected males (Brunner et al, 1993). Allelic variants of MAO-A polymorphisms associated to low enzymatic activity are not inherently conducive to aggressive or violent behavior (Alia-Klein et al, 2008; Caspi et al, 2002; Kim-Cohen et al, 2006), but are nevertheless associated with alterations in the processing of emotional and social inputs (Lee and Ham, 2008; Williams et al, 2009), and a significantly higher risk for antisocial personality disorder in male carriers (Buckholtz et al, 2008; Caspi et al, 2002; Meyer-Lindenberg et al, 2006; Oreland et al, 2007).

Broader insight into the phenotypic outcomes of congenital MAO-A deficiency has been afforded by the development and characterization of MAO-A-knockout (KO) mice. These transgenic animals show a number of aberrant phenotypes, including high brain concentrations of 5-HT and NE, and dysmorphic barrel fields. Importantly, male MAO-A KO mice show a number of behavioral disturbances strikingly similar to those observed in Brunner syndrome patients, including marked reactive aggression toward conspecifics and a distinct inability to attune their defensive responses to different contextual cues (Cases et al, 1995; Godar et al, 2010; Scott et al, 2008).

Interestingly, mice treated with prolonged administration of MAO-A inhibitors in adulthood do not develop the same abnormalities observed in MAO-A KO mice, suggesting that the phenotypical aberrations induced by congenital MAO-A deficiency are partially linked to developmental alterations. This concept is also supported by other convergent lines of research, documenting the presence of dysmorphic barrel formations due to the hyper-activation of 5-HT1B receptors during the first 2 weeks of postnatal life (Cases et al, 1995; Salichon et al, 2001).

Although MAO-A KO mice remain a key instrument to understand the role of this enzyme in emotional regulation, the high severity of their behavioral abnormalities restricts their heuristic value as a research tool to study the more subtle endophenotypic characteristics shown by individuals with low MAO-A activity. A viable experimental alternative could be found in rodents injected with MAO-A inhibitors in the perinatal stages, which have been shown to exhibit aggression in adulthood (Mejia et al, 2002); the applicability of this approach, however, is greatly limited by several confounding factors, such as the limited specificity of these agents and the numerous permanent neurobehavioral sequelae of early stress (Teicher et al, 2003).

An attractive strategy to overcome these limitations is the development of hypomorphic mutations of the Maoa gene, resulting in a lower penetrance of the abnormal phenotypes associated with MAO-A deficiency. A highly dependable method for the production of hypomorphic mutant mice is the intronic insertion of a foreign cassette (Barrow and Capecchi, 1996; Lewandoski, 2001; Mohn et al, 1999; Nagy et al, 1998). Here we report the generation of MAO-ANeo mice, a novel line of hypomorphic MAO-A transgenic animals harboring a neomycin-resistance cassette (NeoR) in intron-12 of the Maoa gene. This mutation results in aberrant splicing of Maoa mRNA and a reduction of the functional Maoa transcript. MAO-ANeo mice show low levels of MAO-A enzymatic activity and normal 5-HT concentrations in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala. The present paper describes the initial characterization of the neurochemical, morphological, and behavioral features of this line of mice, in comparison with both wild-type (WT) littermates and MAO-A KO conspecifics.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Generation of MAO-ANeo mice. The MAO-ANeo construct was specifically designed to harbor a floxed neomycin selection cassette (NeoR) in intron-12 of the Maoa gene and a loxP sequence in intron-11. This particular configuration was devised to generate a hypomorphic variant in which either the NeoR cassette or exon-12 (which encodes for the active site of the enzyme) may be removed upon recombination with Cre sequences. The full details of the generation of MAO-ANeo mice are presented under Supplementary Materials and Methods. Briefly, an MAO-ANeo targeting vector was derived from the plasmid pPGKneo-I—containing the NeoR cassette with a phosphoglycerate kinase-1 (PGK1) promoter—and a 9-kb BamHI MAO-A clone (Figure 1a). The MAO-ANeo targeting vector was electroporated into embryonic stem cells as described and incubated in the culture medium with G418 for neomycin-resistance selection. Two independent neomycin-resistant homologous recombinant clones were selected among the embryonic stem cell lines and used to generate two lines of 129S6 mice. The presence of loxP sequences was then ascertained by genomic DNA PCR and the selected line of MAO-ANeo mice was then expanded into a stable colony. Throughout the study MAO-ANeo mice were compared with WT littermates as well as age-matched MAO-A KO mice on 129S6 genetic background. In particular, for MAO-A KO mice, we used MAO-AA863T KO mice, which show a spontaneous point nonsense mutation strikingly similar to that featured in Brunner syndrome (Brunner et al, 1993). Each line was bred with homogenotypic pairs and backcrossed to 129S6 every three generations.

Figure 1
figure 1

Generation and transcript characterization of MAO-ANeo mice. (a) Construction of the MAO-ANeo targeting vector. (1) The 9.0-kb MAO-A BamHI fragment composed of a 6.7-kb BamHI–BglII and a 2.0-kb EcoRI–BamHI fragment. Unique SphI and EcoRI sites, within intron-11 and 12, respectively, were used for the insertion of loxP and NeoR cassettes. (2) The 0.28-kb SmaI–loxP–EcoRI fragment was embedded into the SphI site of a 6.7-kb BamHI–BglII fragment. (3) A 1.6-kb EcoRI floxed NeoR cassette was directionally ligated in the 5′-direction to a 2.0-kb EcoRI fragment containing exon-13. (4) A 5.7-kb EcoRI–BamHI fragment containing exon-11–loxP–exon-12 was cloned 5′ to the 3.6-kb floxed Neo-exon-13 fragment to complete the targeting vector. (b) Sequence of 210-bp PCR product and its deduced amino-acid sequence. (c) Quantification of WT Maoa mRNA levels in brain regions of MAO-ANeo mice. The values are represented as the mean±SEM. **P<0.01 and ***P<0.001 compared with WT mice. Abbreviations: B, BamHI; Bg, BglII; E, EcoRI; S, SmaI; Sp, SphI; X, XhoI; PFC, prefrontal cortex; AMY, amygdala; HIP, hippocampus; STR, striatum; MID, midbrain; CER, cerebellum.

PowerPoint slide

Genotyping. MAO-ANeo mice were genotyped by PCR using genomic DNA isolated from tail at postnatal day 21, using the primers In11F2 and In11R1 (Supplementary Table). The PCR conditions are detailed under Supplementary Materials and Methods. MAO-A KO mice were genotyped as described previously (Scott et al, 2008).

Cloning of PCR Maoa-Neo chimeric transcripts. PCR products were cloned into the pCR4-TOPO vector (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA), according to the manufacturer's instructions. The plasmids containing the insert were selected and their sequences were verified.

Husbandry. Only adult male mice (2–3 months old; 25–35 g of body weight) were tested throughout the study. The mice were housed in groups (four per cage), with the exception of those used for resident–intruder aggression testing, which were isolated for 2 weeks prior to the behavioral experiment. Food and water were available ad libitum in a housing facility maintained at approximately 22 °C and with a 12-h:12-h light:dark cycle, with lights off at 1800 hours. All procedures used in the present study were in compliance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines, and the protocols were approved by the University of Southern California Animal Use Committee.

The null-allele controls used throughout the study were MAO-AA863T KO mice, a line of MAO-A KO mutants on a background congenic with MAO-ANeo mice (129S6); these mice show a spontaneous point nonsense mutation strikingly similar to that featured in Brunner syndrome (Brunner et al, 1993). MAO-ANeo and age-matched MAO-A KO mice were compared with WT littermates. To minimize litter effects, mice from at least three different litters were used in each test.

Molecular, Neurochemical, and Morphological Analyses

Brain tissue processing. For biochemical analyses, adult male mice were anesthetized with halothane and promptly decapitated. Brains were removed within approximately 30 s after decapitation and select regions were dissected by using a stereotactic atlas (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001) as a guide. The following regions were taken: Prefrontal cortex (1.60–2.50 mm rostral to bregma, comprehensive of medial, orbital, and dorsolateral subdivisions); amygdala (1.20–2.80 mm caudal to bregma); hippocampus (2.00–4.00 mm caudal to bregma); striatum (1.40–0.10 mm rostral to bregma); and midbrain (4.00–6.00 mm caudal to bregma).

Quantitative real-time RT-PCR of Maoa and Maoa-NeoR mRNA. The total RNA was isolated by using the Trizol reagent (Invitrogen). Five micrograms of total RNA were reverse-transcribed into first-strand cDNA by random primers by using SuperScript® III reverse transcriptase (Invitrogen). For the analysis of WT Maoa mRNA, the primers E12F and E14R were used. For the analysis of Maoa-NeoR mRNA, the primers E11F and NeoR1 were used. The primer sequences are presented in the Supplementary Table. To account for differences in starting material, the 18S rRNA was used in separate tubes in triplicate and the average threshold cycle (CT) for the triplicate was used in all subsequent calculations. The PCR was run under the following conditions: 95 °C for 3 min, followed by 35 cycles of PCR consisting of 30 s at 94 °C, 30 s at 56 °C, and 30 s at 72 °C. The relative differences in WT Maoa mRNA concentrations between WT and MAO-ANeo mice were determined by using the ΔΔCT method and presented as the percent of input (taken as 100%). Data were obtained from samples of three independent experiments.

MAO-A catalytic activity assay. Brain removal was performed 40 min after intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of either clorgyline (Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO; dissolved in 0.9%. saline solution) (10 mg/kg; injection volume: 10 ml/kg) or saline. MAO-A activity was determined as described previously (Chen et al, 2007; Grimsby et al, 1997), by incubation with radiolabeled 5-HT.

HPLC determination of monoamine levels. Brain samples were homogenized in a solution containing 0.1 M trichloroacetic acid, 10 mM sodium acetate, and 0.1 mM EDTA. Isoproterenol at 1 μM was used as an internal standard. The homogenates were sonicated and centrifuged, and the supernatants were used for high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) analysis. 5-HT, NE, DA, 5-HIAA, and DOPAC (Sigma-Aldrich) were used as standards. The protein concentrations were determined from the pellet, by using a bicinchoninic acid kit (Pierce, Rockford, IL) following the manufacturer's protocol. The mobile phase was the same as the homogenization buffer (excluding the isoproterenol), with 7% methanol for detection of 5-HT and 5-HIAA. NE, DA, and DOPAC were quantified separately by using a trichloroacetic acid mobile phase solution without methanol. The mobile phases were filtered and deaerated, and the pump speed (Shimadzu LC-6A liquid chromatograph) was 1.5 ml/min. The reverse-phase column used was a Rexchrom S50100-ODS C18 column with a length of 25 cm and an internal diameter of 4.6 mm (Regis, Morton Grove, IL). The compounds were measured at +0.7 V by using a Shimadzu L-ECD-6A electrochemical detector.

Golgi histology and dendritic analyses. Histological analyses were performed as described previously (Garrett and Wellman, 2009; Wellman et al, 2007) and are detailed under Supplementary Materials and Methods. Briefly, male adult (3-month-old) mice (n=20) were overdosed with urethane and transcardially perfused with saline. Brains were removed and processed for Golgi histology by using the Glaser and Van der Loos' modified procedure (Glaser and Van der Loos, 1981). Within the ventral and lateral orbitofrontal cortex, and the basolateral amygdala, of each mouse, 6–12 neurons meeting the criteria for reconstruction (see Supplementary Materials and Methods) were randomly selected and reconstructed.

The morphology of apical and basilar arbors was quantified in three dimensions by using a computer-based neuron tracing system (Neurolucida; MBF Bioscience, Williston, VT), with the experimenter blind to genotype. Differences in the amount and location of dendritic material were also quantified through a three-dimensional version of a Sholl analysis (Sholl, 1956). The dendritic lengths within concentric spheres at 10-μm radii were quantified; for statistical and graphical purposes, the lengths were summed over pairs of radii.

Behavioral Testing

Prior to characterization of emotional behaviors, MAO-ANeo mice were tested for physical features (general appearance, body weight, body temperature, posture and gait, heart rate, breathing frequency) and neurological reflexes (righting reflex, postural reflex, eye-blink reflex, and whisker-orienting reflex) as indicated previously (Crawley, 2000). Visual, olfactory, and acoustic acuity were tested with the visual cliff, buried food, and acoustic startle reflex paradigms, as indicated previously (Bortolato et al, 2007; Godar et al, 2010). Male mice were tested in the following behavioral paradigms: Open field (n=44), elevated plus-maze (n=30), light–dark box (n=33), marble burying (n=43), water mist-induced grooming (n=20), social interaction (n=39), and resident–intruder aggression (n=30). Numbers within each experimental group were defined based on power analyses conducted on preliminary investigations on each paradigm. All tests were conducted between 0900 and 1500 hours. The methodological details for each paradigm are reported under Supplementary Materials and Methods. Each animal was used only once to avoid stress carry-over effects. Light and background noise levels in the experimental room were kept at 10 lux and 70 dB for all the tests (with the exception of light–dark box, for which light in the white compartment was kept at 200 lux).

Data Analysis

Throughout the study, data sets referring to WT littermates of MAO-ANeo and age-matching MAO-A KO were combined, in view of the congenicity of the two backgrounds and the statistical equivalence between the average values and variances of each subgroup. Normality and homoscedasticity of data distribution were verified by using Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Bartlett's tests. Parametric analyses were performed by one- or two-way ANOVAs, as appropriate, followed by Tukey's test (with Spjøtvoll–Stoline correction) for post-hoc comparisons. Non-parametric analyses were performed by Kruskal–Wallis test, followed by Nemenyi's test for post-hoc testing. Correlation analyses were performed by linear regression. Contingency analyses of categorical variables (ie, occurrence of anomalous behaviors within each genotype) were performed by χ2-test. Significance threshold was set at 0.05.

RESULTS

Generation and quantification of mRNA. In all brain regions of MAO-ANeo mice, the presence of NeoR cassette was ascertained by RT-PCR as a 210-bp product, which was conversely absent in the WT littermates (data not shown). Sequencing analysis revealed a chimeric Maoa-NeoR transcript featuring the intact sequences of exons 11 and 12, followed by the PGK1 promoter of the NeoR cassette. This aberrantly spliced form predicts a C-terminal truncated protein with a tail of 10 amino acids encoded by sequences present in the PGK1 promoter (Figure 1b). This chimeric protein is likely to have reduced or null MAO-A activity, as truncation of its C-terminus prevents anchoring to the mitochondrial outer membrane (De Colibus et al, 2005; Ma et al, 2004). Interestingly, all brain regions of MAO-ANeo mice featured significant reductions of WT Maoa mRNA in comparison with the WT littermates (Figure 1c).

MAO-A enzymatic activity. In preliminary experiments, the available antibodies for mouse MAO-A showed very low selectivity for this target in both immunohistochemical and immunoblotting studies. To test the functional changes in brain MAO-A, we measured its enzymatic activity levels throughout different regions of MAO-ANeo mice.

While MAO-A KO mice showed no MAO-A catalytic activity in any brain region, the MAO-ANeo counterparts showed a low, yet detectable activity in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala, which were ablated by treatment with the selective MAO-A inhibitor clorgyline (10 mg/kg, i.p., 40 min before killing) (Figure 2a and b). Conversely, in every other brain region of MAO-ANeo mice, MAO-A activity was below the detectability threshold (Figure 2c–e).

Figure 2
figure 2

Brain-regional levels of MAO-A activity, 5-HT and NE in adult WT, MAO-ANeo, and MAO-A KO mice. (a, b) MAO-ANeo mice display higher brain MAO-A activity levels than MAO-A KO mice in prefrontal cortex and amygdala, but lower than WT controls. The low levels of MAO-A were attenuated by teatment with the MAO-A inhibitor clorgyline (10 mg/kg, i.p.) (c–e) Conversely, MAO-ANeo mice showed comparable MAO-A activity in other brain regions tested. (f, g) MAO-ANeo mice exhibit 5-HT levels comparable to WT mice in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Although similar 5-HT levels were detected in the (h) striatum in all genotypes, both MAO-A mutants exhibited higher levels of 5-HT in the (i) hippocampus and (j) midbrain. (k–o) NE levels were higher in MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice than in WT mice in all brain regions. MAO-A KO mice display higher NE levels than do MAO-ANeo mice in the amygdala. Values are represented as mean±SEM. *P<0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001 in comparison with WT mice and #P<0.05 and ###P<0.001 in comparison with MAO-ANeo mice. 000P<0.001 compared with saline treatment. AMY, amygdala; CLO, clorgyline; HIP, hippocampus; 5-HT, serotonin; MID, midbrain; NE, norepinephrine; PFC, prefrontal cortex; SAL, saline; STR, striatum.

PowerPoint slide

To verify whether the specific alterations in MAO-ANeo mice were due to the presence of NeoR cassette, we compared the levels of enzymatic activity of MAO-ANeo mice with those of its recombinant offspring lines, obtained by their crossing with BALB/c CMV-Cre mice (which ubiquitously express Cre recombinase). Two lines of recombinant mice were generated (Supplementary Figure):

  1. 1)

    MAO-AΔ12 KO mice, resulting from the recombination of the loxP sequence in intron-11 with the downstream loxP sequence of the NeoR cassette; this line featured the loss of both the exon-12 of the Maoa gene and the NeoR cassette, and did not show MAO-A activity in any brain region (data not shown);

  2. 2)

    MAO-Aflox mice, in which Cre-mediated recombination of two Neo-flanking loxP sequences resulted in the loss of the selection cassette; this line showed levels of catalytic activity similar to those in WT mice in all tissues (data not shown), thereby attesting that the reduced MAO-A activity in the brain of MAO-ANeo mice was due to the presence of the NeoR cassette.

As different strains have been shown to extensively influence monoamine levels and emotional responses in rodents (Maas, 1962; Schneider et al, 1992; Trullas and Skolnick, 1993), MAO-Aflox and MAO-AΔ12 KO mice (which carried a mixed 129S6-BALB/c background) could not be used as proper controls for neurochemical and behavioral studies on MAO-ANeo mice.

HPLC analysis. Comparisons of 5-HT levels across genotypes revealed a complex set of region-specific differences. In the prefrontal cortex and amygdala of MAO-ANeo mice, 5-HT levels were equivalent to those shown by WT mice, but significantly lower (P<0.001, ANOVA; Tukey's test) than the MAO-A KO counterparts (Figure 2f and g). Conversely, striatal 5-HT levels appeared equivalent in all genotypes (Figure 2h). Finally, both MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice showed significantly higher 5-HT levels in the hippocampus (Figure 2i) (P<0.01, ANOVA) and midbrain (Figure 2j) (P<0.01, ANOVA) as compared with those in the WT mice. The content of NE in MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice was significantly higher than in the WT controls across all regions (Figure 2k–o). However, the NE levels in the amygdala of MAO-ANeo mice were significantly lower (Figure 2l) (P<0.05; ANOVA) than those observed in MAO-A KO mice. DA levels were equivalent across all regions in the three lines (data not shown). The levels of 5-HT, NE, and DA metabolites (5-HIAA and DOPAC) were significantly reduced in MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice as compared with those in the WT controls (data not shown).

Dendritic analyses. In all treatment groups, complete impregnation (defined as the absence of dendrites trailing off into a series of dots; see Buell, 1982; Coleman and Flood, 1987; Williams et al, 1978) of numerous pyramidal neurons was apparent (Figures 3a and 4a) and both orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala were readily identifiable (Figures 3b and 4b). In the orbitofrontal cortex, total apical branch number and length varied significantly with genotype (Figure 3c and d; F(2, 17)=11.10 and 6.29, respectively, P<0.05). Post-hoc comparisons revealed that apical branch number was increased by 36% and 33% in MAO-ANeo and KO mice, respectively, relative to WT mice (Figure 3d; P<0.05). This increase in branch number was paralleled by 29% and 38% increases in branch length (Figure 3d; P<0.05). Analysis of the amount and distribution of apical dendritic material (Sholl analysis) indicated that this increase was differentially distributed across the apical arbor (Figure 3e; main effect of genotype, F(2, 17)=5.70, P<0.05; for genotype × radius interaction, F(14, 119)=6.98, P<0.05). Post-hoc tests indicated that the increased dendritic material was differentially distributed in MAO-ANeo and KO mice: MAO-ANeo mice showed increases in dendritic material proximal to the soma (MAO-ANeo vs WT, P<0.05 for radii ⩽60 μm from the soma; Figure 3e), whereas for KO mice, the increase in dendritic material was more pronounced in the distal apical arbor (MAO-A KO vs WT, P<0.05 for radii ⩾70 μm; MAO-A KO vs MAO-ANeo, P<0.05 for radii ⩽60 μm and ⩾130 μm from the soma) (Figure 3e).

Figure 3
figure 3

MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice show a robust increase in dendritic arborization in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). (a) Digital light micrograph of Golgi-stained neurons in the OFC of a WT mouse. Scale bar=25 μm. (b) Schematic diagrams of coronal sections through the mouse prefrontal cortex. The coordinates indicate position relative to bregma (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001). Neurons were sampled from the portions of OFC (shaded areas). (c) Computer-assisted reconstructions of Golgi-stained neurons in the OFC of WT, MAO-ANeo, and KO mice. Scale bar=50 μm. (d) The total number and length of apical branches were increased in MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice relative to the WT controls. (e) Both mutant lines showed significant increases in apical dendritic material. (f) Significant increases in basilar branch number and length were detected in MAO-ANeo mice relative to WT, whereas MAO-A KO mice showed decreased number and length of basilar branches. (g) Similarly, MAO-ANeo mice showed increases in basilar dendritic material compared with both WT and MAO-A KO counterparts. The values are represented as the mean±SEM. *P<0.05 in comparison with WT mice; #P<0.05 in comparison with MAO-ANeo mice.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 4
figure 4

MAO-ANeo mice show a subtle reduction in dendritic material in the basolateral amygdala (BLA). (a) Computer-assisted reconstructions of Golgi-stained neurons in the BLA of WT, MAO-ANeo, and MAO-A KO mice. Scale bar=25 μm. (b) Schematic diagrams of coronal sections through the mouse amygdala. The coordinates indicate position relative to bregma (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001). Neurons were sampled from the portions of BLA (shaded areas). (c) Computer-assisted reconstructions of Golgi-stained neurons in the OFC of WT, MAO-ANeo, and MAO-A KO mice. Scale bar=50 μm. (d) The total number and length of dendritic branches were not significantly different in MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice relative to their WT counterparts. (e) In MAO-ANeo mice, decreases in dendritic length were subtle and restricted to the most distal branches, whereas MAO-A KO mice showed larger decreases in both proximal and distal dendritic length. The values are represented as the mean±SEM. *P<0.05 compared with WT mice and #P<.05 and ##P<0.01 compared with MAO-ANeo mice.

PowerPoint slide

MAO-ANeo and KO mice showed contrasting patterns of changes in basilar branch length and number (Figure 3f; F(2, 17)=21.01 and 8.55, respectively, P<0.05). Post-hoc analyses indicated that in MAO-ANeo mice basilar branch number was increased by 26% and 36% relative to WT and MAO-A KO mice, respectively (Figure 3f; P-values <0.05). Basilar branch length showed a similar increase (20% and 34%) relative to WT and MAO-A KO mice, respectively (P-values <0.05). By contrast, MAO-A KO mice showed significant 19% and 21% decreases in basilar branch number and length relative to WT mice (Figure 3f; P-values <0.05).

Sholl analyses indicated that the magnitude of these differences varied across the basilar arbor (Figure 3g; for main effect of genotype, F(2, 17)=5.70, P<0.01; for genotype × radius interaction, F(14, 119)=6.98, P<0.01). MAO-ANeo mice showed increased dendritic material relative to WT mice at 30–40 and 50–60 μm from the soma (Figure 3g; P-values <0.05), whereas KO mice showed significant decreases relative to WT at 50–60 μm and relative to MAO-ANeo at 10–80 μm from the soma (Figure 3g; P-values <0.05).

On the other hand, for basolateral amygdala pyramidal cells, overall branch number and length did not differ significantly across genotypes (Figure 4c and d; F(2, 17)=1.24 and 1.93, respectively, both NS). However, Sholl analysis revealed more subtle alterations in the distribution of dendritic material (Figure 4e; for main effect of genotype, F(2, 17)=3.28, NS; for radius × genotype interaction, F(10, 85)=2.76, P<0.01). Follow-up comparisons showed a very subtle decrease in dendritic material in MAO-ANeo mice relative to WT in the most distal arbor (radii >100 μm from the soma, P<0.01). Decreased dendritic material was more pronounced in MAO-A KO mice, with significant decreases relative to WT at 30–40 and >100 μm from the soma (P<0.05), and decreases approaching significance at 50–60 μm from the soma (P<0.06).

Physical and neurological characteristics and sensory abilities. Similarly to WT and MAO-A KO mice, MAO-ANeo mice did not show any overt alterations in body weight and temperature; likewise, no patent abnormalities of gait, posture, and reflexes were observed. In line with previous findings on MAO-A KO mice (Godar et al, 2010), MAO-ANeo mice did not show any overt changes in visual and olfactory sensitivity, as assessed by the visual cliff and the buried food tests, respectively (data not shown). Acoustic startle analysis revealed a significant reduction of the magnitude of this reflex in both MAO-ANeo and KO mice (WT: 1210.13±82.73 arbitrary units; MAO-ANeo: 894.33±84.39 arbitrary units; MAO-A KO: 851.72±99.58 arbitrary units; F(2, 25)=5.29, P<0.05, ANOVA; P<0.05 for MAO-ANeo vs WT and MAO-A KO vs WT comparisons).

Open field. The behavioral responses of WT, MAO-ANeo, and MAO-A KO mice in a novel open field were analyzed (Figure 5a). Both MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice showed significantly lower locomotor activity than their WT counterparts (Figure 5b) (F(2, 37)=21.7, P<0.001) throughout the 3 days of testing; post-hoc analyses also revealed that the MAO-A KO mice travelled a significantly longer distance than their MAO-ANeo counterparts (P<0.05); these differences were observed on days 2 and 3, but not on day 1. (Figure 5b) In addition, the MAO-ANeo mice showed marked reductions in duration (Figure 5c; F(2, 41)=7.29, P<0.01), relative locomotor activity (Figure 5d; F(2, 41)=8.46, P<0.001), (Figure 5d) and entries (data not shown; P<0.001) in the central quadrant of the arena. No significant genotype × time (day) interactions were found in the analyses of any parameter. Interestingly, linear regression analyses found significant correlations between the distance travelled and the duration of time spent in the center only for WT mice (r2=0.37, P<0.01) but not the MAO-ANeo (r2=0.01; NS) and MAO-A KO counterparts (r2=0.04; NS) (data not shown), ruling out the possibility that the changes in locomotor trajectory observed in MAO-ANeo animals may be an artifactual ‘floor effect’ due to their hypolocomotion.

Figure 5
figure 5

Open-field behavior of MAO-ANeo mice in comparison with WT and MAO-A KO counterparts. (a) Typical locomotor pathways of WT, MAO-ANeo, and MAO-A KO mice in the open field. (b–d) MAO-ANeo mice show significant reductions in (b) total distance, (c) time spent in the central zone, and (d) percent locomotor activity in the central zone. The values are represented as the mean±SEM. **P<0.01 and ***P<0.001 compared with WT mice, and #P<0.05 and ##P<0.01 compared with MAO-ANeo mice.

PowerPoint slide

Elevated plus-maze. In the elevated plus-maze, all genotypes showed similar percent entries and duration in the open and closed arms of the apparatus (Figure 6a–e). A significant effect was found in the analysis of the time spent in the central platform (H(2)=6.99, P<0.05), which was revealed to depend on the higher duration shown by the MAO-A KO mice in comparison with MAO-ANeo (P<0.05) (Figure 6f). No differences in total entries were detected (Figure 6g). Analyses also revealed that both the MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice engaged in fewer stretch-attend postures (Figure 6h) (H(2)=17.97, P<0.001; P<0.05 and P<0.001 for WT vs MAO-ANeo and WT vs MAO-A KO comparisons, respectively), and the MAO-ANeo mice enacted a significantly higher number of exploratory head dips than MAO-A KO mice (Figure 6i) (H(2)=8.64, P<0.05), but not their WT counterparts.

Figure 6
figure 6

Behavioral responses of MAO-ANeo mice in the (a–i) elevated plus-maze and (j–l) light–dark box paradigms. (a–c) No significant differences were detected among genotypes in the percent entries in (a) open arms, (b) closed arms, and (c) center of an elevated plus-maze. (d–e) Similarly, no significant differences were found in the time spent in the (d) open arms and (e) closed arms of the apparatus. (f) MAO-ANeo mice show a significantly lower duration on the central platform than MAO-A KO but not WT mice. (g) MAO-ANeo mice showed an equivalent number of total entries as compared with WT and MAO-A KO mice. (h) Conversely, both MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice showed a reduction in stretch-attend postures (SAPs). (i) MAO-A KO mice engaged in significantly fewer head dips (HDs) than their WT and MAO-ANeo counterparts. (j–l) In the light–dark box, all genotypes showed similar behavioral responses in the (j) time spent in the light compartment, (k) number of transitions between compartments, and (l) latency to enter the dark compartment. The values are represented as the mean±SEM. *P<0.05 and ***P<0.001 compared with WT mice, and #P<0.05 compared with MAO-ANeo mice.

PowerPoint slide

Light–dark box. In the light–dark box, no significant differences among genotypes were found in either the time spent in the brightly-lit compartment (Figure 6j; F(2, 21)=0.47; NS) or the number of inter-compartment transitions, although this last effect approached statistical significance (Figure 6k; F(2, 20)=0.08, P<0.10). The latencies to the first entry in the protected compartment were also statistically equivalent (Figure 6l; H(2)=3.14, NS).

Marble-burying test. We then investigated the performance of MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice in the marble-burying task, a well-validated paradigm to capture the expression of repetitive behaviors in response to mild stressors. Interestingly, both MAO-A-deficient and MAO-ANeo mice showed enhancements in marble-burying activity, which were statistically significant in MAO-ANeo mice (Figure 7a) (F(2, 40)=10.89, P<0.001; P<0.001 and P<0.10 for WT vs MAO-ANeo and WT vs MAO-A KO comparisons, respectively). Similarly, MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO animals showed significant increases in digging duration (Figure 7b) (F(2, 40)=8.64, P<0.001; P<0.001 and P<0.05 for WT vs MAO-ANeo and WT vs MAO-A KO comparisons, respectively) as compared with WT mice. Notably, the reaction of MAO-A KO mice toward the marbles was characterized by frequent tail-rattling and marble-biting episodes; these responses were never observed in either the WT littermates or the MAO-ANeo counterparts (P<0.01 for each behavior; χ2-test).

Figure 7
figure 7

MAO-ANeo mice showed perseverative responses. (a) MAO-ANeo mice engaged in higher marble-burying activity than WT mice. (b) Both MAO mutants showed an increased digging behavior compared with WT controls. (c) In the water mist-induced grooming task, water mist elicited more frequent grooming bouts in MAO-ANeo mice than their WT and MAO-A KO counterparts. (d) MAO-ANeo mice also spent more time grooming than WT but not MAO-A KO mice. The values are represented as the mean±SEM. *P<0.05, **P<0.01, and ***P<0.001 compared with WT mice, and #P<0.05 compared with MAO-ANeo mice.

PowerPoint slide

Water mist-induced grooming. Baseline grooming activity was similar across the three genotypes (data not shown). However, significant effects were observed for both the number of bouts (Figure 7c; F(2, 17)=8.10, P<0.01) and total duration (Figure 7d; F(2, 17)=5.52, P<0.05) of water mist-induced grooming activity. Post-hoc analyses showed that the MAO-ANeo animals engaged in significantly more grooming bouts than both the WT littermates (P<0.01) and the MAO-A KO conspecifics (P<0.05) (Figure 7c). In addition, the MAO-ANeo animals showed significant increases in the duration of water mist-induced grooming response (P<0.05), whereas a statistical trend (P<0.10) was found in the comparison between the WT and MAO-A KO mice (Figure 7d).

Social interaction. In comparison with WT mice, both MAO-ANeo and KO animals showed significant reductions in the total duration (Figure 8a; F(2, 36)=4.72, P<0.05; P-values <0.05 for post-hoc comparisons with WT) and the number (Figure 8b; F(2, 36)=7.50, P<0.01; P<0.05 for MAO-ANeo vs WT, and P<0.01 for MAO-A KO vs WT) of social approaches toward age- and weight-matched, foreign, male, WT conspecifics in an unfamiliar arena. Both the latency to the initial social approach (Figure 8c; H(2)=0.59, NS) and locomotor activity (data not shown) were statistically equivalent across all genotypes.

Figure 8
figure 8

Social behaviors of MAO-ANeo male mice. (a–c) In the social interaction test, both MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice showed a significant reduction in (a) the duration and (b) the number of social approaches as compared with their WT counterparts; however, no differences were detected in the (c) latency to the first social approach. (d–f) In the resident–intruder paradigm, male MAO-ANeo and WT mice showed a marked reduction in (d) fighting duration and (e) frequency compared with MAO-A KO mice; similarly, WT and MAO-ANeo mice showed (f) a significantly longer latency to attack foreign WT intruders in comparison with MAO-A KO conspecifics. The values are represented as the mean±SEM. *P<0.05, **P<0.01, and ***P<0.001 compared with WT mice, and #P<0.05 and ##P<0.01 compared with MAO-ANeo mice.

PowerPoint slide

Resident–intruder aggression. The analysis of aggressive responses in the resident–intruder task showed that, in comparison with both WT and MAO-ANeo male resident mice, the MAO-A KO counterparts showed significant increases in the total fighting duration (Figure 8d; F(2, 29)=8.50, P<0.01; P<0.01 and P<0.05 for WT vs MAO-A KO and MAO-ANeo vs MAO-A KO, respectively) and the number of fighting approaches (Figure 8e; F(2, 29)=7.77, P<0.01; P<0.01 and P<0.05 for WT vs MAO-A KO and MAO-ANeo vs MAO-A KO, respectively); furthermore, the latency to the first aggressive approach was significantly reduced in the MAO-A KO mice in comparison with the other genotypes (Figure 8f; H(2)=16.75, P<0.001; P<0.001 and P<0.01 for WT vs MAO-A KO and MAO-ANeo vs MAO-A KO, respectively). By contrast, no significant difference in any of the above parameters was found between resident WT and MAO-ANeo littermates against age-matched, WT intruders. Similarly to the findings in the social interaction paradigm, motor activity was equivalent across all genotypes (data not shown).

DISCUSSION

In the present report, we have documented the generation and characterization of MAO-ANeo mice, a novel line of MAO-A hypomorphic mutants harboring a NeoR cassette in intron-12 of the Maoa gene, with a neurobiological and behavioral profile distinct from both WT and MAO-A KO conspecifics, encompassing corticolimbic dysmorphology, perseverative behaviors, and social deficits, but no significant enhancement in resident–intruder aggression.

In agreement with previous evidence (Barrow and Capecchi, 1996; Lewandoski, 2001; Nagy et al, 1998), we found that the retention of a foreign NeoR cassette in intron-12 of the Maoa gene resulted in alterations of mRNA splicing and a pronounced reduction in the amount of functional transcript across all brain regions. In particular, our sequencing analyses indicate that the PGK1 promoter sequence in the NeoR cassette may interact with the spliceosome and interfere with the recognition of the correct intervening sequence between exon-12 and intron-12 of the Maoa gene. These post-transcriptional alterations are predicted to result in a non-functional chimeric MAO-A variant, unable to anchor to the mitochondrial outer membrane owing to truncation of its C-terminus (De Colibus et al, 2005; Ma et al, 2004).

Notably, we found small amounts of WT Maoa transcript in all brain regions of MAO-ANeo mice, indicating that aberrant splicing of the Maoa gene does not always occur; however, MAO-A enzymatic activity (presumably arisen from the protein product of WT Maoa mRNA) was only detected in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex, suggesting that the levels of functional enzyme in the other brain regions likely remained below the detection threshold of our assay. Our findings cannot currently elucidate the reasons for the apparent mismatch between the levels of MAO-A transcript and enzymatic activity across different brain areas; nevertheless, the observed region-specific variability may point to differences in the mechanisms of splicing and post-transcriptional processing. Notably, these processes are influenced by neurochemical and/or contextual factors, such as stress (Battaglia and Ogliari, 2005; Gattoni et al, 1996; Lai and McCobb, 2006; Meerson et al, 2010; Meshorer et al, 2005); this last observation may point to a potential explanation for the observed phenomena, in view of the role of environmental stress in the functional modulation of the amygdala and prefrontal cortex.

The lack of MAO-A activity in the hippocampus and midbrain of MAO-ANeo mice resulted in a significant elevation of 5-HT and NE levels, in a manner similar to that in MAO-A KO mice. In contrast with our previous findings on the C3H line of MAO-A KO mice (Tg8) (Chen et al, 2007), we did not detect any significant variation in 5-HT levels in the striatum of either MAO-ANeo or MAO-A KO mice in the 129S6 background; this variation may be related to the abundant MAO-B activity in the striatum of this strain (Bortolato et al, 2008), which may be able to offset some of the neurochemical outcomes of MAO-A deficiency.

The presence of modest amounts of functional MAO-A enzymes in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex of MAO-ANeo mice was sufficient to maintain 5-HT content at levels similar to those observed in their WT littermates and significantly lower than MAO-A KO conspecifics. These findings are in line with previous evidence showing that only relatively small amounts of MAO-A are required to sustain the homeostasis of 5-HT neurotransmission in the brain (Chen et al, 2007). Of note, NE levels were significantly higher than those observed in WT mice in both regions (although the amygdalar content of this monoamine was still significantly lower than its MAO-A KO counterpart). The impact of small amounts of MAO-A on NE is likely more limited than that on 5-HT, in view of the much lower affinity (Strolin Benedetti et al, 1983), as well as the important role of catecholamine-O-methyl-transferase in NE degradation.

MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice showed distinct patterns of morphological alterations in the basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in comparison with the WT controls. Specifically, whereas both MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice showed increased number and length of apical branches in the orbitofrontal cortex, the distribution of this phenomenon was more prominent in the proximal portions of the dendritic tree in MAO-ANeo mice; conversely, MAO-A KO mice showed a significant enhancement in apical dendritic arborizations in distal locations. Furthermore, whereas basilar dendrites were hypertrophic in MAO-ANeo mice, they featured significant reductions in branch number and length in the MAO-A KO animals. Finally, MAO-A KO mice showed subtle decreases in dendritic material in basolateral amygdala, which were not observed in MAO-ANeo mice.

The regulation of dendritic growth is a highly complex phenomenon that involves a host of diverse factors, including neurotrophins, GTPase signaling molecules, and neurotransmitters (Dijkhuizen and Ghosh, 2005; Gallo and Letourneau, 1998; Kwon and Sabatini, 2011; Lieske et al, 1999). Our results cannot help substantiate the neurobiological underpinnings of the dendritic changes in the pyramidal neurons of MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice; however, the observed differential patterns of dendritic arborization in these two genotypes may reflect divergent remodeling processes, possibly in response to distinct alterations in neurochemical inputs (including 5-HT and NE) from other brain regions. Interestingly, alterations in 5-HT signaling may contribute to the abnormalities observed in MAO-A KO mice, in view of the important role of this monoamine in the determination of neurite growth and complexity, as well as in the modulation of pruning (Durig and Hornung, 2000; Lieske et al, 1999).

The analysis of the behavioral characteristics of MAO-ANeo mice revealed an array of abnormalities partially divergent from those manifested by the MAO-A KO counterparts. In particular, we found that, whereas male MAO-ANeo animals showed deficits in social interaction akin to those manifested by their null-allele counterparts, they did not show significant enhancements in resident–intruder aggression. This finding suggests that, in line with evidence in humans (Caspi et al, 2002), reduction of MAO-A enzymatic activity in the brain may be a vulnerability factor for social deficits, but not inherently sufficient to result in aggressive and violent conduct. Notably, convergent lines of evidence have highlighted the key role of early psychosocial stress (abuse and maltreatment) in young male carriers of low-MAO-A-activity variants as an environmental vulnerability factor for aggression (Caspi et al, 2002; Kim-Cohen et al, 2006). Future investigations on MAO-ANeo mice will be critical to evaluate the impact of this important gene × environment interaction in the pathophysiology of reactive aggression and related disorders.

We found that MAO-ANeo and KO mice showed a significant reduction in total locomotor activity in a novel open field; only the hypomorphic mice, however, showed also a distinct proclivity to spend significantly less time in the central zone of the arena, which was found to be unrelated to their hypolocomotion. This phenomenon may be interpreted as an enhancement in thigmotaxis, a parameter related to the natural aversion of rodents to exposed areas, commonly used as an index of anxiety-like responsiveness (Simon et al, 1994; Treit and Fundytus, 1988). Nevertheless, this interpretation is challenged by two orders of arguments: First, the analysis of the differences in trajectories is greatly limited by the variations in total activity. Second, MAO-ANeo mice failed to show anxiety-like responses in other apparatuses specifically designed to capture approach/avoidance conflicts across protected and unprotected areas, such as the elevated plus-maze and the light–dark box.

Conversely, the increase periphery/center ratio may be reflective of perseverative traits (McGrath et al, 1999; Ralph et al, 2001; Risbrough et al, 2006); in line with this possibility, MAO-ANeo mice showed a characteristic enhancement in compulsive behaviors (such as digging, marble-burying, and water mist-induced grooming) in response to neutral environmental stimuli or mild stressors. Additionally, MAO-ANeo mice showed a significant reduction in stretch-attend postures in the elevated plus-maze, a well-characterized index of risk assessment (Rodgers et al, 1992).

The spectrum of phenotypical features observed in MAO-ANeo mice is strikingly similar to the neuropsychological and morphological correlates of low-activity allelic MAO-A variants in humans, including structural abnormalities in the orbitofrontal cortex, social deficits, maladaptive threat perception, and lack of inherent aggressiveness (at least in the absence of concurring environmental factors) (Caspi et al, 2002; Cerasa et al, 2010; Cerasa et al, 2008; Kim-Cohen et al, 2006; Lee and Ham, 2008; Meyer-Lindenberg et al, 2006; Williams et al, 2009). These cross-species analogies highlight the translational validity of MAO-ANeo mice as a dependable murine model to investigate the neurobiological bases of the subtle aberrances associated with low MAO-A activity, as well as their implication in the vulnerability to pathological aggression and related psychiatric disorders.

Notably, the constellation of abnormal emotional traits shown by MAO-ANeo mice, encompassing social deficits, perseverative responses, and deficits in environmental appraisal, is reminiscent of the manifestations featured in numerous psychiatric conditions, ranging from autism-spectrum disorders to schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder. In support of this observation, a putative role of genetic MAO-A variants as a vulnerability factor for these disorders has been advocated by several lines of evidence, often in a gender-specific manner (Camarena et al, 2001; Qiu et al, 2009; Tassone et al, 2011). In particular, low-activity MAO-A variants have been linked to higher severity of social impairments and communication deficits in autistic boys (Cohen et al, 2011), indicating the possibility that MAO-A hypomorphism, rather than affecting the incidence or prevalence of certain psychiatric disorders, may influence the severity of select manifestations, in concomitance with other interacting factors.

It is interesting to speculate that the morphological changes observed in the orbitofrontal cortex may be responsible for some of the behavioral alterations demonstrated in MAO-ANeo mice. In particular, the orbitofrontal cortex modulates behavioral responses to affective states by integrating and controlling inputs from other cortical and subcortical areas. This region has a key role in the performance of critical executive functions, such as impulse control, risk assessment, and the elaboration of decision-making strategies in response to social and environmental stimuli (Barrow and Capecchi, 1996). Accordingly, alterations of the orbitofrontal cortex have been shown to lead to ineffective allocation of adaptive strategies, and deficits in executive functions, including risk assessment, error detection, and cognitive flexibility (Alptekin et al, 2001; Graybiel and Rauch, 2000; Lacerda et al, 2003; Saxena et al, 1998; Saxena and Rauch, 2000; Zald and Kim, 1996a; Zald and Kim, 1996b). Future studies are warranted to ascertain whether the morphological alterations observed in the orbitofrontal cortex in MAO-ANeo and MAO-A KO mice are associated with deficits in executive functions in these lines.

The emotional alterations featured in MAO-ANeo mice may also be underpinned by region-specific alterations of monoaminergic neurotransmission. Specifically, we found that the increase in NE was not paralleled by a commensurate enhancement in 5-HT levels in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala. This imbalance may contribute to the unique set of behavioral abnormalities observed in MAO-ANeo mice; accordingly, previous evidence showed that anxiety-spectrum disturbances are underpinned by perturbed interactions between monoaminergic systems in the cortico-limbic circuitry (Denys et al, 2004; Fineberg et al, 2010; Kontis et al, 2008; Micallef and Blin, 2001; Szeszko et al, 2004; Szeszko et al, 1999). On the other hand, MAO-ANeo mice showed a neurochemical profile similar to MAO-A KO mice in other key regions for anxiety pathophysiology and emotional responsiveness, such as the hippocampus and midbrain. Thus, the behavioral phenotype of MAO-ANeo mice may reflect the collective effect of diverse contributions from each of these areas, resulting in a phenotype distinct from both WT and MAO-A KO mice. Finally, variations in the penetrance and expressivity of the hypomorphic mutation may depend on a set of numerous factors, including developmental and environmental components: For example, the mutated Maoa-Neo chimeric transcript (or protein), albeit non-functional, may still serve other modulatory functions by interacting with other molecular targets, and modify the phenotypical outcomes of MAO-A deficiency. Future studies, including the analyses of dynamic neurochemical parameters—such as extracellular concentrations of neurotransmitters—are warranted to delineate the region-specific roles of MAO-A, 5-HT, and NE in the regulation of behavioral responses of MAO-ANeo mice.

In conclusion, our findings highlight MAO-ANeo mice as a novel, powerful tool to investigate the involvement of MAO-A in social behavior and executive functioning, and model the subtle phenotypical abnormalities associated with low-MAO-A-activity genetic variants in humans.