Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

Functional brain development in humans

Key Points

  • The human brain continues to develop for some time after birth, providing an opportunity for experience to influence neural development. In the first few years after birth, both brain volume and cognitive function increase markedly.

  • Although most neurons are in place by birth, synaptogenesis occurs at a high rate during the first year of life, and the number of synapses peaks during this period at 150% of adult levels. Brain activity patterns change during this time and most myelination is postnatal. Such changes occur at different times for different brain areas.

  • Young infants fail in some cognitive tasks, such as reaching for an occluded object (up to 9 months) or detecting a change in an object passing behind a surface. Although other abilities seem to be adult-like at this stage, visual object processing appears to develop fully only by the second year of life. Very young infants are predisposed to look at faces, and this bias may assist in the development of social processing abilities.

  • Three theories of functional brain development are proposed. The maturational perspective states that cognitive abilities develop as the cortical areas mediating them mature. The interactive specialization approach suggests that cognitive abilities develop as the networks of cortical areas that mediate them develop appropriate interactions. The skill-learning hypothesis proposes that certain regions will be active during the development of skills in infants, but that other regions will be active once the skill has been learned (as in adult motor learning).

  • There is some evidence for each of these theories, and they are not mutually exclusive. Functional brain development in human infants depends on experience and neural activity as well as brain maturation.

Abstract

There is a continuing debate in developmental neuroscience about the importance of activity-dependent processes. The relatively delayed rate of development of the human brain, compared with that of other mammals, might make it more susceptible to the influence of postnatal experience. The human infant is well adapted to capitalize on this opportunity through primitive biases to attend to relevant stimuli in its environment. The infant's interaction with its environment helps to sculpt inter- and intraregional connections within the cortex, eventually resulting in the highly specialized adult brain.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Behavioural testing in infants.
Figure 2: Gamma-band EEG responses from infants show evidence of perceptual binding from at least 8 months.
Figure 3: Three accounts of the neural basis of an advance in behavioural abilities in infants.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Johnson, M. H. Into the minds of babes. Science 286, 247 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Finlay, B. L. & Darlington, R. B. Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science 268, 1578–1584 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Clancy, B., Darlington, R. B. & Finlay, B. L. The course of human events: predicting the timing of primate neural development. Dev. Sci. 3, 57–66 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Born, P. et al. Change of visually induced cortical activation patterns during development. Lancet 347, 543 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Yamada, H. et al. A rapid brain metabolic change in infants detected by fMRI. Neuroreport 8, 3775–3778 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Yamada, H. et al. A milestone for normal development of the infantile brain detected by functional MRI. Neurology 55, 218–223 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Eriksson, P. S. et al. Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nature Med. 4, 1313–1317 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Spreen, O., Risser, A. T. & Edgell, D. Developmental Neuropsychology (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Huttenlocher, P. R. Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia 28, 517–527 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Huttenlocher, P. R. & Dabholkar, A. S. in Development of the Prefrontal Cortex: Evolution, Neurobiology, and Behavior (eds Krasnegor, N. A., Lyon, G. R. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S.) 69–84 (Paul. H. Brookes, Baltimore, 1997).

    Google Scholar 

  11. Bourgeois, J. P. in Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (eds Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M.) 23–34 (MIT Press, Boston, 2001).

    Google Scholar 

  12. Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E. & Mazziotta, J. C. Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann. Neurol. 22, 487–497 (1987).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Huttenlocher, P. R. et al. Synaptogenesis in human visual cortex: evidence for synapse elimination during normal development. Neurosci. Lett. 33, 247–252 (1982).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Matsuzawa, J. et al. Age-related volumetric changes of brain gray and white matter in healthy infants and children. Cereb. Cortex 11, 335–342 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Paus, T. et al. Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Res. Bull. 54, 255–266 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Pfefferbaum, A. et al. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch. Neurol. 51, 874–887 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Giedd, J. N. et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neurosci. 2, 861–863 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Haith, M. M. Who put the cog in infant cognition? Is the rich interpretation too costly? Infant Behav. Dev. 21, 167–180 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Spelke, E. S. Nativism, empiricism and the origins of knowledge. Infant Behav. Dev. 21, 181–200 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Spelke, E. S. et al. Origins of knowledge. Psychol. Rev. 99, 605–632 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Munakata, Y. et al. Rethinking infant knowledge: towards an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. Pyschol. Rev. 104, 686–713 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Mareschal, D., Plunkett, K. & Harris, P. A computational and neuropsychological account of object-oriented behaviours in infancy. Dev. Sci. 2, 306–317 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Johnson, S. C. & Aslin, R. N. Perception of object unity in young infants: the roles of motion, depth and orientation. Cogn. Dev. 11, 161–180 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Slater, A. et al. The role of three-dimensional depth cues in infants' perception of partly occluded objects. Early Dev. Parenting 3, 187–191 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Xu, F. & Carey, S. Infants' metaphysics: the case of numerical identity. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 111–153 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Wilcox, T. & Baillargeon, R. Object individuation in infancy: the use of featural information in reasoning about occlusion events. Cogn. Psychol. 37, 97–155 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Leslie, A. M. et al. Indexing and the object concept: developing 'what' and 'where' systems. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 10–18 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Csibra, G. et al. Gamma oscillations and object processing in the infant brain. Science 290, 1582–1585 (2000).The first demonstration of task-related 'bursts' of neural oscillations in human infants of 8 months. These oscillatory bursts correspond to the ability of the brain to 'bind' spatially separate features into a new single object and provide a direct measure of infants' ability to process objects.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Brothers, L. & Ring, B. A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 107–118 (1992).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Baron-Cohen, S. How to build a baby that can read minds: cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. Curr. Psychol. Cogn. 13, 513–552 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  31. Duchaine, B., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. Evolutionary psychology and the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 225–230 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Johnson, M. H. et al. Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40, 1–19 (1991).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Valenza, E. et al. Face preference at birth. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 22, 892–903 (1996).Confirms previous work, but with an improved methodology, showing that newborns will preferentially orient towards simple face-like patterns.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Mondloch, C. J. et al. Face perception during early infancy. Psychol. Sci. 10, 419–422 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Morton, J. & Johnson, M. H. CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychol. Rev. 98, 164–181 (1991).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D. & Driver, J. Adult's eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychol. Sci. 9, 53–56 (1998).An important link between research on attentional cueing in young babies, and work on social cognition and joint attention. Shows that the attention of young infants can be cued to a target location by the direction of eye gaze of a realistic face.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Farroni, T. et al. Infants' use of gaze direction to cue attention: the importance of perceived motion. Vis. Cogn. 7, 705–718 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Johnson, S., Slaughter, V. & Carey, S. Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds. Dev. Sci. 1, 233–238 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Gergely, G. et al. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56, 165–193 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Meltzoff, A. N. What infant memory tells us about amnesia: long-term recall and deferred imitation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 59, 497–515 (1995).A study using the imitation method to show that before 2 years of age, children encode the behaviour of other humans in terms of the intended goals of their actions.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Csibra, G. et al. Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of 'pure reason' in infancy. Cognition 72, 237–267 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Atkinson, J. Human visual development over the first six months of life: a review and a hypothesis. Hum. Neurobiol. 3, 61–74 (1984).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Richards, J. E. Cortical indexes of saccade planning in infants. Infancy 2, 123–133 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Johnson, M. H. Cortical maturation and the development of visual attention in early infancy. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 81–95 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Hood, B. in Advances in Infancy Research (eds Rovee-Collier, C. & Lipsitt, L.) (Ablex, Norwood, New Jersey, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  46. Johnson, M. H. The inhibition of automatic saccades in early infancy. Dev. Psychobiol. 28, 163–216 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Csibra, G., Tucker, L. A. & Johnson, M. H. Differential frontal cortex activation before anticipatory and reactive saccades in infants. Infancy 2, 159–174 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Gilmore, R. O. & Johnson, M. H. Working memory in infancy: six-month-olds' performance on two versions of the oculomotor delayed response task. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 59, 397–418 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Piaget, J. The Construction of Reality in the Child (Basic Books, New York, 1954).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  50. Diamond, A. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Comparison of human infants and infant rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 74, 24–40 (1989).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Diamond, A. in The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition (eds Carey, S. & Gelman, R.) 67–110 (Lawrence Erlbaum Ass., Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1991).

    Google Scholar 

  52. Bell, M. A. & Fox, N. A. The relations between frontal brain electrical activity and cognitive development during infancy. Child Dev. 63, 1142–1163 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Diamond, A. et al. Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 62, 1–208 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Matthews, A., Ellis, A. E. & Nelson, C. A. Development of preterm and full-term infant ability on AB, recall memory, transparent barrier detour, and means-end tasks. Child Dev. 67, 2658–2676 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Johnson, M. H. Functional brain development in infants: elements of an interactive specialization framework. Child Dev. 71, 75–81 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Friston, K. J. & Price, C. J. Dynamic representations and generative models of brain function. Brain Res. Bull. 54, 275–285 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Neville, H. J., Mills, D. & Lawson, D. Fractionating language: different neural sub-systems with different sensitive periods. Cereb. Cortex 2, 244–258 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. de Haan, M., Oliver, A. & Johnson, M. H. Electrophysiological correlates of face processing by adults and 6-month-old infants. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 36 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  59. Filipek, P. A. Neuroimaging in the developmental disorders: the state of the science. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 40, 113–128 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Filipek, P. A. et al. Morphometric analysis of the brain in develomental language disorders and autism. Ann. Neurol. 32, 475 (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  61. Rumsey, J. M. & Ernst, M. Functional neuroimaging of autistic disorders. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 6, 171–179 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  62. Mills, D. L. et al. Electrophysiological studies of face processing in Williams syndrome. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 47–64 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Miller, E. K. The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1, 59–65 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Rushworth, M. F. et al. Ventral prefrontal cortex is not essential for working memory. J. Neurosci. 17, 4829–4838 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Shadmehr, R. & Holcomb, H. Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science 277, 821–824 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Johnson, M. H. et al. Visual attention in infants with perinatal brain damage: evidence of the importance of left anterior lesions. Dev. Sci. 1, 53–58 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Craft, S. & Schatz, J. The effects of bifrontal stroke during childhood on visual attention: evidence from children with sickle cell anemia. Dev. Neuropsychol. 10, 285–297 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Gauthier, I. et al. Activation of the middle fusiform 'face area' increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nature Neurosci. 2, 568–573 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. Gauthier, I. & Nelson, C. A. The development of face expertise. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 219–224 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  70. Rossion, B. et al. The N170 occipito-temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: an electrophysiological account of face-specific processes in the human brain. Neuroreport 11, 69–74 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Maurer, D. et al. Rapid improvement in the acuity of infants after visual input. Science 286, 108–110 (1999).A study with patients visually deprived over the first months or years of life owing to cataracts. The improvements in acuity following corrective surgery were surprisingly rapid, although some degree of deficit remained even after years of restored vision.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  72. Le Grand, R. et al. Neuroperception: early visual experience and face processing. Nature 410, 890 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Karmiloff-Smith, A. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 389–398 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Paterson, S. J. et al. Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science 286, 2355–2358 (1999).A study that investigated whether the profiles of cognitive abilities and disabilities observed in adults with developmental disorders are also observed during infancy. The results show that profiles of cognitive disability might change during development.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  75. Kellman, P. and Spelke, E. S. Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 483–524 (1983).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S. & Wasserman, S. Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition 20, 191–208 (1985).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  77. Sakai, K. et al. Transition of brain activation from frontal to parietal areas in visuo-motor sequence learning. J. Neurosci. 18, 1827–1840 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank D. Maurer, A. Karmiloff-Smith and D. Mareschal for comments on the manuscript. The UK Medical Research Council supports the author's research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Supplementary information

Related links

Related links

MIT ENCYCLOPEDIA OF COGNITIVE SCIENCES

Positron emission tomography

Magnetic resonance imaging

Autism

Perceptual development

Infant cognition

Neural development

FURTHER INFORMATION

Johnson's research centre

Glossary

HABITUATION

A decrease in the behavioural response to a repeated, benign stimulus.

SACCADE

A rapid eye movement that brings the point of maximal visual acuity — the fovea — to the image of interest.

BALINT'S SYNDROME

A neurological disorder caused by bilateral damage to the parieto-occipital region of the brain, characterized by disorders of spatial perception.

PHENYLKETONURIA

An inherited inability to metabolize phenylalanine which can result in brain and nerve damage leading to mental retardation.

WILLIAMS' SYNDROME

A rare congenital disorder: symptoms include facial abnormalities and deficits in some cognitive skills.

GREEBLES

A category of computer-generated novel objects, originally designed as a control set for faces. Like faces, Greebles are all similar because they have the same number of parts in the same configuration.

NEAR INFRARED SPECTROSCOPY

A form of optical (light) imaging that entails placing sources and detectors on the head, and measuring the scatter or bending of light as it passes through the skull and brain.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Johnson, M. Functional brain development in humans. Nat Rev Neurosci 2, 475–483 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1038/35081509

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/35081509

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing